Distinct forms of implicit learning that respond differentially to performance errors and sensory prediction errors Yohsuke R. Miyamoto, Shengxin Wang, Andrew E. Brennan, Maurice A. Smith Recent studies have investigated the contribution of an explicit strategy alongside implicit learning during visuomotor adaptation (e.g. Mazzoni & Krakauer 2006, Taylor & Ivry 2010, Taylor et al 2014). A prominent idea is that strategy learning is driven by performance error (the discrepancy between desired and actual motion), whereas implicit learning is driven by sensory-prediction error (the discrepancy between actual motion and an internal model prediction). The key consequence of this idea is that implicit learning would proceed in a manner independent of strategy, because sensory prediction errors should be unaffected by strategy. The current study challenges this idea. Here, we decomposed implicit and explicit (strategic) adaptation into temporally-stable (TS) and temporally-labile (TL) parts, and found that strategy is purely TS while implicit learning has distinct TS and TL components. Surprisingly, we found that while TS implicit learning may be driven by sensory-prediction error, TL implicit learning is instead driven by performance error. Subjects adapted short (9cm), rapid (~300ms), point-to-point reaching arm movements to a ±30° visuomotor rotation (VMR). The task required subjects to select an aiming direction strategy for each movement before initiating it, by adjusting the position of an aiming marker using a keypad. Having trial-by-trial information about the aiming direction allowed us to decompose the learning on each trial into separate strategic and implicit components (Fig. 1a). Following a 200 trial baseline period, a ±30° VMR was introduced (balanced across subjects). After 150 and 300 training trials in a single target direction, we tested subjects’ aftereffects in blocks of trials with no visual feedback (Fig. 1b). Because of rest breaks of 1 min+ before these testing blocks, these blocks probed the TS components of adaptation ( decay~16.5s for labile VMR adaptation; Hadjiosif & Smith 2013). Consistent with Taylor et al. 2014, rapid strategic adjustments allowed mean performance to quickly achieve near-zero error during training (Fig. 2a). Mean implicit learning increased more gradually, however, and after its quick initial rise, mean strategic learning gradually decreased in a complementary fashion. Despite a match between the mean adaptation patterns we observed and those previously reported, we discovered a remarkable inter-individual diversity in the level of strategy employed. This diversity in strategy had little effect on performance because strategy and implicit learning were largely complementary and thus strongly anti-correlated across subjects (r=-0.91), so as to maintain a nearly fixed level of combined (strategy+implicit) learning as shown in Fig. 3j. Inter-individual variability in strategy (s.d.=9.6°) thus dwarfed the variability in combined learning (s.d.=1.5°), as shown in Fig. 2b-c. At asymptote, about 50% of subjects displayed < 5° of aiming strategy, whereas 20% displayed > 20°. To explore this considerable diversity, we divided individuals into low, mid, and high-strategy subgroups (Fig. 2d) based on their asymptotic strategy levels. We then compared the aftereffects of strategic and implicit learning, after 1 minute rest breaks that would remove TL adaptation, for the low, mid, and high-strategy subgroups. Strategy displayed TS aftereffects that closely matched the overall (TS+TL) level of strategy during late training (Fig. 3a,b), indicating that strategic learning is TS over 1-5 minutes (the onset and offset latencies for the testing blocks) with little contribution from a TL component (Fig. 3c). In contrast, TS implicit learning was largely dissociated from the overall (TS+TL) implicit learning observed during the training period across subjects (Fig. 3d,e, bar graphs in Fig. 3k, l), indicating its distinct TS and TL components. Critically, TS implicit learning in high-strategy subjects was consistently larger than their overall (TL+TS) implicit learning measured during training (p < 0.01, Fig. 3e), revealing a surprisingly negative TL component (p < 0.01, Fig. 3f). But why would TL learning be consistently negative in high-strategy subjects in the presence of a perturbation that should drive positive learning? We suggest that this could be the case if the TL component was reacting to overactive TS learning. Indeed, for high-strategy subjects, we found that combined (strategy+implicit) learning during the testing blocks overshot the ideal level (p = 0.01, Fig. 3g). Correspondingly, TS combined learning was greater than overall (TL+TS) combined (strategy+implicit) learning during late training (p < 0.01, Fig. 3h) due to negative TL combined learning (p < 0.01, Fig. 3i) arising from the negative TL implicit learning observed in 3f. These findings suggest that negative TL implicit learning acts to compensate for overactive TS combined learning to prevent overcompensatory performance errors. This would, however, require TL implicit learning to respond to performance errors, and would result in a strong relationship between TL implicit learning and the highly diverse strategy levels that underlie the differences in TS combined (strategy+implicit) learning shown in Fig 3g. Correspondingly, we find clear evidence for an exceptionally strong relationship between TL implicit learning and strategy level (Fig 3k), despite little relationship between TS implicit learning and strategy (Fig 3l), suggesting that the latter may be driven by sensory-prediction error. In summary, dissection of learning into temporally-stable and temporally-labile components revealed that labile implicit learning (Fig. 3f, k) was crucial for maintaining task performance by bolstering stable learning in low-strategy individuals and counteracting it in high-strategy individuals. Of note, is that Mazzoni & Krakauer 2006 and Taylor & Ivry 2010 likely missed these effects because their 8-target paradigm ensured large intertrial intervals, leading to a decay of labile learning that would allow overactive stable learning to proceed unchecked.

