Cogn Process (2015) 16 (Suppl 1):S197–S201 DOI 10.1007/s10339-015-0699-4

SHORT REPORT

Automatic imitation of the arm kinematic profile in interacting partners Alessandro D’Ausilio1 • Leonardo Badino1 • Pietro Cipresso2 • Alice Chirico3 Elisabetta Ferrari1 • Giuseppe Riva2,3 • Andrea Gaggioli2,3



Published online: 31 July 2015 Ó Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015

Abstract Cognitive neuroscience, traditionally focused on individual brains, is just beginning to investigate social cognition through realistic interpersonal interaction. However, quantitative investigation of the dynamical sensorimotor communication among interacting individuals in goal-directed ecological tasks is particularly challenging. Here, we recorded upper-body motion capture of 23 dyads, alternating their leader/follower role, in a tower-building task. Either a strategy of joining efforts or a strategy of independent action could in principle be used. We found that arm reach velocity profiles of participants tended to converge across trials. Automatic imitation of low-level motor control parameters demonstrates that the task is achieved through continuous action coordination as opposed to independent action planning. Moreover, the leader produced more consistent and predictable velocity profiles, suggesting an implicit strategy of signaling to the follower. This study serves as a validation of our joint goaldirected non-verbal task for future applications. In fact, the quantification of human-to-human continuous sensorimotor interaction, in a way that can be predicted and controlled, is probably one of the greatest challenges for the future of human–robot interaction.

& Alessandro D’Ausilio [email protected] 1

RBCS – Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences Department, Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Via Morego, 30, 16163 Genoa, Italy

2

Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Verbania, Italy

3

Psychology Department, Catholic University of Milan, Milan, Italy

Keywords Social neuroscience  Joint action  Automatic imitation  Kinematic analysis  Velocity profiles  Towerbuilding task

Introduction Group coordination in ecological tasks is difficult to measure. However, we do that instinctively by sending and receiving socially relevant messages in all our interactions (i.e., hand gestures, facial expressions, and speech). Among all possible communication channels, complex coordinated behavior can arise without the need for verbal communication to happen (Sebanz et al. 2006; Ne´da et al. 2000; Riley et al. 2011). Although non-verbal communication is clearly an important part of social competence, recent research has mainly focused on minimal interaction to maintain experimental control (D’Ausilio et al. 2015). However, in many everyday social interactions, behavior is continuous, temporally overlapping, and based on wholebody motion (Tognoli et al. 2007). Therefore, group-level behavioral dynamics should be studied as complex, interactive systems in which information transfer is continuous rather than discrete (D’Ausilio et al. 2012; Glowinski et al. 2013; Badino et al. 2014). In this framework, group-level coordination is grounded upon our tuning to the detection of human biological motion primitives (Johansson 1973; Casile et al. 2010), which in turn drives the implicit motor contagion observed between interacting partners. The phenomenon of motor contagion (or automatic imitation) is the tendency to involuntarily reproduce specific features of observed actions. For instance, participants’ movements are automatically contaminated by the velocity profile of a moving dot (Bisio et al. 2010). Such automatic motor contagion is

123

S198

reduced when the interacting partner violates the biological laws of motion (Bisio et al. 2014). These findings suggest that a low-level sensory-motor matching mechanism forms the basis of higher levels of social interaction by facilitating group behavioral entrainment (Dumas et al. 2014). Here, we exploit the concept of automatic imitation of kinematic profiles, in couples of participants playing a joint game. The game consisted in a tower-building task with wooden blocks (tower-building task). The goal of the game was to jointly balance the trade-off between speed and accuracy. Participants were alternating their role of either leader or follower, across trials (see ‘‘Methods’’ for details). We recorded upper-body motion capture with a Vicon system and tested whether automatic imitation of arm velocity profiles was present in each dyad. In principle, the task can be accomplished at different degrees of coordination among participants. The strongest coordination requires the negotiation of motor parameters and thus continuous information transfer. Otherwise, minimal coordination requires only the negotiation of action timing and thus reduced and discrete information transfer. The former end of this continuum would be evidenced by the mutual contagion of low-level motor control parameters. The latter instead would be evident if lags between individual actions are reduced in the absence of motor imitation. Moreover, if automatic kinematic imitation is present, the leader and the follower should differ in this regard. In fact, we predict that the leader should evidence greater kinematic predictability to help the follower build more reliable models of his/her behavior.

