C 2005) International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 26, No. 4, August 2005 ( DOI: 10.1007/s10764-005-5308-4

Dominance Turnover Between an Alpha and a Beta Male and Dynamics of Social Relationships in Japanese Macaques Nobuyuki Kutsukake1,2,3 and Toshikazu Hasegawa1 Received March 15, 2004; revision June 15, 2004; 2nd revision July 30, 2004; accepted August 4, 2004

We report a case of turnover between an alpha (GN) and a beta male (R7) and its effects in a troop of provisioned Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata fuscata) in Shiga-Heights, Nagano Prefecture, Japan. The aggression between the 2 males was caused by the intrusion of GN towards the consort of R7. R7 received support from his brother and mother, and consequently defeated GN. After the turnover, R7 attacked GN frequently, which may have functioned to stabilize the dominance relationship between them. Also, R7 selectively attacked females friendly to GN soon after the turnover. Although we never observed polyadic aggression among males during the stable dominance period, 20 cases of polyadic aggression occurred among the 6 highestranked males in the 2 days following the turnover, and one case occurred on the fourth day. R7 and GN formed stable conservative alliances for attacking subordinate males. Males that did not participate in the turnover began to form revolutionary coalitions to attack higher-ranking males, but they were thwarted by the conservative coalitions between the dominants. Mutualism was a plausible explanation for the patterns of coalition formation because most of them were conservative with little associated cost. Seven females had a high proximity index (C-score) to GN before the turnover, but a significantly lower proximity index after the turnover. On the day of the 1 Department

of Cognitive and Behavioral Science, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, The University of Tokyo. 2 Department of Biological Science, Graduate School of Science, The University of Tokyo. 3 JSPS Research Fellow. 4 To whom correspondence should be addressed at 7-3-1 Hongo, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo, Japan 113–0033; e-mail: [email protected]. 775 C 2005 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 0164-0291/05/0800-0775/0 

776

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

turnover, 6 non-lactating females suddenly became receptive, suggesting that the turnover induced immediate receptivity in the females. KEY WORDS: Japanese macaques; turnover between the alpha and beta male; coalitions and alliances; dominance struggle; friendships; receptivity.

INTRODUCTION Many primate species living in a stable social group have a dominance relationship among group members. Occasionally, the dominance relationship changes as a result of severe aggression within the group (Ehardt and Bernstein, 1986; Gygax et al., 1997). In such cases, the aggressive turnover of a top-ranked individual, usually an alpha male, by members of the same group may be the most drastic social event in the society, because the topranked individual has a strong influence restricting subordinate behavior, such as access to resources (Keddy-Hector and Raleigh, 1992; Pusey & Packer, 1997; Silk, 1987; Walter and Seyfarth, 1987). However, only a few records detail agonistic turnovers between a top-ranked male and group member(s) in a stable group wherein the membership was not artificially disturbed, either in the wild or in captivity (Itoigawa, 1993; Mizuhara, 1964; Nakamichi et al., 1995; Nishida, 1983; Perry, 1998a,b; Sprague et al., 1996; de Waal, 1982, 1986; Witt et al., 1981) as a result of their unpredictability and the difficulty in observation. In addition, it seems likely that the reports on dominance turnover have included a substantial bias in the data because reporters in anecdotal studies did not use systematic observation methods. During a research project on the social behavior of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata fuscata; Kutsukake, 2000; Kutsukake and Castles, 2001), we observed the whole process of agonistic turnover between the alpha male (GN) and beta male (R7). We describe how the turnover occurred and via systematic observations and data collected for other studies, we report on 3 aspects of the social dynamics after change of the alpha male. First, we report the strategy used by a male to maintain his dominance or challenge higher-ranking males. We not only focused on the interaction between the alpha and beta males whose dominance position changed but also examined the dominance struggles among other males resulting from coalition formation, (i.e., coordinated aggression towards conspecifics). Coalitions represent cooperative behavior, and their occurrence has been confirmed in various primates (Harcourt and de Waal, 1992; Chapais, 1995; Cords, 1997). Coalitions may incur a substantial cost to the participants (Dunbar, 1988), and are termed alliances when they last for a long time and remain stable (Harcourt and de Waal, 1992). Males sometimes struggle for dominance position via coalitions during or following a dominance rank change

