NOVA

Science Publishers, Inc.

SCHIZOTYPAL PERSONALITY TRAITS: AUDITORY HALLUCINATION-LIKE EXPERIENCES AND ATYPICAL HEMISPHERIC LATERALIZATION

NS P

400 Oser Avenue, Suite 1600 Hauppauge, N. Y. 11788-3619 Phone (631) 231-7269 Fax (631) 231-8175 E-mail: [email protected] http://www.novapublishers.com

Tomohisa Asai Eriko Sugimori Yoshihiko Tanno In: "Advances in Psychology Research. Volume 74" Editor: Alexandra M. Columbus ISBN: 978-1-61761-985-4 2011

In: Advances in Psychology Research. Volume 74 Editor: Alexandra M. Columbus, pp. 127-143

ISBN 978-1-61761-985-4 © 2011 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

The exclusive license for this PDF is limited to personal website use only. No part of this digital document may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted commercially in any form or by any means. The publisher has taken reasonable care in the preparation of this digital document, but makes no expressed or implied warranty of any kind and assumes no responsibility for any errors or omissions. No liability is assumed for incidental or consequential damages in connection with or arising out of information contained herein. This digital document is sold with the clear understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering legal, medical or any other professional services.

Chapter 7

SCHIZOTYPAL PERSONALITY TRAITS: AUDITORY HALLUCINATION-LIKE EXPERIENCES AND ATYPICAL HEMISPHERIC LATERALIZATION Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno Department of Cognitive and Behavioral Science Graduate School of Arts and Sciences The University of Tokyo

ABSTRACT Individual differences in schizotypal personality traits (schizotypy), which might be the predisposition to schizophrenia, have commonly been explored as a means of examining the nature and structure of schizophrenia symptoms. Research on schizotypal personality in the general population may provide a particular opportunity to study the biological and cognitive markers of vulnerability to schizophrenia without the confounding effects of long-term hospitalization, medication, and severe psychotic symptoms (Raine and Lencz, 1995). A systematic review of general-population surveys indicated that the experiences associated with schizophrenia and related categories, such as paranoid delusional thinking and auditory hallucinations, are observed in an attenuated form in 5–8% of healthy people (Os et al., 2009). These attenuated expressions could be regarded as the behavioral marker of an underlying risk for schizophrenia and related disorders, just as high blood pressure indicates high susceptibility for cardiovascular disease in a dose– response fashion (Os and Kapur, 2009). Auditory hallucination (AH) refers to the perception that one‘s own inner speech originates outside the self. Patients with AH make external misattributions of the source of perceived speech. Recent studies have suggested that auditory hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia might occur in the right hemisphere, where they might produce irregular and unpredicted inner speech, which their auditory and sensory feedback processing system does not attribute to themselves. 

Tel: +81-3-5454-6259 Fax: +81-3-5454-6979 [email protected]

128

Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno In the present study, general participants judged self–other attribution in speech subjectively in response to on-line auditory feedback presented through their right, left, and both ears. People with high auditory-hallucination-like experiences made external misattributions more frequently under the right- and left-ear only conditions compared with the both-ears condition. We interpreted this result as suggesting that people with a high degree of proneness to AH might have disorders in both the right and left hemispheric language-related areas: speech perception deficit in the left hemisphere and prediction violation in speech processing in the right hemisphere. A perspective that situates schizophrenia on a continuum with general personality variations implies that this disorder constitutes a potential risk for everyone and, thus, helps to promote understanding and correct misunderstandings that contribute to prejudice.

INTRODUCTION Many people with schizophrenia describe a sense of passivity to their experiences, in that their actions, thoughts, or emotions are experienced as created for them by some external agent rather than by their own will. These positive symptoms of schizophrenia are included among Schneider‘s first-rank symptoms for the diagnosis of schizophrenia (Mellors, 1970; Schneider, 1959). In most cases, the actions carried out when people feel that they are being controlled by alien forces are not discrepant with their intentions (Frith, Blakemore and Wolpert, 2000a, 2000b). In other words, people with schizophrenia have an abnormal sense of agency, that is, of the feeling of causing our own actions (Gallagher, 2000). Phenomena such as delusions of control, auditory hallucinations, and thought insertion may all be caused by an abnormal sense of agency (Frith et al., 2000a; Gallagher, 2004; Lindner, Their, Kircher, Haarmeier and Leube, 2005). For example, one‘s own speech could seem to be auditory hallucinations (McGuigan, 1966). The activation of Broca‘s area, which can produce but cannot listen to speech, has been associated with auditory hallucinations (McGuire, Shah and Murray, 1993). Therefore, these people might produce speech but not think that they actually spoke. As a result, they may hear their own voices as the voices of others. The abnormal sense of agency in schizophrenia has been shown empirically. Some studies reported that when required to make judgments about the origin of hand actions or movements based upon biased feedback (self-action recognition task), people with schizophrenia were more likely than normal controls to misattribute their own actions (Daprati et al., 1997; Franck et al., 2001), which might be interpreted as delusions of control. As well, schizophrenic patients with auditory hallucinations tend to misattribute their own speech (self-speech recognition task: e.g., Johns et al., 1999; 2001). It was recently suggested that psychopathological models of schizophrenia that include the sense of agency may also apply to schizotypal personality traits (schizotypy). Cyhlarova and Claridge (2005) indicated that schizotypal people, identified by questionnaires or semi-structured interviews, might have a predisposition to schizophrenia. Although schizotypal people can have schizophreniclike experiences, many can live normal lives. The traits of schizophrenia are generally considered to span a continuum. By applying the paradigms of the previous studies (Johns et al., 1999; 2001; Franck et al., 2001; Sato and Yasuda, 2005), Asai and Tanno (2007, 2008) found that people high in schizotypy also tend to have an abnormal sense of agency in their actions including speech both on the explicit and implicit measures (Asai et al., submitted).

