Current Biology, Vol. 13, 522–525, March 18, 2003, 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. DOI 10.1016/S 09 60 - 98 22 ( 03 )0 0 16 5- 9

An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action J.M. Kilner,1,2 Y. Paulignan,1 and S.J. Blakemore3 Institut des Sciences Cognitives 67 Boulevard Pinel Bron 69675 France 2 Wellcome Department of Imaging Science 12 Queen Square London WC1N 3BG United Kingdom 3 Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience 17 Queen Square London WC1N 3AR United Kingdom

1

Summary It has been proposed that actions are intrinsically linked to perception and that imagining, observing, preparing, or in any way representing an action excites the motor programs used to execute that same action [1–3]. There is neurophysiological evidence that certain brain regions involved in executing actions are activated by the mere observation of action (the socalled “mirror system;” [4, 5]). However, it is unknown whether this mirror system causes interference between observed and simultaneously executed movements. In this study we test the hypothesis that, because of the overlap between action observation and execution, observed actions should interfere with incongruous executed actions. Subjects made arm movements while observing either a robot or another human making the same or qualitatively different arm movements. Variance in the executed movement was measured as an index of interference to the movement. The results demonstrate that observing another human making incongruent movements has a significant interference effect on executed movements. However, we found no evidence that this interference effect occurred when subjects observed a robotic arm making incongruent movements. These results suggest that the simultaneous activation of the overlapping neural networks that process movement observation and execution infers a measurable cost to motor control. Results and Discussion The notion that actions are intrinsically linked to perception was proposed by William James, who claimed that “every mental representation of a movement awakens to some degree the actual movement which is its object” [1]. The implication is that observing, imagining, or in any way representing an action excites the motor programs used to execute that same action [2, 3]. Interest in this idea has grown recently, in part due to the neurophysiological discovery of “mirror” neurons in the monkey venCorrespondence: [email protected]

tral premotor cortex. These neurons discharge both when the monkey performs specific hand movements and when it observes another monkey or human performing the same movements [4, 5]. There is also evidence that in humans several brain regions are activated both during action generation and during observation of others’ actions [6–9]. Observing a movement has measurable consequences on the peripheral motor system. During action observation there is a significant increase in the motor-evoked potentials from the hand muscles that a subject would use in making such a movement [10], and this effect is temporally linked to the observed movement [11]. In addition, reaction times to initiate a finger movement or to grasp an object are significantly slowed down after the visual presentation of a different finger movement [12] or of a hand executing a different grasp [13]. This suggests that, during observation of action, the specific neural networks subserving that particular movement are already tuned for action [1–3]. If the motor system is geared up to execute observed movements, this might result in interference when the observed movement is qualitatively different from the simultaneously executed movement. To investigate this hypothesis, an experiment was performed in which eight right-handed, naive volunteers made sinusoidal movements with their right arm while observing arm movements that were either congruent or incongruent with their own movements. In each condition, the subject, standing, was instructed to make natural sinusoidal movements of the right arm from the shoulder; these movements were made either vertically or horizontally at a rate of 0.5 Hz (see Figure 1). Subjects practiced the movement until they were proficient at producing the desired whole arm movements. While making these arm movements, the subject observed movements made by another effector, either a human or a robot, situated 2 m away from the subject. The observed movements were either horizontal or vertical, and therefore either congruent or incongruent with the subject’s own arm movements. In addition, there were two baseline conditions in which subjects moved their arm either horizontally or vertically without watching anything. An Optotrak 3020 recording system was used for recording the data (see Figure 2), and variance in the movement was used as a measure of interference to the movement. The results of a repeated-measures 2 ⫻ 2 ⫻ 2 ANOVA revealed significant main effects of movement direction (df ⫽ 1,7; F ⫽ 17.408; P ⬍ 0.005), movement congruency (df ⫽ 1,7; F ⫽ 7.037; P ⬍ 0.05), and observed effector (df ⫽ 1,7; F ⫽ 52.041; P ⬍ 0.0005). However, only the interaction between observed effector and movement congruency was significant (df ⫽ 1,7; F ⫽ 12.335; P ⬍ 0.01). None of the interactions involving movement direction was significant (direction ⫻ congruency: df ⫽ 1,7; F ⫽ 5.149; P ⬎ 0.05; direction ⫻ effector: df ⫽ 1,7; F ⫽ 2.289; P ⬎ 0.05; direction ⫻ congruency ⫻ effector: df ⫽ 1,7; F ⫽ 3.989; P ⬎ 0.05). In other words, the variance in horizontal movements was significantly different from the variance in vertical movements, and