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

777

(Itoigawa, 1993; Mizuhara, 1964; Nakamichi et al., 1995; Perry, 1998a,b; Witt et al., 1981; de Waal, 1982; 1986; Nishida, 1983; Nishida and Hosaka, 1996). No previous researcher has systematically analyzed male-male coalitions in Japanese macaques because, in general, fewer cases of cooperative bonds have been confirmed among male versus female primates (van Hooff and van Schaik, 1992, 1994), and because male Japanese macaques rarely form coalitions in the context of intra-group actions (Koyama, 1967; Watanabe, 1979; Grewal, 1980; Takahata, 1982). In our study group all 6 high-ranking males were from the lineage of the alpha female, which may be uncommon in male-dispersing species like macaques. Natal males from the alpha female lineage can postpone emigration to gain alpha-status in provisioned groups (Chapais, 1983), which provides an opportunity to examine the strategies of coalition formation, partner choice, and the influences of kinship in coalition formation among male Japanese macaques. Also, previous studies showed special role of the alpha female in macaque society; she often influences dominance among males (Gouzoules, 1980; Chapais, 1983; Chapais and Lecomte, 1995; Nakamichi et al., 1995; Raleigh and McGuire, 1989). For example, Chapais (1983) observed a provisioned free-ranging group of rhesus macaques and reported that sons of an alpha female occupied high-ranking positions with her support, a very similar process to matrilineal rank inheritance among females. As the situation in our study group is quite similar to that of Chapais (1983), we paid particular attention to the role of the alpha female during the dominance instability period. Second, we investigated changes in heterosexual friendly relationships on turnover of the alpha male. Various studies of primates living in multimale-multifemale societies have confirmed special, affiliative relationships between adult males and unrelated, anestrous females for nonsexual purposes (Smuts, 1999). The male-female relationships—friendships (Smuts, 1999)—are typically characterized by a high frequency of proximity, frequent grooming, and agonistic support. Given that a relationship can be described as investments that benefit the participants (Kummer, 1978), it is predictable that relationship quality changes when the dominance rank of the friendship partner changes. However, only a few researchers have investigated the changes in male-female friendship with a turnover. Nakamichi et al. (1995) observed turnover between an old alpha-male and a beta-male in Japanese macaques and found that the number of adult females in proximity to the new alpha male increased after the male gained the alpha position (Raleigh and McGuire, 1989: vervet monkeys Cercopithecus aethiops). Perry (1997, 1998b) observed replacement of the alpha male in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys Cebus capucinus and reported that the proximity scores of adult females and the previous alpha male decreased after his rank declined.

778

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

Third, we report a sudden change in receptivity of nonlactating females soon after the turnover. Previous studies showed that female primates do not always cycle regularly, and various social factors influence facultative female receptivity. For example, exposure to a novel male facilitates the start of female receptivity in macaques (Wilson and Gordon, 1979; Okayasu et al., 1992). Female receptivity also can change after takeover of the group in hamadryas baboons (Colmenares and Gomendio, 1988; Swedell, 2000; Zinner and Deschner, 2000). Our case is unique, because the intra group change in male-dominance rank induced changes in female receptivity. METHODS Study Troop, Troop Composition, and Background We examined Shiga A-1, a free-ranging troop of Japanese macaques in Shiga Heights, Nagano Prefecture, Japan. Demographic records—births, deaths, immigrations, and emigrations—dating to 1962 are available and all individuals are identifiable. The staff of the Jigokudani Monkey Park provisions the macaques 3–4 times daily and tourists are prohibited from feeding them. Japanese macaques have a clear mating and nonmating seasons. The mating season is, ca. from October to February at Shiga Heights, but most cases of mating occurred from October through December. The turnover occurred on 9 November 1998, during the mating season. The first author observed turnover process from a distance of 10 m, from which we could record the interactions in detail. Kutsukake has intermittently observed the troop since October 1997, and has studied dominance and aggression among group members (Kutsukake, 2000; Kutsukake and Castles, 2001). Basically, observations were from 08:00 to 17:00 h. The troop comprised ca. 230 individuals, including 22 adult (>6 yr) males and 77 adult (>5 yr) females. The group contained 5 kin groups (T, N, K, R and M); among which, all the kin groups except M stemmed from one matriline (Kutsukake, 2000). All of the females are basically named according to their kin group, e.g., MB is from kin group M. Because kin group T was a large kin group containing >40 adult females, we also used family names to indicate the subunits within kin group T, e.g., the alpha female and her daughter are the T(g) group. The matrilineal rank inheritance rule (Kawamura, 1958; Chapais, 1992) predicts that kin group M ranked below the other 4 kin groups. Kutsukake (2000) investigated the dominance relationships among 69 adult females >6 yr old in 1998. Based on the average dominance ranks of all females in each kin group, the dominance relationship among kin groups was T > M > N > R