Schizotypal Personality Traits

129

Individual differences in schizotypal personality have commonly been explored as a means of examining the nature and structure of schizophrenia symptoms. Research on schizotypal personality in the general population may provide a particular opportunity to study the biological and cognitive markers of vulnerability to schizophrenia without the confounding effects of long-term hospitalization, medication, and severe psychotic symptoms (Raine and Lencz, 1995). Relatives of schizophrenia patients score significantly higher on measures of schizotypal personality, which suggests that within the spectrum of schizophrenia disorders there is a range in which schizotypal traits may be expressed, and that this range is at least partly genetic (Kremen, Faraone, Toomey, Seidman and Tsuang, 1998; Lenzenweger, 2006; Plated and Gallup, 2002). Recent studies have suggested that in schizophrenia, an abnormal sense of agency might be caused by an abnormal action prediction system in the sense of the agency model (e.g., Frith et al., 2000a; 2000b; Blakemore et al., 2002; Seal, Alemam, and McGuire, 2004; Jones and Fernyhough, 2007. In this model, two information processing pathways are implicated (the action-predictive and the executive pathways), and the discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensations causes the misplaced sense of agency (the sense that ―I am the one who generates the action.‖). With auditory hallucinations and self–other attributions in speech, patients with schizophrenia experiencing auditory hallucinations and those in the general population with high auditory-hallucination proneness do not attribute their own speech to themselves (e.g., Johns et al., 2001; Asai et al., submitted), and furthermore, they do not show sensory cancellation of self-produced speech, indicating that their implicit external misattribution occurs on a neural level (Ford and Mathalon, 2004, 2005; Ford et al., 2001). How does abnormal prediction lead to the external misattribution in speech? One possibility is that abnormal hemispheric lateralization occurs in patients with schizophrenia. Atypical cerebral lateralization may represent a risk factor for developing schizophrenia (Crow, 2004). Patients with schizophrenia have shown differences in lateralization as measured by handedness (Reilly et al., 2001), and schizotypal personality scores are increased among mixed-handed general participants (Annett and Moran, 2006; Somers, Sommer, Boks, and Kahn, 2008) even in non-Western cultures (Asai and Tanno, 2009). These results might implicate a relationship between schizophrenia or schizotypal personality and non-lateralized cerebral functioning, at least with regard to motor ability. Furthermore, in schizophrenia, nonlateralization has been empirically suggested not only in motor skills, but also in language functions (e.g., Asai et al., 2009; Blyler, Maher, Manschreck, and Fenton, 1997; Hugdahl et al., 2008; Lenzenweger and Maher, 2002; Lohr and Caligiuri, 1997; Ngan et al., 2003; Tabarés-Seisdedos et al., 2003). Moreover, it has been suggested that the essential disconnection between the two hemispheres might equalize the two hemispheres, at least in relation to some motor and language functions (Asai et al., 2009). Patients with schizophrenia might have second language areas in the right hemisphere (e.g., ―bicameral mind;‖ for a review, see Cavanna et al., 2007; Sher, 2000; Olin, 1999). Many studies have shown that the right homologues of the language-related areas are activated during auditory verbal hallucination (e.g., Sommer et al., 2007). In line with this discussion, in considering the relationship between abnormal hemispheric laterality in schizophrenia and external misattribution in speech, Sommer et al. (2003) suggested a challenging but important hypothesis that connected the major theories explaining schizophrenia. As mentioned above, the decreased cerebral asymmetry found in schizophrenia has been replicated using several techniques. In addition, functional

130

Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno

imagingstudies have reported decreased lateralization of language-related activation in patients with schizophrenia compared with healthy controls (Sommer et al., 2001). It could be hypothesized that inner speech, originating from right cerebral homologues of the language areas, is perceived as an auditory hallucination. In the right hemisphere, ―prediction violation‖ might occur. Self-produced language activity normally leads to inhibition of the language perception areas (McGuire et al., 1996) because self-produced sensation can be predicted accurately and filtered (e.g., Blackmore et al., 1998). When this inhibitory mechanism fails, verbal thoughts may not be recognized as originating from the self and may be attributed erroneously to an external source. Indeed, inhibition of language perception might be more prone to failure when language activity is derived from an unusual site (i.e., from contralateral homologue areas in the right hemisphere). These serial hypotheses are summarized in Figure 1. Patients with auditory hallucinations might have a second language area in the right hemisphere, but this irregular language processing may not be predicted, and feedback sensations may not be filtered or attenuated. As a result of this prediction violation, individuals would perceive self-produced (inner) voices as originating externally. To examine this hypothesis, we administered a simple experiment assessing self–other speech attribution for each ear (hemisphere) in relation to auditory-hallucination proneness in the general population, as with our previous studies (Asai et al., submitted; 2008).

Note: SP, area for speech perpection; solid lines, direct links between areas; dashed line, transmission though corpus callosum. Figure 1. Schematic of speech perception paths of intra and interhemispheric information transfer in general population and people with AVH (Altered for Asai et al., 2009).

METHODS Participants Fifty-one university students (aged 18–22 years, mean = 19.6; 30 men, 21 women) participated in the experiment and completed the questionnaire. Participants were recruited from a pool of students in an introductory psychology class. We sent an e-mail describing the experiment and the questionnaire, to which interested participants responded voluntarily. None had a history of mental disease or hearing difficulties, and none reported any hearing

Schizotypal Personality Traits

131

problems at the time of the experiments. We obtained written informed consent from all participants before conducting the experiment.