Methods Subjects The study involved 46 participants (23 males and 23 females) who were recruited among the Italian Institute of Technology staff (mean age 29.26, SD = 2.92). Procedures were approved by the local ethics committee (ASL-3, Genova), and participants did not receive any compensation. Materials We used three sheets of papers on which were drawn squares (Fig. 1a). Twelve colored cubes (four red, four yellow, and four green) were used to build the tower, and each performance was videotaped. Kinematic recordings Participant movements were measured via a motion-capture system (Vicon system) with nine near-infrared

123

Cogn Process (2015) 16 (Suppl 1):S197–S201

cameras with acquisition frequency set at 100 Hz. Reflective markers were placed on both shoulders and the dominant arm elbow and wrist, and head (four markers for the first subject and three for the second member of the couple; see Fig. 1c). Procedure The experiment consisted of two phases and lasted about 20 min. In the first phase (i.e., single trial), each member of a couple performed the task alone, to familiarize with it. In the second phase (i.e., couple trials), each couple performed the task together 10 times (see Fig. 1b). In the single trial, each member of a couple had to build a tower using all the 12 cubes. They were asked to build the tower as quickly as possible, by using just one hand, one cube at a time. In the couple trials, each couple had to build a tower, as quickly as possible, using all the 12 cubes (six each participant; see Fig. 1a). The only constraint concerned the specific turn-taking sequence. The leader had to place the first cube and choose the color of the cubes to place. The follower had to place a cube of the same color chosen by the leader. In each trial, the leader was switched, and thus, each participant acted as a leader for five trials and as a follower for the other five trials. Analyses The trial performance of a dyad was measured as the average normalized interval between the instant in which the leader placed a cube on the top of the tower and the instant in which the follower placed the next cube on top of the tower (henceforth turn-taking time). The turn-taking time was computed by observing the peaks of the Euclidean distance between the wrist position at each time step and the wrist position at time 0 (see Fig. 1d). The turntaking time was normalized to take into account the increasing distance between the cubes’ original position and the top of the tower. The automatic motor imitation was defined as the maximum cross-correlation value between the velocity profiles over time of the dominant wrist of the two participants. The minimum and maximum lag used to compute cross-correlation was set as the minimum and maximum (not-normalized) turn-taking time, respectively. Crosscorrelation was only computed on trials where participants successfully completed the task. To test the statistical significance of the measured maximum cross-correlation, we ran a bootstrap-based significance test (Efron and Tibshirani 1993). Each bootstrap replication consisted of the same number of time series pairs as the original experiment. To generate a bootstrap (normal) distribution of the cross-correlation (for each

Cogn Process (2015) 16 (Suppl 1):S197–S201

A

S199

B

C Head markers Wrist Euclidean distance from starting point Vs. Time

D Elbow markers

Wrist markers

Distance (mm)

Shoulder markers

600 500 400

Leader Follower

300 200 100

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

Time (sec)