Fig. 1: Experimental paradigm

Fig. 1: (a) Prior to each movement, subjects indicated their strategy by positioning the aiming marker used to guide their aiming. Hand direction relative to the aiming marker defines implicit learning. (b) Subjects experienced a baseline period with no rotation, and training periods in which the cursor movement was rotated by ±30°. Gray regions (NVF test) indicate blocks of trials with no visual feedback, in which we tested for subjects’ aftereffects from training after a rest break. Fig. 2: (a, b) Mean learning curves for implicit learning (red), strategic learning (blue), and overall learning (green). Errorbars in (b) indicate standard deviations across subjects. (c) Distributions of implicit learning, strategy, and overall learning at asymptote are represented also by gaussian fits. (d) We separated subjects into 3 subgroups based on the level of strategy at asymptote. Light blue represents the low-strategy subgroup mean, darker blue shades represent the mid and high-strategy subgroups. Fig. 3: (a, d, g) Comparison of learning in the training direction between overall learning (stable + labile) measured during training vs stable learning measured during the testing blocks. a, e, d show strategic, implicit, and combined (strategy+implicit) learning, respectively, for the 3 strategy groups introduced in Fig. 2d. Error bars show 2 SEM. (b, e, h), Temporally-stable learning measured during testing blocks is plotted against overall learning, which consists of both labile and stable components, measured during asymptotic training. (b) plots temporally-stable strategy, (e) plots temporally-stable implicit, and (h) plots temporally-stable combined (strategy+implicit) learning. Each dot is an individual subject, colored by strategy group. Stars indicate group means. (c, f, i) Like Fig. b, e, h, but plotting temporally-labile learning (measured by temporal decay from training to aftereffects in the testing block), rather than stable learning. (j, k, l) Relationships between (j) overall implicit learning (stable + labile), (k) labile implicit learning, or (l) stable implicit learning vs strategy at asymptote. Red bars at right indicate size of labile and stable components for each strategy group. Asterisks indicate significant differences from zero at p<0.01.

NVF test

140 254

404 518

Trial

Mean learning 30

Implicit Strategy

No visual feedback testing 0

c

140

Trial

254

404

Asymptotic learning distributions

30 Subjects

Combined learning Strategy

0

0

Implicit

20 Learning (deg)

{ { {

40

j

d

Low / mid / high strategy subgroups 30 No visual feedback testing

0 0

140

Trial

High-strategy (n = 9) Mid-strategy(n = 13) Low-strategy (n = 26) 254 404

30

High-strategy

Overall

30

Overall

Stable

b

Mid-strategy Overshoots ideal level

High-strategy (HS)

0

High-strategy (HS)

Low-strategy

Mid-strategy

Low-strategy

0

Combined Learning

g

Low-strategy

Mid-strategy

0

Stable

Overall

e

Stable

h 30

y=

0 0

y=

x

y

r = 0.931 p < 0.001

-30

c

f

=x

0

Stable greater than overall implicit for HS group

i

Negative labile learning for HS group

30

x

Stable greater than overall learning for HS group Negative labile learning for HS group

0 0

0 -30

0 30 Overall strategy during training (deg)

Structure of asymptotic learning 30

0

Implicit

d

Ideal Performance

30

ng

0

Stable (deg)

30

Labile (deg)

Learning (deg)

Spread of learning curves (s.d.)

Strategy

a

ni

0

Combined learning

No visual feedback testing

n = 48

b

Strategy (deg)

Fig. 3: Generalization function data reveal overactive temporally stable learning and negative temporally labile learning in high strategy subjects

Overall implicit (deg)

a

Learning (deg)

Fig. 2: Strategy learning shows large inter-individual diversity but combined learning does not

ar

0 -324 -124 -10

NVF test

le

NVF test

p < 0.001

0 30 Strategy (deg)

k

0 30 Overall implicit learning during training (deg)

Labile implicit dependence on strategy 20 ** **

0 ** p < 0.001

0 30 Labile Strategy (deg) implicit

l Stable implicit (deg)

30

Labile implicit (deg)

Perturbation schedule

ne d

b

Cursor rotation (deg)

Strategy + Implicit = Combined learning

bi

Start location

m

Aiming Marker

Strategy Co I mp mb licit ( rel Handined ati d lea ve ire rn to cti ing tar on ge t)

Cursor Target

Co

Strategy vs implicit decomposition

Learning (deg)

a

0 30 Overall combined learning during training (deg)

Stable implicit dependence on strategy 20

**** **

0

0 30 Stable Strategy (deg) implicit

Distinct forms of implicit learning that respond differentially to ...

the variability in combined learning (s.d.=1.5°), as shown in Fig. 2b-c. At asymptote, about 50% of subjects displayed <. 5° of aiming strategy, whereas 20% ...