Apparatus The experiment was conducted in a soundproof room. The visual stimuli were created and the experiments conducted using MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) and Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). Participants spoke into a microphone attached to headphones (SENNHEISER HMD280PRO), and the auditory input was amplified (BEHRINGER MIC200). Auditory input below 1 kHz was filtered at +3 dB, and auditory input above 1kHz was filtered at -3 dB using an equalizer (BEHRINGER FBQ800) in order to obtain a subjective approximation of each participant‘s own voice (Shuster and Durrant, 2003). Then, the pitch was either unchanged (no distortion) or raised by 1 (moderate) or 4 (severe) semi-tones using an effecter (ZOOM RFX-2200) and fed back to the subjects through headphones (SENNHEISER HMD280PRO). These parameters were decided in accordance with our previous study (Asai et al., submitted). We predicted that participants would attribute the fed-back voice to themselves about 50% of the time under the moderatedistortion condition but 0 % of the time under the severe condition. To reduce any effects of bone conduction and to prevent participants from hearing their own voices directly, pink noise at 70 dB SPL was generated and mixed into the fed-back voice using a sound mixer (YAMAHA MW10C) (Toyomura et al., 2007). Their fed-back voice was presented to the right, the left, or both ears through headphones using the mixer (pan-pot function) so that the total signal levels were equal under each ear condition. Pink noise was also presented through the bone-conduction headphone (Golden Dance co., Ltd. MGD-701BK) to suppress the bone conduction sound of their speech. For example, under the right-ear condition, while pink noise was presented to both ears through the headphones, participants first read aloud the presented word and then listened as their voice was presented to the right ear only through the headphones. During this time, only pink noise was presented to the left ear. A few signals were sent from the right ear to the right auditory cortex (ipsilateral) through the preponderance of the contralateral auditory pathways (Rosenzweig, 1951). The noise from the contralateral ear was supposed to reduce this ipsilateral pathway relatively (Asai et al., 2009). Participants were briefly trained to speak aloud at approximately 65–70 dB, as if murmuring. Under these conditions, each participant was able to clearly recognize their voice over the noise through the headphones.

Materials We used 50 four-mora (the prosodic unit in Japanese) words as visual stimuli, selected from an established list of words for speech (Amano et al., 2009). This list was controlled for familiarity and phonological balance. We chose 50 of the most familiar words for the present experiment (five for the practice trials and 45 for the test trials).

132

Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno

Procedure Participants read the presented words aloud into the microphone. They were required to finish reading within approximately 400 ms after the words were presented following a countdown from three to one, during which time they could prepare for speech while viewing the presented words. After they heard the fed-back voice, participants were required to judge intuitively whether the voice they heard was their own, regardless of the feeling of distortion. That is, they could answer, ―This is my own voice,‖ even when they could detect distortion, and vice versa (self-other attribution task). Nine conditions were used (three distortion conditions × three ear conditions), and participants completed 45 trials (five repetitions for each of the nine conditions). The orders of the 45 words and the three conditions were double randomized.

Questionnaire After the participants finished the experiment, they completed a battery of questionnaires. We assigned a random number to each participant, and data from the questionnaires were crosschecked with experimental data using this randomly assigned number. The battery included the following questionnaires, all of which, except for the AHES-17 and SOAS, have been translated into Japanese and demonstrate good reliability and validity. 1. Auditory hallucination proneness: the Auditory Hallucination Experience Scale 17 (AHES-17, Asai et al., 2009) is a brief version of the Auditory Hallucination Experience Scale (AHES, Sugimori, Asai, and Tanno, 2009), which has been developed in Japan because a scale for directly measuring auditory hallucination-like experiences was needed. The Launay–Slade Hallucination Scale (LSHS; Launay and Slade, 1981) and its revised version (LSHS-R; Waters, Badcock, and Matbery, 2003) measure hallucination-like experiences, including auditory hallucinations, but do not focus on auditory hallucinations separately. The AHES-17 is a self-report 17-item questionnaire with responses based on a 5-point Likert scale (1–5) measuring the frequency of auditory hallucination-like experiences (e.g., ―I heard someone‘s voice, but nobody was actually around.‖). The scores for this scale range from 17 to 85. Test–retest reliability (r = 0.78, p < .0001) and internal reliability (α = 0.84) were adequate, and the investigation of criterion-related validity showed that the AHES-17 was highly correlated with scales measuring positive schizotypy, including auditory hallucination proneness (Asai et al., 2009). 2. Positive schizotypal personality: the Oxford Schizotypal Personality Scale (STA; Claridge and Broks, 1984; Cyhlarova and Claridge, 2005; Gregory, Claridge, Clark, and Taylor, 2003) is a 37-item true–false self-report questionnaire based on the DSM-III diagnostic criteria for schizotypal personality disorder. It measures schizotypal traits, especially perceptual aberrations that are analogous to positive symptoms, such as auditory hallucinations, thought insertions, and delusions of control.

Schizotypal Personality Traits

133

3. Schizotypal personality traits: the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire Brief (SPQB: Raine and Benishay, 1995) is a shortened version of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ: Raine, 1991). SPQB is a 22-item true–false selfreport questionnaire measuring schizotypal personality traits. Though it consists of three subscales: Cognitive–Perceptual (Cog: positive schizotypy), Interpersonal (Int: negative schizotypy), and Disorganization (Dis: disorganized schizotypy), resent studies have suggested that this three factor structures in SPQ(B) might be open to question (e.g., Compton et al., 2009; Fonseca-Pedrero et al., 2009). Furthermore, each subscale score ranges, for example, from 0 to 6 (Dis). So we didn‘t use these subscale scores and adopted total SPQB score as whole schizotypal personality. 4. Sense of agency: the Sense of Agency Scale (SOAS; Asai et al., 2009) is a newly developed prototype measure for assessing the sense of agency. It includes 22 items that are related to an abnormal sense of agency in the general population according to previous experimental studies. The scale consists of three subscales: misattribution of the agent (Mental Self: e.g., ―I sometimes turn around feeling as if someone called my name in a crowd.‖), uncontrollability of one‘s own body (Physical Self: e.g., ―I sometimes feel I cannot move my body as I want.‖), and self-assertiveness in social situations (Social Self: e.g., ―I sometimes feel my behavior has some effect on society.‖). Responses are based on a 4-point Likert scale. Test–retest reliability (r = 0.73, p < .0001) and internal reliability (α = 0.77) were adequate. 5. Depression: the Self-rating Depression Scale (SDS; Zung, 1965) is a well-known self-report questionnaire comprised of 20 items; responses are based on a 4-point Likert scale measuring depressive tendencies. 6. Anxiety: the Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T; Spielberger, Gorsuch, and Lushene, 1970) is a well-known self-report questionnaire consisting of 20 items; responses are based on a 4-point Likert scale measuring anxiety traits. 7. Handedness: the H.N. Handedness Scale (HNHS; Hatta and Kawakami, 1995; Hatta and Nakatsuka, 1975) is a revised version of the Edinburgh Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) for use with Japanese participants. Revisions were necessary because cultural differences render the original Edinburgh Inventory inappropriate for Japanese participants. The scale is often used in Japan to measure or control for handedness (e.g., Ogawa, Inui, and Sugio, 2006). Participants respond to this scale by indicating whether they use their right, left, or either hand for 10 common actions: handling an eraser; striking a match; thumb tacking; hammering; brushing their teeth; throwing; and using a pair of scissors, a knife, a screwdriver, and a shaver or lipstick. This scale ranges from -10 to +10; a ‗‗right‖ response is scored as +1, a ‗‗left‖ response is scored as -1 and a response of ‗‗either‖ is scored as zero. Higher scores for all questionnaires indicate a stronger tendency on the relevant dimension. SDS and STAI were used as control measures; that is, we predicted that these scales would not be related to the present study although they might be related to schizotypal personality traits, especially to auditory hallucination proneness or abnormal experiences relating to the sense of agency (that is, ―Mental Self‖).