Fig. 1 a Top view of the setup; b a dyad executing the task; c Vicon point-light display of a dyad executing the task; d Euclidean distance between the instantaneous wrist position and the starting point wrist position of a dyad executing the task (color figure online)

trial), we created 10,000 bootstrap replications with replacement, where velocity profile pairs were sampled by coupling the time series of two randomly selected subjects belonging to two different dyads but building the tower at the same trial number. The ‘‘same trial number’’ constraint allows a stricter significance test that takes into account the increasing predictability of the velocity time series over trials which, in turn, can increase cross-correlation values (see ‘‘Discussion’’). Since cross-correlation computation requires same-length time series, we ‘‘stretched’’ the shortest time series to equal the longest time series by applying resampling. Resampling is a global, uniform scaling technique as opposed to non-uniform techniques such as dynamic time warping which apply local transformations to the time series to adjust local mismatches between two time series and thus can heavily affect crosscorrelation computation. At each trial number, we computed the average bootstrap maximum cross-correlation value and its bootstrap-t confidence interval. The bootstrapt confidence interval was defined by setting a p value (p = 0.005 after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons). An additional analysis was carried out to assess whether the leader exhibits more predictable movements (in terms of velocity profile) than the follower. The predictability of

the leader (and the follower), given the history of her velocity profile within a trial, was defined as the maximum autocorrelation value computed in the minimum and maximum interval times that took the leader (the follower) to place two cubes on top of the tower.

Results The actual measured maximum cross-correlation is significant in most trials (except trial 1 and 4) according to the bootstrap-based significance test (p = 0.0053, p = 0.0001, p = 0.0001, p = 0.0072, p = 0.0001, p = 0.0020, p = 0.0001, p = 0.0002, p = 0.0033, p = 0.0006, where p was determined by the position in the vector of bootstrap maximum cross-correlations of the closest value to the actual measured maximum cross-correlation). The leader mean maximum autocorrelation (mean = 0.6732, SE = 0.0058) is significantly higher than the follower mean maximum autocorrelation (mean = 0.6582, SE = 0.0056) according to a two-tailed t test (t[410] = 1.9727, p = 0.0492). Similarly, the overall mean maximum cross-correlation (mean = 0.6772, SE = 0.0044) is significantly higher than the follower mean maximum autocorrelation (t[406] = 2.7757, p = 0.0058). There is no

123

S200

Cogn Process (2015) 16 (Suppl 1):S197–S201

significant difference between the overall mean maximum cross-correlation and the leader mean maximum autocorrelation (t[406] = 0.5749, p = 0.5657).

Discussion As expected, participants improve their performance across trials. This is shown by the reduction in turn-taking time that reaches a plateau around the seventh trial (Fig. 2a). Concurrently, the increase in kinematic motor imitation (Fig. 2b) means that participants were showing contagion of low-level motor parameters. At the same time, kinematic imitation lags were also reduced (Fig. 2c), suggesting a process of continuous optimization of their imitation strategies, throughout the task. Interestingly, the leader was also generating velocity profiles that were more predictable than that of the follower (Fig. 2d). High kinematic predictability helps the follower build more efficient models of leader’s behavior. The leader may have indeed implicitly put into place a strategy to optimize coordination, increasing behavioral consistency,

C

Turn-taking Time 2.4

0.8

2.2

0.75

2

XC lag (sec)

Normalized Turn-taking time (sec)

A

and ultimately kinematic readability and predictability. This in turn may explain the better task performance. This phenomenon is often referred to as signaling (Pezzulo and Dindo 2011; Pezzulo et al. 2013). In fact, when participants have access to different amounts of information, joint action is asymmetric (Schmidt et al. 2011). To cope with such unbalance, the leader is supposed to either make his action more predictable (Vesper et al. 2011) or perform her actions in a way that provides relevant task information to the partner (Sacheli et al. 2013). The tower-building task we present here has, however, some critical features that set it apart from previous similar studies (i.e., Vesper and Richardson 2014). Subjects did not have to imitate each other nor they had to synchronize the timing of their pick-move-place action. Target position (the tower) was always the same, and thus, there is no added value in signaling its location through the modulation of kinematic parameters. The only motoric constraint was to optimize the turn taking. Therefore, an optimal coordination strategy might have been to simply increase peak velocity of their ballistic movements—not motor imitation.