335KB Sizes 1 Downloads 244 Views

Recommend Documents

Distinct Hematopoietic Stem Cell Subtypes Are Differentially ...
Mar 4, 2010 - dilutions of whole bone marrow—myeloid-biased (My-bi), lym- phoid-biased ...... files were generated with GCOS 1.0 software (Affymetrix).

Implicit learning as a mechanism of language change
much of the learning that humans do is unintentional and implicit, that is, outside of ... found that implicit learning takes place in adult speakers and listeners.

Opportunities to Respond - Illinois RtI
Equity and Access for All Students: MTSS Conference 2014. Keeping Students Engaged: Practical Strategies for Increasing Feedback and Opportunities to ...

pdf-1446\forms-for-people-designing-forms-that-people ...
Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. pdf-1446\forms-for-people-designing-forms-that-people-can-use-from-robert-barnett-and-associates.pdf.

Learning in Implicit Generative Models
translation, or fine-grained spatio-temporal models tracking the spread of disease. Alternatively, we ... and ecology, since the mechanistic understanding of such systems can be used to directly create a data simulator ... Without a likelihood functi

Structural Priming as Implicit Learning: A Comparison ... - Springer Link
One hypothesis is that it is a short-term memory or activation effect. If so, structural priming .... call a priming score. For instance, dative priming .... Paper presented at the Twelfth Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference,. New York, NY.

Uniting to Respond to Hate Speech & Violence_Speakers Bio.pdf
STAND UP! Uniting to Respond to Hate Speech & Violence_Speakers Bio.pdf. STAND UP! Uniting to Respond to Hate Speech & Violence_Speakers Bio.pdf.

Prepare, respond, recover
Rio de Janeiro is a vibrant city known for hosting large festivals and international events that ... Page 10 ... The general public can also pull up web-based maps to locate hospitals ... There's a reason people say, “Hope for the best. Prepare for

Arbuscular mycorrhizae respond to plants exposed to ... - CiteSeerX
length was concentrated in the top soil layer. The increased presence of .... g m. −2. ; Grace-Sierra Horticulture Product Company). ... was run for several years partly to ensure recovery ... and no mycorrhizal fungi (data not shown), it was ex-.

Implicit Theories 1 Running Head: IMPLICIT THEORIES ...
self, such as one's own intelligence (Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan, 1999) and abilities (Butler,. 2000), to those more external and beyond the self, such as other people's .... the self. The scale had a mean of 4.97 (SD = 0.87) and an internal relia

Distinct subcellular targeting of fluorescent ... - Semantic Scholar
Electrophysiology. Agonist-evoked currents were monitored at room temperature in the .... (data not shown and Maricq et al., 1991; Eisele et al., 1993), as was the ..... Kittler, J.F., Wang, J., Connolly, C.N., Vicini, S., Smart, T.G. & Moss, S.J..

Implicit Interaction
interaction is to be incorporated into mainstream software development, ... of adaptive user interfaces on resource-constrained mobile computing devices.

Field performance of differentially aged seeds using ...
hydration-dehydration either with water or using blackgram seed leachate (1:1) ... through this treatment and this was reflected on enhancement of plant height, ...

Astrocytes respond to hypoxia by increasing glycolytic ...
2Department of Anatomy and Cell Biology, College of Medicine, University of Saskatchewan,. Saskatoon, SK ..... FL: Verlag Chemie. p 126–133. Walz W. 1997.

The Variability of Organs Differentially Involved in ...
respectively. Standard and keel petal size varied in parallel, not only in conspecific individuals but also across species .... massively in winter or early spring.

Physiological Condition Differentially Affects the ...
Mar 30, 2010 - Thermal data loggers (resolution ... physiological, behavioral, and environmental data were log10 transformed to reduce ...... cessful recovery of the physiological status of coho salmon on board a ... Elsevier, San Diego, CA.

Physiological Condition Differentially Affects the ...
Mar 30, 2010 - solely on endogenous energy reserves, as sockeye salmon cease .... of clean river water passing through the individual's mouth and gills.

Physiological Condition Differentially Affects the ...
Mar 30, 2010 - physiological stress, are reproductively advanced, are ill pre- .... Wireless) were used with three-element or four-element Yagi ...... technology.

UWB cross-polarization discrimination with differentially fed ...
UWB cross-polarization discrimination with differentially fed, mirrored antenna elements.pdf. UWB cross-polarization discrimination with differentially fed, ...

Distinct Ex Vivo Susceptibility of B-Cell Subsets to ... - Journal of Virology
Dec 11, 2007 - enabling a detailed susceptibility analysis. Importantly, B-cell ..... 3.1 image analysis software (SynGene). ATP uptake assay ... Statistical analyses. ... Statistical analyses of significance ...... Tangye, S. G., and K. L. Good. 200