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

779

> K. In particular, 2 M females (MB and MR) were dominant over nearly half of the females in kin group T (Figure 2 in Kutsukake, 2000). Ten of the 22 adult males usually stayed in the center of the troop, and we determined the dominance order among them by observing retreatsupplant behaviors. GN, a healthy 22-yr-old, acquired the status of alpha male in December 1997, after the previous alpha male emigrated to a neighboring troop. He was the brother of the alpha female (TG: 24 yr). A beta male R7 (11 yr) was a son of TG and thus a nephew of GN. Seven highestranking males belonged to the matriline of TG. (Fig. 1). The stable dominance order among 7 high-ranking males before the turnover was GN > R7 > R4 (14 yr) > ZK (6 yr) > N (9 yr) > M (10 yr) > IN (7 yr). For simplicity we used male names that do not begin with T, though they were from kin group T(g). Males ranking below the seventh rank were from 2 kin groups: T [though not from the T(g) family] and middle-ranking N. Several females unrelated to GN were frequently in proximity to him in the period since we started observations in October 1997. M kin-group members, in particular were most often in proximity to GN. The fact that kin group M remained near GN, had frequent grooming interactions with him, and received his support during aggressive interactions, probably explains why 2 females in M dominated the females expected to become

Fig. 1. Genealogy among the 6 highest-ranked males and the alpha female (TG). The numbers written below correspond to the year of birth, and the numbers above each male indicate his dominance rank before and after the turnover.

780

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

dominant based on matrilineal rank inheritance rules (Kawamura, 1958; Chapais, 1992; Chapais and Lecomte, 1995). According to Eishi Tokida (personal comm.) director of Jigokudani Monkey Park, the relationships were established before the start of our observations. GN emigrated from the troop in September 1999, 10 mo after the turnover. However, it is unknown whether his emigration was related to the turnover Observations in May 2000 confirmed that GN was solitary.

Observation Methods Kutsukake and Castles (2001) observed most of the group members daily and opportunistically recorded all aggressive interactions, which revealed the general characteristics of intra-group aggression. Following the turnover, we used focal animal sampling (Altmann, 1974) to observe the previous alpha male (GN) for 10 days (total observation: 72 h). Simultaneously, we conducted all occurrence sampling for male-male interactions among the 6 highest-ranked males (Altmann, 1974). Kutsukake stood where he could look around the whole area of the feeding site and observe their positions and behaviors. Whenever the distance among >2 of the 6 highest-ranked males became <10 m, we paid special attention to them. In general, it is impossible to conduct focal observations and all-occurrence sampling observations simultaneously. However, males usually stayed in an open area, where visibility was quite good, were inactive and did not change their position frequently, and were immobile during the mating season. Male dyads, except R7 and GN, kept their distance from each other and rarely associated. As a result of the favorable observation conditions, Kutsukake was able to conduct both focal and all occurrence sampling simultaneously and to observe ≥4 males for > 98% of the observation time. Therefore, it is highly likely that we observed all polyadic interactions among them while they were at the feeding site. However, we were unable to observe the macaques on the morning of the third day after the turnover because they remained in the mountains and did not come to the feeding site. Only one polyadic interaction involved IN, the seventh-ranked male (fig. 1), which received redirected aggression from the third-ranked male, R4. Other males did not participate in the polyadic interactions among the 6 highest-ranked males. This indicates that not all high-ranking adult males from T(g) participated in the dominance competition. We checked dominance relationships among males via the sampling ad libitum (Altmann, 1974) by daily observations of supplant-retreat behaviors. The dominance relationship among males did not change, except the turnover between GN and R7.