1.8 1.6 1.4 1.2

Cross-Correlation Best Lag

0.7 0.65 0.6 0.55 0.5 0.45

1 0.8

0.4

0.6

0.35 1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

1

10

2

3

4

5

trial

7

8

9

10

trial

D

Velocity Profile Cross-Correlation 0.72

0.72

0.7

0.7

0.68 0.66 0.64 0.62 0.6

Bootstrap

0.58

AC and XC values

Largest XC value

B

6

Cross-Correlation and Auto-Correlation p = 0.0058

p = 0.0492

0.68 0.66 0.64 0.62 0.6 0.58

0.56

0.56 1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

trial Fig. 2 a Normalized turn-taking time across trials (bars represent standard error of the mean); b maximum cross-correlation value of velocity profiles across trials. In black, real computed values (bars represent the standard error). In gray, the bootstrap results (bars are the confidence intervals); c best lag for the maximum cross-

123

XC

AC Leader

AC Follower

correlation value (bars represent the standard error); d comparison between the average across trials of the cross-correlation between participants and autocorrelation of the leader and follower (bars represent the standard error)

Cogn Process (2015) 16 (Suppl 1):S197–S201

The only information that the leader might have wanted to signal was the next cube to place (or its color). In our first iteration of the tower-building task, the relative position of colored cubes was kept constant and a screen prevented from seeing object grasping (Fig. 1b). Nevertheless, arm trajectories could still be modulated to offer some hints about the location of the next cube. However, it is important to note that by focusing on velocity profiles, instead of position data, our analyses are independent from this kind of signaling strategies. Importantly, we show that as a byproduct of action coordination, participants exhibit a motor contagion that does not seem to be directly functional to task optimization. More interestingly, the fact that the leader generates velocity profiles with higher predictability suggests that motor contagion is not merely a side effect but may offer a significant coordination advantage. The present work is a first step toward the standardization of the tower-building task and the definition of a set of features and analyses to extract sensorimotor communication. However, there are serious technical issues to be solved, when trying to quantify real social interaction. The first one regards the complexity of capturing whole-body signals in ecological scenarios. State-of-the-art motioncapture technologies already allow accurate recording of whole-body kinematics, although these methods are typically expensive and time-consuming. At the same time, these methods completely neglect the dynamic component of body movement and thus the forces exchanged with the environment. On the other hand, data analysis is the real open issue when it comes to data interpretation. In fact, there is no standard procedure to analyze data or extract quantitative markers of group sensorimotor coordination and information flow between participants. These issues are the real bottleneck to allow the use of these methods in applied scenarios. Future work from our group directly aims at solving both issues. Finally, we believe that quantifying the sensorimotor markers of real social interaction in ecological contexts may have important applied relevance. For example, diagnosis and rehabilitation of social communication disorders may benefit from standardized tasks and quantitative methods extracting these automatic low-level motor markers of interaction. Indeed, such detailed description of interactive non-verbal behavior may be useful in the classification of patients’ subtypes, monitoring progress during rehabilitation or even comparing different protocols.