134

Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno

RESULTS At least two methods are available to examine the relationship between questionnaire scores and experimental performances: linear relationships between scores and performance (e.g., Asai et al., 2008) and comparisons between groups according to questionnaire scores (e.g., Asai and Tanno, 2007). Multiple rather than single measures are most reliable (e.g., Asai et al., 2009). To examine the relationship between auditory-hallucination proneness and self-attribution, first we selected the participants scoring highest (top third; 17 participants) and lowest (bottom third; 17 participants) on the AHES (see Table 1 for demographic data), in accordance with previous studies. Later, we examined the relationships of our data to other questionnaires and other methods (e.g., linear relationships) to examine the individual differences. Figures 2 and 3 show the relationship between self-attribution in speech and ear condition in each group, as in Asai and Tanno‘s (2008) study. Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with pitch distortion and the ear condition as the within-subject variables showed that only the main effect of pitch distortion was statistically significant (F(2, 32) = 39.6, p< .001) in the low-AHES group (Figure 2). In contrast, in the high AHES group, the main effects of pitch distortion (F(2, 32) = 152.5, p< .001) and of ear condition (F(2, 32) = 4.01, p< .05) were significant (Figure 3). Regarding the main effects of ear condition, multiple comparisons using Ryan‘s method (i.e., R-E-G-W‘s F test) for post-hoc analysis revealed that scores under the both-ears condition were significantly higher than under either one-ear condition (right or left) (p< .05). These results suggest a relationship between the AHES, selfattribution in speech, and ear (hemisphere) conditions. In the low AHES group, there was no difference between the ear conditions. In contrast, in the high AHES group, less feedback was attributed to participants‘ own voice under the one-ear conditions (right or left) than under the both-ears condition, regardless of the pitch distortion. To confirm these results, we then examined the linear relationship among these variables. Table 1. The demographical data (Mean and Standard Deviation) between the low and the AHES groups

Note: The significances were revealed by χ2 tests for sex ratio and the number of left-handers (HNHS scores <-4), and MANOVA for Age-STAI. HNHS=Hatta and Nakatsuka Handedness Scale, AHES=Auditory Hallcuination Experience Scale, STA=Schizotypal personality scale, SPQB=Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire Brief, SOAS=Sense of Agency Scale, SDS=Selfrating Depression Scale, STAI=Trait anxiety inventory.

Schizotypal Personality Traits

Figure 2. Self-attribution ratio and ear condition in lowAHES group.

Figure 3. Self-attribution ratio and ear condition in highAHES group.

135

136

Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno Table 2. Inter-correlations between the experimental laterality indexes and questionnaire scores

Note: N = 51. Values are Pearson‘s correlation coefficients. Correlations were tested for statistical significance using a one-tailed procedure based on a directional a priori hypothesis. LIR= (right ear)-(both ears), LIL=(left ear)-(both ears).

We calculated the two laterality indices under the both-ears conditions as baseline scores in accordance with the results of the group comparisons. The laterality index for the right ear (LIR) was calculated as follows: (self-attribution rate under the right-ear condition) - (selfattribution rate under the both-ears condition). Finally, these values were averaged across pitch distortion conditions. The laterality index for the left ear (LIL) was calculated in the same way. Negative LIR and LIL values meant that the participant attributed the fed-back voice to him or herself less under the one-ear condition (right or left) than under the both-ears condition. Table 2 shows the Pearson‘s correlation matrix for these laterality indexes and questionnaire scores. Not surprisingly, AHES and LIR or LIL showed a significant negative correlation, confirming the results of the group comparison analysis. Furthermore, this correlation matrix revealed that other schizotypal traits, including positive or whole schizotypy, and other mental traits, including anxiety and depression, might not related to the lateralized self-attribution pattern. Auditory-hallucination proneness might be specific to this phenomenon. On the sense of agency scale, the ―Mental Self (misattribution of the agent)‖ subscale was significantly correlated with LIL. Although the self–other attribution task is supposed to relate to this factor, unlike AHES, ―Mental Self‖ was only correlated with LIL. We had hypothesized that people with high AHES would attribute the fed-back voice to themselves less under the left-ear (right hemisphere) condition than under either the both-ears or the right-ear condition. The results, however, suggest that under the right-ear condition, they also attributed the speech to themselves less. Yet, according to the results of SOAS, external misattribution of the agent should be related to the left ear (right hemisphere) condition only, as we hypothesized above. These findings are discussed below.