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

781

To investigate the friendly females for GN and R7, we used proximity as an index of friendship (Kurland, 1977; Cords, 1997). Kutsukake recorded adult females proximate (0–0.5 m, 0.5–1 m, and 1 − <5 m) to GN or R7 every hour for 3 observation periods: Preturnover (mating season): 19–25 October and 7–8 November 1998, resulting in 17 scans for GN and 15 scans for R7. Posturnover (mating season): 9–18 November 1998, resulting in 39 scans for GN and 40 scans for R7. Stable (non-mating season): 38 days from March–September 1999, when GN emigrated from the troop, resulting in 76 and 77 scans, respectively. For all 3 periods, we collected data only during periods of no artificial feeding. We include no datum from December to February in the analysis because heavy snow caused the macaques to concentrate by a hot spring to thermoregulate. As an index of affiliative relationships between females and males, we used a composite proximity score (C-score), which considered the proximity level of a female to a male (Smuts, 1999; Perry 1997, 1998b). The following equation derived the scores: C-score = (P0−0.5 m )/1 + (P0.5−1 m )/3 + (P1−5 m )/12 wherein PA−Bm is the proportion of proximity from A meter to B meter. Each weight figure corresponds to the proportion of the middle point of each range (0.25 for 0∼0.5 m, 0.75 for 0.5 ∼ 1 m, and 3 for 1 ∼ 5 m). To allow the scores to range between 0 (never ≤ 5 m) and 1 (always ≤ 0.5 m), we multiplied the proportions by the arbitrary value 4, and used 1, 3, and 12 as weighted values. For preturnover period data, we calculated an average C-score for each female in proximity to GN and R7 at least once. The number of scans is small because of the sudden, unpredictable occurrence of the turnover and the restricted data collection due to the heavy snow. However, we believe that the estimate of the proximate relationship is reliable because the distribution of the C score is not continuous and most of the females had extremely low C scores (fig. 2c, f), while the C scores of several unrelated females are higher than those of other females (fig. 2a, b, d). We defined those females as affiliative females of GN or R7. Jigokudani Monkey Park staff and Kutsukake recorded reproductive states of adult females during the mating season. We determined receptivity by the criteria of copulation and the presence of a seminal plug. Definitions Aggression is indicated by facial threat, threat vocalization, confrontation while staring at each other, chasing, lunging, grasping, and biting. If

782

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

Fig. 2. Changes in the C-score of each female before the turnover, after the turnover, and during the dominance-stable period. (a) Females from kin group M that were friendly with GN; (b) 3 females from other kin groups; (c) 3 females from the T(g) family group and other females (N = 21) that were in proximity to GN at least once during the pre-turnover, postturnover or stable period; (d) females from the T(e) family group that were friendly with R7; (e) 3 females that increased their proximity to R7 after the turnover; (f) females (N = 17) that were in proximity to R7 at least once during the pre-turnover, post-turnover or stable period.

two aggressive interactions were separated by >1 min, they were counted as separate interactions. Support is the intervention of a male towards one of a pair of males in conflict. A coalition is any interaction in which ≥2 males jointly attack ≥1 male. Coalitions were classified as revolutionary, conservative, or bridging coalitions (Chapais, 1995). Revolutionary coalitions form between subordinates against a dominant, (e.g., alpha vs. beta and gamma). Conservative coalitions are between dominants against a subordinate, e.g., alpha and beta vs. gamma. Bridging coalitions are between

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

783

2 individuals against a middle-ranked individual, e.g., alpha and gamma vs. beta). Japanese macaques sometimes solicit aid from other males by repeatedly turning the head quickly from an opponent to the coalition partner. Therefore, we also recorded the occurrence of the head flag display and its target. Data Analysis First, we present the characteristics of aggression between adults during the period of stable dominance. The data are from Kutsukake and Castles (2001), who observed 543 cases of aggression during ca. 630 h of observation between June 1998 and March 2000. (aggressive interactions observed during the period of the present study were not included in the dataset). We also deleted aggression data that involved juveniles in order to present a general picture of aggression between adults in the troop. We paid particular attention to the frequency of polyadic aggression. With regard to the behavior of males following the turnover, we investigated the number of polyadic interactions and the number of supports, coalitions formed, and solicits-for-aid in dyad levels as follows. If A supported B, which had confronted C in one polyadic interaction. We recorded that A had supported B once. When the direction and the initiation of the support was unclear, we excluded the data from the analysis. We counted the number of coalitions formed in each dyad by ignoring the direction and initiation of agonistic support. For instance, we counted one coalition in an A-B dyad. When 3 males (A, B, C) simultaneously attacked male (D), we recorded that a coalition was formed in 3 dyads: A-B, B-C, and A-C. Coalition partners often changed in one polyadic interaction, in which case, we counted one coalition when it was formed once in a dyad. For example, if A and B cooperatively attacked C, but then A and C formed a coalition against D in the middle of the battle, we recorded one coalition in the A-B and A-C dyads, and support from A to C. To investigate the effect of kinship, rank differences, and age differences on the frequency of coalition formation, we conducted a matrix correlation test (Hemelrijk 1990), which is a revised version of the Mantel test (Schnell et al., 1985) that considers interindividual variation in behavior frequency. We conducted 5000 permutations. Since the paternity of the 6 highest-ranked adult males was unknown, we considered only maternal kinship in the analysis, i.e., r = 0.25 for brothers, r = 0.125 for uncle-nephew pairs, and r = 0.0625 for distant kin such as granduncle-grandnephew pairs (Chapais et al., 1997). One could argue that it is meaningless to test the relationship between maternal kinship and the frequency of coalition formation because all of the males were from T(g) and related. Chapais et al. (1997)