S201

References Badino L, D’Ausilio A, Glowinski D, Camurri A, Fadiga L (2014) Sensorimotor communication in professional quartets. Neuropsychologia 55:98–104 Bisio A, Stucchi N, Jacono M, Fadiga L, Pozzo T (2010) Automatic versus voluntary motor imitation: effect of visual context and stimulus velocity. PLoS One 5(10):e13506 Bisio A, Sciutti A, Nori F, Metta G, Fadiga L, Sandini G, Pozzo T (2014) Motor contagion during human–human and human–robot interaction. PLoS One 9(8):e106172 Casile A, Dayan E, Caggiano V, Hendler T, Flash T, Giese MA (2010) Neuronal encoding of human kinematic invariants during action observation. Cereb Cortex 20(7):1647–1655 D’Ausilio A, Badino L, Li Y, Tokay S, Craighero L, Canto R, Aloimonos Y, Fadiga L (2012) Leadership in orchestra emerges from the causal relationships of movement kinematics. PLoS One 7(5):e35757 D’Ausilio A, Novembre G, Fadiga L, Keller PE (2015) What can music tell us about social interaction? Trends Cogn Sci 19(3):111–114 Dumas G, Laroche J, Lehmann A (2014) Your body, my body, our coupling moves our bodies. Front Hum Neurosci 8:1004 Efron B, Tibshirani RJ (1993) An introduction to the bootstrap. Chapman & Hall, New York Glowinski D, Mancini M, Cowie R, Camurri A, Chiorri C, Doherty C (2013) The movements made by performers in a skilled quartet: a distinctive pattern, and the function that it serves. Front Psychol 4:841 Johansson G (1973) Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Percept Psychophys 14(2):201–211 Ne´da Z, Ravasz E, Brechet Y, Vicsek T, Baraba´si AL (2000) The sound of many hands clapping. Nature 403(6772):849–850 Pezzulo G, Dindo H (2011) What should I do next? using shared representations to solve interaction problems. Exp Brain Res 211(3–4):613–630 Pezzulo G, Donnarumma F, Dindo H (2013) Human sensorimotor communication: a theory of signaling in online social interactions. PLoS One 8(11):e79876 Riley MA, Richardson MJ, Shockley K, Ramenzoni VC (2011) Interpersonal synergies. Front Psychol 2:38 Sacheli LM, Tidoni E, Pavone EF, Aglioti SM, Candidi M (2013) Kinematics fingerprints of leader and follower role-taking during cooperative joint actions. Exp Brain Res 226(4):473–486 Schmidt RC, Fitzpatrick P, Caron R, Mergeche J (2011) Understanding social motor coordination. Hum Mov Sci 30(5):834–845 Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G (2006) Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci 10(2):70–76 Tognoli E, Lagarde J, DeGuzman GC, Kelso JA (2007) The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104(19):8190–8195 Vesper C, Richardson MJ (2014) Strategic communication and behavioral coupling in asymmetric joint action. Exp Brain Res 232(9):2945–2956 Vesper C, van der Wel RP, Knoblich G, Sebanz N (2011) Making oneself predictable: reduced temporal variability facilitates joint action coordination. Exp Brain Res 211(3–4):517–530

123

Automatic imitation of the arm kinematic profile in ... - Springer Link

Andrea Gaggioli2,3. Published online: 31 July 2015 ... human–robot interaction. Keywords Social neuroscience 4 Joint action 4 Automatic imitation 4 Kinematic ...

639KB Sizes 4 Downloads 305 Views

Recommend Documents

LNCS 6361 - Automatic Segmentation and ... - Springer Link
School of Eng. and Computer Science, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel. 2 ... OPG boundary surface distance error of 0.73mm and mean volume over- ... components classification methods are based on learning the grey-level range.

Handling Subject Arm Uncertainties for Upper Limb ... - Springer Link
Mar 3, 2016 - These uncertainties act as additive noise with input ..... J., et al., “Executive Summary: Heart Disease and Stroke Statistics-. 2015 Update: A ...

EEG Based Biometric Framework for Automatic Identity ... - Springer Link
The energy of brain potentials evoked during processing of visual stimuli is ... achieved by performing spatial data/sensor fusion, whereby the component ...

Management of Diabetes in Pregnancy - Springer Link
Dec 3, 2011 - profound effects on multiple maternal organ systems. In the fetus, morbidities ... mellitus . Metformin . Glyburide . Pregnancy; diabetes management. Clinical Trial Acronyms. ACHOIS Australian Carbohydrate Intolerance Study in. Pregnant

Real-time automatic license plate recognition for CCTV ... - Springer Link
Nov 19, 2011 - input video will be obtained via a dedicated, high-resolu- tion, high-speed camera and is/or supported by a controlled capture environment ...