DISCUSSION The objective in the present study was to examine self–other speech attribution in the right hemisphere in people with high proneness to auditory hallucination. First, in people with low AH, laterality of self–other speech attribution was not confirmed. They attributed the feedback voice to themselves with equal frequency regardless of ear condition. Fu et al. (2006) showed the relationship between bilateral temporal lobes and on-line self–other speech

Schizotypal Personality Traits

137

attribution: the speech information is sent to left language areas first and might then be returned to the bilateral temporal lobes. No effect of ear condition might indicate smooth transmission of these signals through the corpus callosum. In general, a laterality effect of ear condition in the general population would be observed only under specific experimental conditions, namely classic dichotic listening, in which participants hear a different auditory stimulus in each ear (e.g., Hugdahl et al., 2007) or a reaction time paradigm (e.g., Asai et al., 2009), because of the connection between the two hemispheres through the corpus callosum. Considering that the present study presented one speech stimulus and required participants to answer at their own pace, the absence of laterality effect in people with low AH was unsurprising. On the other hand, the effect of ear condition appears in people with high AH. Because previous studies have suggested that external misattribution in speech (i.e., auditory hallucination) might occur in the right hemisphere (e.g., Sommer et al., 2007), we had hypothesized that, if an inter-hemispherical disconnection exists (e.g., Lohr et al., 2006)), under the left-ear condition, people with high AH would be more likely to attribute the fedback voice to an external source (i.e., to feel it is ―the other‘s voice‖) than under the right-ear or both-ears conditions. Compared with the both-ears condition, under the left-ear condition, participants with high AH did misattribute the fed-back voice to an external source, as expected. According to the hypothesis of Sommer et al. (2003), speech processing may occur in the right hemisphere (cerebral homologues of the language areas) in subjects with AH. This irregular language processing, however, cannot be predicted, and the feedback sensation cannot be filtered or attenuated (e.g., Ford et al., 2004). As a result of this prediction violation, subjects would hear the self-produced (inner) voices as the voices of others. The present study supports this view, providing the first data from a behavioral experiment in line with this challenging hypothesis, but from the viewpoint of the difference between the bothears and left-ear conditions. However, contrary to our expectation, the right-ear condition showed results similar to those for the left-ear condition. People with high AH made an equally high number of external misattributions under the right-ear condition compared with the both-ears condition. The ―Mental Self‖ subscale in SOAS refers to the tendency for misattribution of the agent, including auditory hallucination. Participants with high Mental Self scores may also externally misattribute information reaching the right hemisphere. Given that AHES was highly correlated with Mental Self (r = .53, p< .001), this result seems inevitable. Under the right-ear condition, however, Mental Self was not correlated with LIR, indicating that the difference between the right-ear and both-ears condition may not have been the result of misattribution of the agent, but may have occurred for some other reason. Indeed, many studies have suggested that the language-related areas in the left hemisphere of patients with AH might be disordered. Patients with AH might have a deficit in speech perception related to the left temporal lobe, regardless of whether the speech originates with the self or another (e.g., Hugdahl et al., 2007). In addition, Mechelli et al. (2007) suggested that when patients with AH appraised their own speech after the fact, they show impaired functional integration between the left superior temporal and anterior cingulate cortex. In a postmortem study, Allen et al. (2007) also showed that the misidentification of self-generated speech in patients with AH was associated with functional abnormalities in the anterior cingulate and left temporal cortex. These results suggested that patients with AH might have an abnormality in the left temporal

138

Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno

cortex that leaves them deficient in a self-voice representation. This creates a disorder in speech perception when they identify theirs retrospectively rather than on line. Therefore, it could be that not the on-line ―prediction violation‖ but other reasons, perhaps including disorders of speech perception, explain the difference between the right-ear and both-ears condition. Further research should examine this possibility using brain-imaging techniques. When taken together, our findings indicate that people with AH might have disorders in both the right and left language-related areas: speech perception deficit in the left hemisphere and prediction violation in speech processing in the right hemisphere. Indeed, Woodruff et al. (1997) have suggested that AH is associated with a reduced left and increased right temporal cortical response to auditory perception of speech. Patients with AH process speech stimuli less well in the left hemisphere but better in the right hemisphere. However, if speech stimuli are produced by the patients themselves, the speech being processed in the right hemisphere might be perceived as another‘s voice. Further research should focus on both right and left hemisphere language processing and self-other attribution in speech. Although the present study showed the effect of laterality on self–other speech attribution for the first time by behavioral experiment, some unavoidable limitations should be noted. Because we discussed brain abnormalities from behavioral data alone, speculative interpretations were inevitable. Although the dichotic listening paradigm (e.g., Hugdahl et al., 2008), which we applied in the present study, has been developed to provide non-invasive brain measurements, particularly regarding neural pathways, a follow-up brain imaging study should provide further insight on this topic. We could present a basic experimental methodology for further research.

REFERENCES Allen, P., Amaro, E., Fu, C.H., Williams, S.C., Brammer, M.J., Johns, L.C., and McGuire, P.K. (2007). Neural correlates of the misattribution of speech in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 162-169. Amano, S., Sakamoto, S., Kondo, T. and Suzuki, Y. (2009). Development of familiaritycontrolled word lists 2003 (FW03) to assess spoken-word intelligibility in Japanese. Speech Communication, 51, 76-82. Annett, M. and Moran, P. (2006). Schizotypy is increased in mixed-handers, especially righthanded writers who use the left hand for primary actions. Schizophrenia Research, 81, 239-246. Asai, T., and Tanno, Y. (2007). The relationship between the sense of self-agency and schizotypal personality traits. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 162-168. Asai, T., and Tanno, Y. (2008). Highly schizotypal students have a weaker sense of selfagency. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 62, 115-119. Asai, T., and Tanno, Y. (2009). Schizotypy and handedness in Japanese participants revisited. Laterality, 14, 86-94. Asai, T., Takano, K., Sugimori, E. and Tanno, Y. (2009). Development of the sense of agency scale and its factor structure. The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 80, 414-421 (in Japanese). Bentler, P.M., Bonnet, D.G.,. 1980. Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238-246