784

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

investigated the relatedness threshold for nepotism, e.g., agonistic support, for female Japanese macaques and found that nepotistic behavior drops beyond grandmother-granddaughter and sibling dyads, i.e., r = 0.25 (Kurland, 1977; Belisle and Chapais, 2001). Although the relatedness threshold for male Japanese macaques has not been investigated, it is reasonable to assume that males have a similar relatedness threshold of nepotism or a higher relatedness threshold, given that males interact less with closely-related individuals than females do, which validates the analysis of the relationship between maternal kinship and coalition-formation frequency because the maternal relatedness among the 6 highest-ranked males varied widely. Via the Wilcoxon signed-rank test, we compared the mean C-score of GN- and R7-affiliative females before and after the turnover. Because the baseline for male-female proximity differs between the mating and nonmating seasons, we did not compare pre-or post-turnover conditions with stable-period conditions. The significance level is p < 0.05. RESULTS Background We observed 407 dyadic and polyadic aggressive interactions and classified them by the combination of sexes between the opponents Table I. During the stable dominance period, dyadic aggression between adult males (n = 7) is far less than between adult male and adult female (n = 61) or between adult females (n = 302). After considering that Shiga A1 contained fewer adult males (n = 22) than adult females (n = 77), we found that the number of aggressive interactions in the 3 categories of Table I. Dyadic and polyadic aggression during the dominance stable period

Dyadic aggression Polyadic aggression

Note. M: male; F: female.

Sex combination

# cases

M vs. M M vs. F F vs. F M & M vs. M M & M vs. F M & F vs. M M & F vs. F F & F vs. M F & F vs. F 3F vs. F M & 2F vs. M

7 61 302 0 0 12 9 5 8 2 1

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

785

sex combination differed from the expected numbers (Chi-squared test; χ2(2) = 66.2, P < 0.0001), i.e., the number of female-female aggressions is significantly higher than the number of male-female and male-male aggressions (Fisher’s exact probability test, Ps < 0.001). Of 37 cases of polyadic aggressive interactions, none involved 3 males or male-male coalitions attacking adult females; the absolute number of dyadic and polyadic (male-male coalition vs. single male) aggressive interactions among males or male-male coalitions was quite low during the period of stable dominance. Before the turnover, 27 females were in proximity to GN at least once. Visual inspection of the C-score distribution suggests that 7 females that were not from T(g) clearly had higher C-scores than those of other females (Figure 2a, b, c), and they were the only females whose C-scores exceeded the mean (0.12) of the C-score distribution. Four (MB, MG, MR, and MK) were from M, while the other 3 females (TW, TM, and NM) were from other kin groups (Table II). This result coincides with and reinforces our previous observation that M females formed special affiliative relationships with GN (Kutsukake, 2000). NM was in receptivity during these periods and was guarded by GN. On the other hand, the 4 M females and TW and TM were not receptive. Before the turnover, 17 females were in proximity to R7 at least once. Visual inspection of the C-score distribution suggests that among them, 3 clearly had higher scores than the others, and they were the only females whose C-scores exceeded the mean (0.03) of the C-score distribution (Figure 2d,f). All were from T(e), which is a branch of T, and they were not receptive before the turnover. Table II. Identity and reproductive condition of the females Affiliative male GN

R7

Related females

Name MB MR MG MK TW TM NM Te5 Te1 Te3 TW (see above) KR TK TG Tg1 Tg3

Kin Age group 24 19 5 6 17 17 8 13 7 15 15 19 24 7 5

M M M M T T N T(e) T(e) T(e) — K T T(g) T(g) T(g)

Reproductive condition

Bore in 1999

Latest infant

Menopausal? Active Active Active Lactating Lactating Active Active Lactating Lactating — Active Active Active Active Lactating

No No Yes No No No Yes Yes No No — Yes No Yes No No

1993 1994 Nulliparous Nulliparous 1998 1998 1997 1997 1998 1998 — 1998 (dead) 1994 1997 1998 1998