Brief Communication 523

Figure 1. Experimental Design To investigate the hypothesis that interference should occur when an observed movement is qualitatively different from a simultaneously executed movement, we performed an experiment in which eight healthy, righthanded, naive volunteers (four females; age range 23–32 years) made sinusoidal movements with their right arm at the same time as observing movements that were either congruent or incongruent with their own movements. For testing the hypothesis that interference effects are not simply a result of increased attentional demands or increased task complexity and that they are specific to observing biological incongruent movements, the observed movements were made either by another human or by a robotic arm. There were ten conditions, eight of which formed a factorial design in which the factors were (1) executed movement direction, (2) congruency between observed and executed movement, and (3) observed effector (robot or human). In each condition, the subject (S) was instructed to make sinusoidal movements of the right arm from the shoulder; these movements were either vertical or horizontal at a rate of 0.5 Hz. While making these arm movements, the subject observed movements made by another effector situated facing the subject; this was either the right arm of the experimenter (E) or a robotic arm (R; RT100, OxIM Ltd, Oxon, UK), and the movements were either congruent or incongruent with the executed movements. In addition, there were two baseline conditions in which subjects moved their arm either horizontally or vertically without watching anything. Within each trial subjects made ten sinusoidal arm movements. Each subject performed two trials of each condition, in a random order. The subject was instructed to watch the index finger if they were observing the human experimenter or the tip of the robotic hand if they were observing the robot. The subject was asked to make arm movements in time with those of the effector. No other instruction was given. When the human experimenter made the observed movements, he was blindfolded. An Optotrak 3020 (Northern Digital, Waterloo, Ontario) recording system recorded the movements from five IRED (Infra Red emitting diodes) markers. Three markers defined the XY plane, which was parallel to the floor such that the X dimension was in the direction of the subject’s horizontal movements. Thus, the Z dimension was in the direction of the subject’s vertical movements. One marker was attached to the subject’s right index finger. One marker was attached either to the end of the robot arm or to the experimenter’s right index finger, depending on the condition. The IREDs were sampled at a frequency of 250 Hz, and these data were used in the subsequent analysis. Velocity profiles of the robot movement and observed human movement were qualitatively different; it was flat for the majority of the movement for the robot and curved for the duration of the observed human movements. This was reflected in the mean peak velocities, which were 0.40 ms⫺1 for the robot’s movements and 1.27 ms⫺1 for the observed human movements. The average velocities over the entire movements were 0.21 ms⫺1 for the robot’s movements and 0.69 ms⫺1 for the observed human movements.

this difference was independent of both the kind of movements (congruent or incongruent) being observed and the effector (human or robot) of the observed movement. In general, horizontal movements were associated with more variance than vertical movements. This simply reflects the fact that subjects naturally made more arching movements in the XZ plane during horizontal movements than during vertical movements, as can be seen in Figure 2. The significant interaction between the observed effector and movement congruency was due to more interference in subjects’ arm movements when they watched human arm movements that were incongruent with their own movements than in any other condition (Figure 3). Executed movements in all the conditions in which subjects observed movements were compared with the baseline condition, in which subjects executed the same movements without observing any movements. The only condition that differed significantly from the baseline movement condition was the condition in which subjects watched the experimenter making incongruent arm movements (t ⫽ 6.815; P ⬍ 0.0005). There was no significant difference between subjects’ movement in the

baseline condition and their movement when they watched either the robot or the experimenter making congruent movements (t ⫽ ⫺0.72; P ⬎ 0.05 and t ⫽ 0.861; P ⬎ 0.05, respectively) or when they watched a robot making incongruent movements (t ⫽ 0.2; P ⬎ 0.05). Only the observation of another human making incongruent arm movements significantly interfered with the execution of arm movements. No significant facilitation effect on the executed movement was observed in any of the conditions. This lack of a significant facilitation effect, which may have been predicted in the congruent conditions, might have been due to the measure we used, which was designed specifically to test our a priori hypothesis that there would be interference when subjects observed incongruent human movements. The present results demonstrate that observing the movements of another human has a measurable interference effect on simultaneously executed actions. The finding that observing a robot making incongruent movements had no significant effect on executed movement demonstrates that the interference effect is not simply due to increased attentional demands or task complexity. Moreover, these results suggest that there