Schizotypal Personality Traits

139

Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., and Frith, C. D. (1998). Central cancellation of selfproduced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 635-640. Blakemore, S.J., Wolpert, D.M., and Frith, C.D. (1998). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 635-640. Blyler, C. R., Maher, B. A., Manschreck, T. C., and Fenton, W. S. (1997). Line drawing as a possible measure of lateralized motor performance in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 26, 15-23. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433-436. Browne, M.W., and Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K.A. Bollen and J.S. Long (Eds.) Testing Structural Equation Models. Beverley Hills, CA: Sage, 132-162 Cavanna, A.E., Trimble, M., Cinti, F., and Monaco, F. (2007). The "bicameral mind" 30 years on: a critical reappraisal of Julian Jaynes' hypothesis. Functional Neurology, 22, 11-15. Cavanna, A.E., Trimble, M., Cinti, F., and Monaco, F. (2007). The "bicameral mind" 30 years on: a critical reappraisal of Julian Jaynes' hypothesis. Functional Neurology, 22, 11-15. Chapman, J.P., Chapman, L.J., and Allen, J.J. (1987). The measurement of foot preference. Neuropsychologia, 25, 579-584. Claridge, G., and Broks, P. (1984). Schizotypy and hemisphere function: I. Theoretical considerations and the measurement of schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 643-670. Compton, M.T., Goulding, S.M., Bakeman, R., and McClure-Tone, E.B. (2009). Confirmation of a four-factor structure of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire among undergraduate students. Schizophrenia Research, 111, 46-52. Crow, T. J. (2004). Cerebral asymmetry and the lateralization of language: core deficits in schizophrenia as pointers to the gene. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 17, 97-106. Cyhlarova, E., and Claridge, G. (2005). Development of a version of the Schizotypy Traits Questionnaire (STA) for screening children. Schizophrenia Research, 80, 253-261. Daprati, E., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Proust, J., Pacherie, E., Dalery, J., and Jeannerod, M. (1997). Looking for the agent: An investigation into consciousness of action and selfconsciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition, 65, 71-86. Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Paíno-Piñeiro, M., Lemos-Giráldez, S., Villazón-García, U., and Muñiz, J. (2009). Validation of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief Form in adolescents. Schizophrenia Research, 111,53-60. Ford, J. M., and Mathalon, D. H. (2004). Electrophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia during talking and thinking. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 38, 37-46. Ford, J. M., and Mathalon, D. H. (2005). Corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia: Can it explain auditory hallucination? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 58, 179-189. Ford, J. M., Mathalon, D. H., Heinks, T., Kalba, S., Faustman, W. O., and Roth, W. T. (2001). Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 2069-2071. Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N., Marie-Cardine, M., Dalery, J., d‘Amato, T., and Jeannerod, M. (2001). Defective recognition of one‘s own actions in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 454-459.

140

Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno

Frith, C. (2005). The neural basis of hallucinations and delusions. Comptes Rendus Biologies, 328, 169-175. Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000a). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia:Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 357-363. Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000b). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 355, 1771-1788. Fu, C.H., Vythelingum, G.N., Brammer, M.J., Williams, S.C., Amaro, E. Jr, Andrew, C.M., Yágüez, L., van Haren, N.E., Matsumoto, K., and McGuire, P.K. (2006). An fMRI study of verbal self-monitoring: neural correlates of auditory verbal feedback. Cereb Cortex,16, 969-77. Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 14-21. Gallagher, S. (2004). Neurocognitive models of schizophrenia: a neurophenomenological critique. Psychopathology, 37, 8-19. Gregory, A. M., Claridge, G., Clark, K., and Taylor, P. D. (2003). Handedness and schizotypy in a Japanese sample: an association masked by cultural effects on hand usage. Schizophrenia Research, 65, 139-145. Hatta, T., and Kawakami, A. (1995). Patterns of handedness in modern Japanese: a cohort effect shown by re-administration of the H.N. Handedness Inventory after 20 years. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 505-512. Hatta, T., and Nakatsuka, Z. (1975). Handedness inventory. In: Ohno, D. (Ed.), Papers on Celebrating the 63rd Birthday of Prof. Ohnishi. Osaka City University, Osaka, Japan, pp. 224-245. Hu, Li-tze, and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. Hugdahl, K., Løberg, E. M., Jørgensen, H. A., Lundervold, A., Lund, A., Green, M. F., and Rund, B. (2008). Left hemisphere lateralisation of auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: a dichotic listening study. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 13, 166-179. Hugdahl, K., Løberg, E.M., Specht, K., Steen, V.M., van Wageningen, H, and Jørgensen, H.A. (2007). Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: the role of cognitive, brain structural and genetic disturbances in the left temporal lobe. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,1, 6. Johns, L. C., and McGuire, P. K. (1999). Verbal self-monitoring and auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Lancet, 353, 469-470. Johns, L. C., Rossell, S., Frith, C., Ahmad, F., Hemsley, D., Kuipers, E., and McGuire, P.K. (2001). Verbal self-monitoring and auditory verbal hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 31, 705-715. Kremen, W. S., Faraone, S. V., Toomey, R., Seidman, L. J., and Tsuang, M. T. (1998). Sex differences in self-reported schizotypal traitsin relatives of schizophrenic probands. Schizophrenia Research, 34, 27-37. Launay, B. and Slade, P. D. (1981). The measurement of hallucinatory predisposition in male and female prisoners. Personality and Individual Differences, 2, 221-234.