Learning Contextual Metrics for Automatic Image ... - Springer Link
Mi) dx. (6). In our metric learning method, we regularize each metric by Euclidean distance function. So we add the following single regularization term into our ...

Visceral regeneration in the crinoid - Springer Link
sic characteristic of life, although it can be lost when their costs are higher than their ... individuals with visceral regeneration in progress [7, 25–28], indicates that the ... In the following stages, the regrowth of the intestinal tract can i

Existence of Dyons in the Coupled Georgi–Glashow ... - Springer Link
Feb 4, 2011 - results show that, while the magnetic charge is uniquely determined by the topological monopole number, the electric charge of a solution can be arbitrarily prescribed in an open interval. 1. Introduction. Particle-like static solutions

Finding Equivalent Rewritings in the Presence of ... - Springer Link
of its applications in a wide variety of data management problems, query op- ... The original definition of conjunctive queries does not allow for comparisons.

The role of attention in illusory conjunctions - Springer Link
a major source of support for the feature-integration the- ory since, unlike other ... either within the attended subset or outside it, but not be- tween the attended and ...... Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 247-250. POSNER, M. 1. (1980).

Climate and the evolution of serpentine endemism in ... - Springer Link
Nov 12, 2011 - that benign (e.g., high rainfall and less extreme temperatures) ... small populations with novel adaptations, and because competition with non- ...

Monitoring deterioration of vegetation cover in the ... - Springer Link
Mar 19, 2009 - a decrease in near infrared (NIR) and increase in the visible wavelengths ... the fact that soil spectra, lacking these properties, do not show a ...

Use of Patterns for Knowledge Management in the ... - Springer Link
Data Management), cPDm (collaborative Product Definition management) and PLM. (Product ... internal knowledge of the enterprise, and also to customers and stakeholders. In general ... and the patterns created in the chosen software.

Population and distribution of wolf in the world - Springer Link
In addition, 50 wolves live in the forest of the low ar- ... gulates groups (mostly red deer) to live forever. .... wolf, holding a meeting every a certain period, pub-.

The performance of rooks in a cooperative task ... - Springer Link
Received: 12 April 2009 / Revised: 30 October 2009 / Accepted: 6 December 2009 / Published online: 18 December 2009. © Springer-Verlag 2009. Abstract In ...

The Equivalence of Bayes and Causal Rationality in ... - Springer Link
revised definition of rationality given that is not subject to this criticism. .... is nonempty for every w ∈W (i.e. we assume that Bi is serial: (∀w)(∃x)wBix).

Frequency-dependence of the Switching Voltage in ... - Springer Link
Seoul National University of Science and Engineering, Seoul 01811, Korea. I-Wei Chen ... University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104, USA.

The Equivalence of Bayes and Causal Rationality in ... - Springer Link
Page 3 .... is expressed formally by the own-choice knowledge condition: (OK) For all i and for all w,x∈W, if wBix then ... In order to introducing trembles, we would need to make ... be inter-connected: my opponents' choices given that I play si.

The challenge of ethical behavior in organizations - Springer Link
Ronatd R. Sims is Associate Professor in the School of Business. Administration at the ... and management training and development, and organizational transitions. His articles ... While these challenges must all be met by organiza- tions and manager

The Incredible Economics of Geoengineering - Springer Link
Dec 6, 2007 - As I shall explain in this paper, its future application seems more likely than not. ... because the incentives for countries to experiment with ...

The Strength of Weak Learnability - Springer Link
high probability, the hypothesis must be correct for all but an arbitrarily small ... be able to achieve arbitrarily high accuracy; a weak learning algorithm need only ...

The time course of amplitude speciWcation in brief ... - Springer Link
Apr 16, 2008 - This limits the time available to prepare the move- ment. To deal with short preparation intervals, performers are likely to prepare a motor ...