Current Biology 524

Figure 3. Interference Effect of Observing Biological Movement on Executed Movements Figure 2. Single Subject Movements in Each Condition (A–D) Individual movements made in the XZ plane by a single subject during four of the conditions. Data were segmented offline into movements from right to left and left to right for horizontal conditions and from up to down and down to up for vertical movements. Therefore, there was a maximum of 40 segmented movements per condition per subject. For clarity, all of the movements have been normalized so that their mean in the X and Z axes is equal to zero. The scale of all four plots is illustrated in (D). The plots show horizontal and vertical movements that the subject made while observing the robot making congruent movements (A), while observing the robot making incongruent movements (B), while observing the experimenter making congruent movements (C), and while observing the experimenter making incongruent movements (D). Data were lowpass filtered at 10 Hz with a second-order Butterworth filter.

is a distinction between observing human and robotic movements in terms of this interference effect, supporting the proposal that the brain processes biological and nonbiological movements differently. There is a large body of evidence showing that distinct neural processing systems exist for these two types of movement. In particular, the superior temporal sulcus (STS) has been shown to respond selectively to biological motion, in monkeys [14] and in humans [15–18]. In the current study, an interference effect was found for human (biological) incongruent movements only. There are many aspects of human movement that could cause interference in the incongruent condition, including the “biological” velocity profile of the movement, the bodily posture, or the presence of bodily, head, or facial features of the human. Which aspect of the human movement is the trigger for the interference, and which is absent in robotic movements, is unknown and requires further experimentation. Although the STS is activated by the observation of biological movements, it is not activated by the execution of action. Therefore, it is unlikely that the interfer-

For each segmented movement the variance in the movement orthogonal to the dominant dimension of movement and in the dominant dimension of the incongruent movement was calculated. Thus, if the subject made a movement from left to right, the X dimension was the dominant-movement dimension, and the variance was calculated for the movements in the Z dimension, and visa versa. The mean of the movement variances was calculated across all trials for each condition. The effect of executed-movement direction (horizontal or vertical), observed effector (robot or experimenter), and congruency (horizontal or vertical observed movements) on these variance means in each condition was analyzed with a repeated measures 2 ⫻ 2 ⫻ 2 ANOVA. Paired t tests were used for comparing each observation condition in which the subject made horizontal movements with the baseline condition, in which the subject made horizontal movements without observing anything. The same analysis was performed on the vertical movements. A Bonferroni correction was used to correct for multiple comparisons. Mean variances in executed movement for each condition averaged over direction of movement and then across subjects. Mean variances (and standard error bars) are shown for five conditions: observing the robot making congruent movements (robot congruent); observing the experimenter making congruent movements (human congruent); observing the robot making incongruent movements (robot incongruent); observing the experimenter making incongruent movements (human incongruent); and the baseline condition (no observation). The only condition that differed significantly from the baseline movement condition was the condition in which subjects watched the experimenter making incongruent arm movements (t ⫽ 6.815; P ⬍ 0.0005). The other three comparisons were not significantly different (P ⬎ 0.1).

ence effect of observing biological motion on action found in the present study occurs at the level of the STS. A number of other brain regions, including premotor and parietal cortex, are activated both by the observation and the execution of action in humans [6–9]. Action observation activates human premotor and parietal cortex in a somatotopic manner; watching mouth, hand, and foot movements activates the same functionally specific regions of premotor cortex as making those respective movements [19]. Neurons in the premotor

Brief Communication 525

cortex of monkeys discharge both when the monkey performs specific hand movements and when it observes another monkey or human performing the same movements [4, 5]. These mirror properties are also displayed by a significant number of neurons in the inferior parietal lobe [20, 21]. If the effect observed in the current experiment is due to interference within a common neural network that encodes both observed and executed movements, then it seems likely that this could occur within the premotor cortex and/or the parietal cortex. Indeed, there is evidence that mirror neurons in the premotor cortex distinguish between hand actions and actions made by a tool; they respond only to a hand movement and not to the same movement performed made with a pair of pliers [20]. This distinction between the observation of biological and mechanical movement in terms of the mirror responses in premotor cortex might account for the current finding that the interference effect was specific to the observation of incongruent human movements. It has been proposed that the mirror system might have evolved to facilitate communication, empathy, and the understanding of other people’s mental states [22]. Simulating other people’s actions would trigger an action representation from which the underlying goals and intentions could be inferred on the basis of what our own goals and intentions would be for the same action [23]. The mirror system is a possible neural mechanism for simulation of other people’s actions [22]. The current results suggest that this mirror system may have evolved at a small but significant cost to motor control. The results provoke many questions. What is the crucial factor in the observed movement that causes the interference? Is it the goal of the observed movement, or more basic movement properties, that interfere with executed movement? What is special about observed biological movement? Is it the specific kinematics of the movement or the representation of a human? Further experiments are being designed to investigate these questions. Acknowledgments This research was supported by the Wellcome Trust (United Kingdom) and the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (France). S.J.B. and J.M.K. are supported by Wellcome Trust International Fellowships, and Y.P. is supported by the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (France). Received: December 13, 2002 Revised: January 23, 2003 Accepted: January 23, 2003 Published: March 18, 2003 References 1. James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology. (New York, NY: Holt). 2. Jeannerod, M. (1994). The Representing Brain—neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behav. Brain Sci. 17, 187–202. 3. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 129–154. 4. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 131–141.

5. Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119, 593–609. 6. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E., Perani, D., and Fazio, F. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution. Exp. Brain Res. 111, 246–252. 7. Decety, J., Gre`zes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Grassi, F., and Fazio, F. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject’s strategy. Brain 120, 1763–1777. 8. Hari, R., Fross, N., Avikainen, E., Kirveskari, E., Salenius, S., and Rizzolatti, G. (1998). Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95, 15061–15065. 9. Grafton, S.T., Arbib, M.A., Fadiga, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography. 2. Observation compared with imagination. Exp. Brain Res. 112, 103–111. 10. Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., and Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 2608–2611. 11. Gangitano, M., Mottaghy, F.M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation. Neuroreport 12, 1489–1492. 12. Brass M, Bekkering H, Prinz W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychol (Amst) 106, 3–22. 13. Craighero, L., Bello, A., Fadiga, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hand action preparation influences the responses to hand pictures. Neuropsychologia 40, 492–502. 14. Oram, M.W., and Perrett, D.I. (1994). Responses of anterior superior temporal polysensory (STPa) neurons to biological motion stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 99–116. 15. Frith, C.D., and Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds — a biological basis. Science 286, 1692–1695. 16. Allison, T., Puce, A., and McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 267–278. 17. Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., and Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 711–720. 18. Gre`zes, J., Fonlupt, P., Bertenthal, B., Delon-Martin, C., Segebarth, C., and Decety, J. (2001). Does perception of biological motion rely on specific brain regions? Neuroimage 13, 775–785. 19. Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G.R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R.J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., and Freund, H.J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 400–404. 20. Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 661–670. 21. Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action. In Attention & Performance XIX, W. Prinz and B. Hommel, eds. (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), pp. 334–355. 22. Gallese, V., and Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 493–501. 23. Goldman, A.I. (1992). In defense of Simulation Theory. Mind and Language 7, 104–119.

An Interference Effect of Observed Biological ... - Semantic Scholar

Current Biology, Vol. ... the motor programs used to execute that same action .... robot's movements and 1.27 ms 1 for the observed human movements.

143KB Sizes 14 Downloads 330 Views

Recommend Documents

An Interference Effect of Observed Biological ... - Semantic Scholar
culated for the movements in the Z dimension, and visa versa. The tion per subject. .... responses in premotor cortex might account for the. 11. Gangitano, M.

The Kuleshov Effect - Semantic Scholar
Statistical parametric maps (from right hemisphere to left hemisphere) and parameter estimate plots illustrating the main effect of faces presented in the (c) negative-neutral contexts and (d) positive-neutral contexts. Faces presented in both negati

An Experimental Study on the Capture Effect in ... - Semantic Scholar
A recent measurement work on the cap- ture effect in 802.11 networks [12] argues that the stronger frame can be successfully decoded only in two cases: (1) The.

Verb Production and the Semantic Interference Effect 1 ...
interference paradigm can be a useful tool to constrain claims about ..... the picture word interference paradigm as a tool to further study the ..... LISTENING.

Verb Production and the Semantic Interference Effect 1 ...
[email protected]. In three ... (i)20()J Institute for Cognitive Science, Seoul National University. ... (1990) used the time-course of the semantic interference ..... the data reveals that the semantic interference effect is only present when.

Structural and biological characterization of three ... - Semantic Scholar
Nov 11, 2004 - European hornet, Vespa crabro, and its analogs. J. Peptide Res. 50, 88–93. Manning, D.R., Gilman, A.G., 1983. The regulatory components of adenylate cyclase and transducin. A family of structurally homologous guanine nucleotide-bindi

The effect of disfluency on mind wandering during ... - Semantic Scholar
United States b University of Notre Dame, Department of Computer Science .... findings and those for tasks like list learning suggests that disfluency only affects surface level learning (e.g. ... The experiment utilized a between-subjects design.