Schizotypal Personality Traits

141

Lenzenweger, M. F. (2006). Schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia: Paul E. Meehl's blueprint for the experimental psychopathology and genetics of schizophrenia. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 115, 195-200. Lenzenweger, M. F., and Maher, B. A. (2002). Psychometric schizotypy and motor performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 546-55. Levy, J. (1974). Psychobiological implications of bilateral asymmetry. In S. J. Dimond and G. Beaumont (Eds.). Hemispheric function in the human brain. London: Elek Science. Lindner, A., Their, P., Kircher, T. T. J., Haarmeier, T., and Leube, D. T. (2005). Disorders of agency in schizophrenia correlate with an inability to compensate for the sensory consequences of actions. Current Biology, 15, 1119-1124. Lohr, J. B., and Caligiuri, M. P. (1997). Lateralized hemispheric dysfunction in the major psychotic disorders: historical perspectives and findings from a study of motor asymmetry in older patients. Schizophrenia Research, 27, 191-198. McGuigan, F. J. (1966). Covert oral behaviour and auditory hallucinations. Psychiatry, 158, 307-316. McGuire, P. K., Shah, G. M., and Murray, R. M. (1993). Increased blood flow in Broca‘s area during auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Lancet, 342, 703-706. McGuire, P.K., Silbersweig, D.A., Wright, I., Murray, R.M., Frackowiak, R.S., and Frith, C.D. (1996). The neural correlates of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery in schizophrenia: relationship to auditory verbal hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry,169,148-159. McGuire, P.K., Silbersweig, D.A., Wright, I., Murray, R.M., Frackowiak, R.S., and Frith, C.D. (1996). The neural correlates of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery in schizophrenia: relationship to auditory verbal hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 148-159. Mechelli, A., Allen, P., Amaro, E. Jr, Fu, C.H., Williams, S.C., Brammer, M.J., Johns, L.C., and McGuire, P.K. (2007). Misattribution of speech and impaired connectivity in patients with auditory verbal hallucinations. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 1213-22. Mellors, C. S. (1970). First rank symptoms of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 117, 15-23. Ngan, E. T., Vouloumanos, A., Cairo, T. A., Laurens, K. R., Bates, A. T., Anderson, C. M., Werker, J. F., and Liddle, P. F. (2003). Abnormal processing of speech during oddball target detection in schizophrenia. Neuroimage, 20, 889-897. Ogawa, K, and Inui T. (2007). Lateralization of the posterior parietal cortex for internal monitoring of self- versus externally generated movements. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1827-1835. Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113. Olin, R. (1999). Auditory hallucinations and the bicameral mind. Lancet, 354, 166. Pelli, D. G. (1997). The video toolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies.Spatial Vision, 10, 437-442. Plated, S. M., and Gallup Jr., G. G. (2002). Self-face recognition is affected by schizotypal personality traits. Schizophrenia Research, 57, 81-85. Raine, A. (1991). The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17, 555-564.

142

Tomohisa Asai, Eriko Sugimori and Yoshihiko Tanno

Raine, A., and Benishay, D. (1995). The SPQ-B: A brief screening instrument for schizotypal personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorder, 9, 346-355. Raine, A., and Lencz, T. (1995). Conceptual and theoretical issues in schizotypal personality research. In A. Raine, T. Lencz, and A. Mednick (Eds.), Schizotypal personality (pp. 315). New York: Cambridge University Press. Reilly, J.L., Murphy, P.T., Byrne, M., Larkin, C., Gill, M., O'Callaghan, E., and Lane, A. (2001). Dermatoglyphic fluctuating asymmetry and atypical handedness in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 50, 159-168. Rosenzweig, M. R. (1951). Representations of the two ears at the auditory cortex. AmericanJournal of Physiology, 167, 147-214. Sato, A., and Yasuda, A. (2005). Illusion of self-agency: Discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensory consequences of actions modulates the sense of self-agency, but not the sense of self-ownership. Cognition, 94, 241-255. Schneider, K. (1959). Clinical psychopathology. New York: Grune and Stratton. Sher, L. (2000). Neuroimaging, auditory hallucinations, and the bicameral mind. Journal ofPsychiatry Neuroscience, 25, 239-240. Sher, L. (2000). Neuroimaging, auditory hallucinations, and the bicameral mind. Journal ofPsychiatry Neuroscience, 25, 239-240. Shuster, L.I. and Durrant, J. D. (2003). Toward a better understanding of the perception of self-produced speech. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 1-11. Somers, M., Sommer, I. E., Boks, M. P., and Kahn, R. S. (2009). Hand-preference and population schizotypy: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 108, 25-32. Sommer, I., Ramsay, N., Kahn, R., Aleman, A., and Bouma, A. (2001). Handedness, language lateralisation and anatomical asymmetry in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 344-351. Sommer, I.E., Aleman, A., and Kahn, R.S. (2003). Left with the voices or hearing right? Lateralization of auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatry Neuroscience, 28, 217-218. Sommer, I.E., Aleman, A., and Kahn, R.S. (2003). Left with the voices or hearing right? Lateralization of auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatry Neuroscience, 28, 217-218. Sommer, I.E.C., Diederen, K.M.J., Blom, J-D., Willems, A., Kushan, L., Slotema, K., Boks, M.P.M., Daalman, K., Hoek, H.W., Neggers, S.F.W., and Kahn, R.S. (2008). Auditory Verbal Hallucinations Predominantely Activate the Right Inferior Frontal Area. Brain, 131, 3169-3177. Sommer, I.E.C., Ramsey, N.F., and Kahn, R.S. (2001). Lateralization in schizophrenia: an fMRI study. Schizophrenia Research,52, 57-67. Sommer, I.E.C., Slotema C.W., de Weijer A.D., Blom J., Daalman K., Neggers S., Somers M., Hoek H., Aleman A., and Kahn R. (2007). Can fMRI-guidance Improve the Efficacy of rTMS Treatment for Auditory Verbal Hallucinations? Schizophrenia Research, 93, 406-408. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., and Lushene, R. E. (1970). In cursive: STAI. Manual for the state-trait anxietyinventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist. Sugimori, E., Asai, T., and Yoshihiko, T. (2009). Reliability and validity of the Auditory Hallucination-like Experience Scale. The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 80, 389-396. (in Japanese).