786

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

Turnover Between the Alpha and Beta Males At 1000 h on 9 November, GN interfered between R7 and his consorting female (Ngn: 24 yr) and attacked her. R7 rushed GN, and they began to tug and bite each other, which lasted for ca. 3 min. This aggressive interaction was unusually loud. Most members of the troop observed the interaction, and nearly 30 individuals surrounded the conflict within a distance of 20 m. During the aggression, all 7 GN-affiliative females were present but did not participant in the aggression or support GN. R4 (the third-ranked male and R7’s brother) and TG (the alpha female and R7’s mother) joined the aggression and supported R7 by biting GN. In the course of the conflict, TG threatened and lunged at R4, even though both had jointly supported R7 against GN. R4 stopped attacking GN. R7 then bit GN for about 1 min. When injured GN escaped from R7, he first showed the bared-teeth expression, which signals subordination, to R7, indicating that the dominance relationship between them had reversed. R7 and most of the individuals that had surrounded the conflict, including R4 and TG, followed GN. TG then moved in front of R7 and performed a facial threat, as though she were intervening in the aggression, but R7 ignored her. GN lay down and became immobile at 10:04h, and R7 sat ca. 5 m from him. After the aggression concluded, R7 was in proximity to GN, and attacked him 15 times in 2 h. The alpha female, TG, and her daughter (Tg3) stayed with GN and R7, and groomed GN. GN’s friendly females, except for TW, were found near GN and R7. However, TW stayed with GN and R7. TW presented and copulated with R7 in the presence of GN 11 times within 1 h of the turnover. Because, we concentrated on observing male-male interactions, we can not specify the location of the other GN-affiliative females except for MR and TM, which sat ca. 30 m from GN. R7 approached and attacked them successively 9 times within 34 min after the turnover. There were several adult females other than MR, TM, and TG near GN, but R7 did not attack them. In addition, aggression by R7 directed against MR or TM is nil in the aggression data during the stable dominance condition (Kutsukake and Castles, 2001), rejecting the possibility that R7 attacked them frequently irrespective of the turnover. Furthermore, no persistent attack towards females other than GN-affiliative females occurred after the turnover, which facts suggest that R7’s attacks were not random but highly selective against MR and TM. GN received at least 8 wounds that bled, possibly from R7’s canines. We assume that the injures did not cause severe injury to GN because he was able to walk normally the following day. However, he remained relatively immobile, did not feed for the remainder of the day, and did not

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

787

engage in sexual activity for 3 days. R7 received 5 wounds that bled, but he seemed to have no difficulty performing his daily activities. R4 and TG were not wounded. Dyadic Aggressive Interactions Among Adult Males R7 attacked GN 35 times in the 9 days following the dominance turnover (fig. 3a). Twenty-six aggressive interactions (74%) occurred on the first day. Aggression was always unidirectional and included physical contact and in most cases (33/35 cases) GN showed the bared-teeth facial expression. We observed no reconciliation—a postconflict affiliation between the opponents (Kutsukake and Castles, 2001; Aureli et al., 2002)—between them. The first explicit affiliation, grooming, between them occurred 6 days after the turnover (15 Nov). Except for the GN-R7 dyad, the 6 highest-ranked adult males were seldom near each other, and we observed neither dyadic aggression nor grooming interactions among them. Polyadic Aggression Among the Six Highest-Ranked Males After the turnover, we observed 21 polyadic interactions among the 6 highest-ranked males. Twenty of 21 polyadic interactions occurred within 2 days after the turnover (Figure 3b). In polyadic aggressive interactions, the mean number of adult males that were involved is 3.38 (min: 3; max: 5; median: 3). No polyadic interaction among males involved physical contact. The most frequent pattern (in 15 polyadic interactions) was GN attacking a lower-ranked male, which occurred mainly within 1.5 days of the turnover (Figure 3b). Aggression was directed toward R4 9 times; GN solicited R7 all 9 times and R7 supported GN 5 times. GN attacked ZK 4 times, for which GN solicited R7 3 times and R7 supported GN twice. GN attacked N 3 times, for which GN solicited R7 all 3 times, but R7 never supported GN. Accordingly, GN became quite aggressive toward the lowerranking males and enlisted support from R7. Seven of 21 polyadic interactions included aggression directed towards a dominant male. GN was the target of aggression in 4 polyadic interactions (by R4 once, ZK once, N-M coalition once, and M once), R7 in one polyadic interaction (by N-M coalition), R7-GN coalition in one polyadic interaction (R4-ZK coalition once), and R4-ZK coalition in 1 polyadic interaction (by N-M coalition once). In 3 cases of relatively simple polyadic interactions in which GN was attacked by a lower-ranking male, R7 supported him in reaction to his

788

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

Fig. 3. (a) Frequency of aggression of R7 towards GN following the turnover per h. G indicates the first observed grooming interaction between R7 and GN. (b) Number of polyadic aggression involving the 6 highest-ranking males. The dashed line indicates the number of cases of aggression directed from GN to lower-ranking male(s). In both (a) and (b), observations could not be conducted in the morning of November 11, 1998.

solicitation of support, and they won the encounter (against R4, ZK, and M once each). Also, GN solicited R7 against R4 once, but R7 did not support him. R4 retreated. In rather complex polyadic interactions involving 4 males, lowerranking males formed revolutionary coalitions:

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

789

Example 1 (10:56 h, 9 Nov): R4 threatened GN. ZK then sided with R4 and threatened GN. R7 approached and sided with GN, (which did not solicit support from R7), and confronted the R4-ZK coalition. GN chased R4, and R4 chased (redirected towards) ZK. Example 2 (13:23 h, 9 Nov): N and M approached and sided with each other, and moved towards R7, which was threatened by the N-M coalition, but ignored them. After that, the N-M coalition moved to GN and threatened GN. GN ignored them. M dissolved the coalition and moved away from the confrontation. N continued to threaten GN alone, but eventually moved away. Example 3 (15:25 h, 12 Nov): N and M approached and sided with each other and moved towards R4. R4 was threatened by the N-M coalition. ZK and TG approached the confrontation and sided with R4. M dissolved the coalition and fled from the R4-ZK-TG coalition. N bit TG but was chased away by R4 and ZK. The examples indicate that the males that did not participate in the turnover (ZK, N, and M) also began a dominance challenge and that both the reduced-in-rank GN and other males (R7 and R4) became targets of the dominance challenges by lower-ranking males.

Coalition Formation and Soliciting for Aid Among Males Regarding coalition formation, we were unable to determine the direction in all polyadic interactions since individuals who formed a coalition approached simultaneously and began the aggression, e.g., N-M coalition in examples 2 and 3. Table III is a support-receiver matrix of cases in which the identity of the supporter was apparent. In 11 of 12 cases, the dominant was supported against a subordinate. Table IV shows the number of coalitions formed, not considering the direction of the agonistic support. We observed 16 conservative coalitions and 4 revolutionary coalitions, while, there was Table III. Agonistic support among 4 high-ranking males Receiver Supporter

R7 GN R4 ZK

R7

GN

R4

ZK

0 1D 0 0

6D 0 1D 0

1D 1D 0 1D + 1S

0 0 0 0

S : the support to a subordinate male against a dominant male. D: the support to a dominant male against a subordinate male.

790

Kutsukake and Hasegawa Table IV. Coalition formation among the 6 highest-ranking males

R7 GN R4 ZK N M

R7

GN

R4

ZK

N

M



9C —

2C 3C —

1C 0 1C+1R —

0 0 0 0 —

0 0 0 0 3R —

C: the conservative coalition. R: the revolutionary coalition.

no bridging coalition, which suggests that revolutionary coalition is rare. Of the 16 conservative coalitions, 9 were between GN and R7, indicating that they formed a stable alliance against lower-ranking males. Via a matrix correlation test, coalition formation is not related to maternal relatedness (Figure 4: TauKr = 0.773, P = 0.06), age differences (TauKr = 0.202, P = 0.35) or rank differences (TauKr = −0.337, P = 0.82). The head-flagging display occurred in 17 polyadic interactions, 21 times among 5 dyads (Table V). All times, except for one from R7 to GN, were directed from a subordinate to a dominant male. GN displayed most frequently to R7 (16 times), and succeeded in obtaining his support 5 times (31%). GN never solicited support from another male. R7 displayed to GN once against R4 and GN supported him. R7 did not respond to solicitations from any male other than GN, though R4 and ZK each displayed to R7 once. GN supported both of R4’s solicitations, even though GN frequently attacked him with support from R7.

Fig. 4. Number of coalitions formed among the 6 highest-ranking males. Relationships with (a) maternal kinship, (b) rank difference, (c) age difference.

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

791

Table V. Solicit for aid among the 4 highest-ranking males

Initiator GN R7 R4 R4 ZK

Target

Direction (hierarchy up/down)

# head flagged display

# success

R7 GN R7 GN R7

up down up up up

16 1 1 2 1

5 1 0 2 0

Changes in the C-Score for Each Female Before and After Turnover We compared the C-score for each affiliative female to GN before and after the turnover, and found a significant C-score decrease after the turnover (Figure 2a, b; Wilcoxon signed-rank test: N = 7, T = 0, p = 0.016). After the turnover, only the alpha-female and her daughters had high Cscores (Figure 2c) and frequently groomed GN. Other females were rarely in proximity to GN. However, in the next non-mating season (1999), 4 M females also showed the highest C-scores for GN. This suggests that separation by M females from GN after the turnover was temporal and did not last until the nonmating season. Other females were not proximate to GN in the next nonmating season (Figure 2b, c). Regarding the females affiliated with R7, we could not compare the C scores before and after the turnover because of the small sample size (N = 3; Figure 2d). For each scan during the pre- and post-turnover periods and the stable period, we calculated the total C-score of all females for GN and R7 (Figure 5), and compared the total C-score before and after the turnover via the Mann-Whitney U test. For GN, the total C-score decreased significantly (Npre = 17, Npost = 39, U = 129.0, P < 0.0001), indicating that the many females that had surrounded GN before the turnover separated after the turnover. For R7, no significant difference showed before or after the turnover (Npre = 15, Nafter = 40, U = 262.0, P = 0.55).