Limited effect of fly-wheel and spinal mobilization ... - Semantic Scholar
Jun 11, 2011 - none of the countermeasures are, of course, ''perfect'' and it is appropriate ...... as subjects maintain spinal posture and their motor skill in the exercise ... the maintenance of the musculature during bed-rest, there is a relativel

Effect of Global Context on Homophone Ambiguity ... - Semantic Scholar
provide converging evidence for this interpretation. Eye-tracking data analysis. .... to the evolution of perceptual and cognitive systems. Cognitive Science: A ...

Effect of vitamin AD3E utilization at the pregnancy ... - Semantic Scholar
system such as vitamin A,E and D (Priyantha et al .,. 2012). Some studies refer to that the deficiency in the .... AD3E in G2 and low level of iron content in feedstuff of G1These results agree with results of Hafez ... 1- At the last of 3 months pre

Avoiding Interference: How People Use Spatial ... - Semantic Scholar
profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. .... One disadvantage is the need and cost of PDAs. Another is that .... custom single display groupware application, created atop the. SDGToolki

Cross-layer Interference-aware Routing for ... - Semantic Scholar
Jul 6, 2006 - We introduce the concept of set-based routing in an attempt to in- corporate ... However, [3] assumes no interference between links in the same.

Avoiding Interference: How People Use Spatial ... - Semantic Scholar
1Department of Computer Science, 2Department of Psychology. University of Calgary. Calgary, Alberta, Canada, T2N 1N4. +1 403 220 6087. @cpsc.ucalgary.ca, [email protected]. ABSTRACT. Single Display Groupware (SDG) lets multiple co-located people, each

Pulmonary Rehabilitation: Summary of an ... - Semantic Scholar
Documenting the scientific evidence underlying clinical practice has been important ... standard of care for the management of patients with chronic obstructive ...

The Effect of Unemployment Benefits and ... - Semantic Scholar
key policy parameters: the effect of UI on reservation wages and the effect of nonemployment durations on wage offers. ... we call the causal effect of unemployment duration on wages – is important for understanding the costs and ...... of UI exten

Kondo Effect in the Presence of Itinerant-Electron ... - Semantic Scholar
Dec 10, 2003 - system's ground state can be tuned to have a fully com- pensated local spin, in which case the QD conductance is found to be the same for each spin channel, G" И G#. The model.—For ferromagnetic leads, electron- electron interaction

Effect of Melatonin on Sleep, Behavior, and ... - Semantic Scholar
Article Plus (online only) materials for this article appear on the Journal's Web ..... c Mann-Whitney U test. VAN DER HEIJDEN ET ..... Neuropsychol Dev Cogn C.

Effect of substrate modes on thermal transport in ... - Semantic Scholar
Aug 12, 2011 - thus creates an average net thermal current from the center to the edge. ... steady-state heat current and that the time-averaged ε of the middle atoms is ... 2(a). This profile is nonlinear, with temperature “slips” near the ther

The effect of disfluency on mind wandering during ... - Semantic Scholar
United States b University of Notre Dame, Department of Computer Science .... findings and those for tasks like list learning suggests that disfluency only affects surface level learning (e.g. ... The experiment utilized a between-subjects design.

Limited effect of fly-wheel and spinal mobilization ... - Semantic Scholar
Jun 11, 2011 - a Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Center for Muscle and Bone Research, Hindenburgdamm .... experiments/bed_Rest_2001.html). ..... between groups (pr0.017, otherwise pZ0.048; see text and Table 3 for further details).

The Effect of Unemployment Benefits and ... - Semantic Scholar
in the part of the wage offer distribution relevant for workers' employment decisions. If the condition on reemployment wages is satisfied in the data, the only effect of nonem- ployment durations on wages must arise from a change in the wage-offer d

Field-Effect Tunneling Transistor Based on Vertical ... - Semantic Scholar
Feb 2, 2012 - Page 1. www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1218461/DC1. Supporting Online Material for. Field-Effect Tunneling Transistor ...

The Mid-Domain Effect and Species Richness ... - Semantic Scholar
abstract: If species' ranges are randomly shuffled within a bounded geographical domain free of environmental gradients, ranges overlap increasingly toward ...