Schizotypal Personality Traits

143

Tabarés-Seisdedos, R., Salazar-Fraile, J., Selva-Vera, G., Balanzá-Martinez, V., BallesterSánchez, F., Cózar-Santiago, R., Leal-Cercós, C., and Gómez-Beneyto, M. (2003). Abnormal motor asymmetry only during bimanual movement in schizophrenic patients compared with healthy subjects. Schizophrenia Research, 61, 245-253. Toyomura, A., Koyama, S., Miyamaoto, T., Terao, A., Omori, T., Murohashi, H. and Kuriki, S. (2007). Neural correlates of auditory feedback control in human. Neuroscience, 146, 499-503. Waters, F. A. V. , Badcock, J. C., and Maybery, M .T. (2003). Revision of the factor structure of the Launay –Slade Hallucination Scale LSHS-R. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1351-1357. Woodruff, P.W.R., Wright I.C., Bullmore E.T., Brammer M., Howard R.J., Williams S.C.R., Shapleske J., Rossell S., David A.S., McGuire P.K., Murray R.M. (1997). Auditory Hallucinations and the Temporal Cortical Response to Speech in Schizophrenia: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1676-1682. Zung, W. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 63-70.

Chapter 1

Patients with AH make external misattributions of the source ... The exclusive license for this PDF is limited to personal website use only. No part of this digital ...... (2001). Verbal self-monitoring and auditory verbal hallucinations in patients with.

487KB Sizes 2 Downloads 494 Views

Recommend Documents

chapter p chapter 1
Write the product in standard form. 5) (3 + 5i)(2 + 9i). 5). Find the product of the complex number and its conjugate. 6) -1 - 5i. 6). CHAPTER 1. Find the domain of ...

Chapter 1
converged to the highest peak because the selective pressure focuses attention to the area of .... thus allowing the formation of non-equal hyper-volume niches. In order to ..... The crossover operator exchanges the architecture of two ANNs in.

Chapter 1
strategy entails, the research findings are difficult to compare. .... rooms (cf. Li 1984; Wu 2001; Yu 2001). Comprehensive Surveys of EFL Learner Behaviours.

Chapter 1
increasing timeliness, and increasing precision [265]. Example: New data enable new analyses ..... but they allow researchers to take on problems of great scale and complexity. Furthermore, they are developing at ..... For MySQL, Chapter 4 provides i

Chapter 1
Digital System Test and Testable Design: Using HDL Models and Architectures ... What it is that we are testing in digital system test and why we are testing it? ..... mainframe cabinet containing the power distribution unit, heat exchanger for liquid

Chapter 1
Shall I send for the doctor?" The last thing he needed was a dose of the site doctor's hippopotamus-blood-and-cat- hair paste. He clambered quickly to his feet. Reonet peered at him through her fringe and pretended to continue with her work. The rest

Chapter 1
The expression x2 is greater than 2x for x 5 3. Through trial and error you can find that 3 is the only value of x where x2 is greater than 2x because 32 5 3 p 3 5 9 and. 23 5 2 p 2 p 2 5 8. Problem Solving. 48. 4s 5 4(7.5) 5 30. The perimeter is 30

Chapter 1
Impact of Bullying on Hospital, GP and Child Psychiatric Health Services. Long-term ... Appendix for Parents – Is My Child Being Bullied? Fears ..... per cent of lone mothers living alone had major problems in the area of social contact. ... Childr

Chapter 1
The Continuum Publishing Group Ltd 2003, The Tower Building, 11 York Road, London SE1 7NX ... trying to find a link between this information and its insight into peda- ..... reports on Chinese EFL learners' strategies for oral communication.

CCG Chapter 1 TV
CPM Educational Program. Lesson 1.3.2A Resource Page. Shapes Toolkit. Equilateral. Triangle: Isosceles. Triangle: Scalene. Triangle: Scalene Right. Triangle: Isosceles. Right. Triangle: Square: Rectangle: A quadrilateral with four right angles. Paral

Chapter 1 -
The Challenges and Opportunities. Of Marketing in Today's Economy. • Power Shift to Customers. • Massive Increase in Product Selection. • Audience and ...

Chapter 1
This is known as producing an ARCHIVE of the data. (Schools normally archive data for 7 years before destroying it). Note: Archived data is NOT used for retrieving the file if something goes wrong, it is used for storing little used or redundant data

Chapter 1 - FAO
schedule, which changed from odd- to even-numbered years in 2010, to align with the new FAO. Conference schedule. ... globalization. Many countries were decentralizing the responsibility for forest planning and management while facing the impacts of

Chapter 1 - GitHub
Jan 23, 2017 - 1. What are all these things? 2. What is the mean of yi? 3. What is the distribution of ϵi? 4. What is the notation X or Y ? Drawing a sample yi = xi β + ϵi. Write code which draws a sample form the population given by this model. p

Chapter 1
in improving learner proficiency, accounting for 63.4% of the variance. Although Liu ..... strategies used by Masters' degree level English and non-English majors.

Chapter 1 -
The electrical costs of running a roller coaster ..... at a General Electric ...... $160,000 and 65% of the tellers' time is spent processing deposits and withdrawals:.

chapter 1
Engineering Program coordinator and a member of his committee, Prof. ... Carolina State University for evaluating this dissertation as his external examiner. ...... a vehicle traveling between O-D pairs is geometrically distributed. ...... estimates

Chapter 1 -
Create a Student Details Application using the ADO Data control and the Insert ... The data in this application is accessed using the ActiveX Data Objects and the ...

Chapter 1.pdf
6 Unit A Cells and Systems NEL. Cell Theory. Cells are the basic unit of all living things. By looking closely at living. things over the centuries, scientists have gathered a great deal of. evidence to support what they call the There are two main.

Chapter 1 Introduction
This dissertation is mainly concerned with offline routing control in diverse IP networks. The notion "diverse. IP networks" refers to networks with different routing technologies such as the classical. Interior Gateway Protocols (IGPs)3, Multi-Proto

chapter 1.pdf
Operating system monitors the overall activity of the computer and provides ... means they store all information (numbers, text, ... to what we'll do in this course.

Chapter 1: Introduction
Computer-System Architecture. ▫ Operating-System Structure. ▫ Operating-System ... systems, video games. ○ Users. > People, machines, other computers ...

Unit 1 Chapter 1.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. Unit 1 Chapter ...

chapter 1 B.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. chapter 1 B.pdf.