Change of Receptivity in Females On the turnover day, at least 6 of 52 nonlactating adult females with no previous sign of sexual receptivity began to show signs of receptivity. Via simulation, we calculated the likelihood of beginning of sexual receptivity in them on the same day. The null hypothesis is that the simultaneous start of receptivity could occur in 6 females in their menstrual cycles. The length of the cycle in nonpregnant females (31.8 ± 11.8 days) is based on

792

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

Fig. 5. Mean (+SE) of the total C-scores for GN and R7 before the turnover, after the turnover, and during the dominance-stable period.

Takahata’s (1980) research on another provisioned troop in Arashiyama, Japan. We created an artificial data set of 52 female cycles. The data set has a normal distribution and the mean and SD matches Takahata’s (1980) data. We refer to the length of each cycle randomly generated as Mi. The possibility that X of Y females began receptivity on a certain day can be calculated by the following function (Dunbar, 1988, 1999; Nunn, 1999): X Y−X   Y! × (1/Mi) × (1 − 1/Mj) X! × (Y − X)! i=1

j=1

The simulation revealed that the probability of 6 females becoming receptive on the same day (0.004%) was less than the significant level. R7 copulated with 2 of the females. In the following year (1999), 14 females gave birth in the troop; however, 2 females did not. TW also became receptive on the turnover day, but she is not included in the analysis because she was lactating then. TW showed a sudden C-score increase for R7 (Figure 2e) and copulated with him. TK and KR also had an increased C-score for R7 (Figure 2e) after the turnover and copulated with him. Because TK and KR had been receptive when the turnover occurred, their increased C-scores for R7 were not caused by the start of receptivity. However, the 3 females did not birth the next year.

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

793

DISCUSSION Turnover and Coalitionary Aggression Among Males In this study, mating conflict resulted in the turnover between the alpha and beta males, and the turnover occurred with severe aggression involving a high cost of physical injury. Although the absolute number of dyadic and polyadic aggressive interactions was low among high-ranking males during periods of dominance stability, because of the demography of the group (fewer adult males than adult females), there was a sudden increase in aggression among males, both dyadic, i.e., aggression from R7 to GN, and polyadic. The increased frequency of aggression during the dominance instability period concurs with previous observations on chimpanzees and white-faced capuchin monkeys (de Waal, 1982; Perry 1998ab). In this study, the plausible function of dyadic aggression was to stabilize the new dominance hierarchy, i.e., R7 attempted to stabilize his dominance over GN by frequently attacking him soon after the turnover (Figure 3a) even though GN had already shown the formal signal of submission soon after the turnover. Conversely, GN might have attempted to maintain his beta position with the help of R7 in polyadic interactions, despite frequent aggression from R7. Also, GN was the only male from which R7 solicited aid against R4. Furthermore, R7 used GN as a conservative alliance partner to defend his alpha position from challenges by subordinates, even against his own brother R4. Indeed, R7 reacted to 31% of GN’s solicitations for aid and supported him. The data suggest that R7 and GN formed a conservative alliance, despite the fact that R7 harassed GN within the dyadic dominance relationship. A conservative alliance between alpha and beta males also occurred in chimpanzees (de Waal, 1982). Males also used aggression in order to destabilize the dominance rank after the turnover. Not only the males involved in the turnover (R7, GN, R4) but also the subordinates (ZK, N, M) contributed to the aggression. In addition, both the reduced-in-rank GN and other males, such as R7 and R4, became targets of dominance challenges. This suggests that the turnover and temporal dominance instability stimulated the males to try to increase their ranks. Coalitionary dominance struggles among males have been reported only anecdotally or in nonsystematic observations in macaques by Itoigawa (1993), Mizuhara (1964), Nakamichi et al. (1995), and Witt et al., (1981), and a quantified analysis failed to show that male macaques use coalitions to enhance their social rank in male bonnet macaques Macaca radiata (Silk, 1993). Regular occurrence of male-male coalition formation to improve social rank—political coalitions—in dominance competitions had

794

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

been reported only in chimpanzees by de Waal, (1982), Nishida, (1983), and Nishida and Hosaka, (1996). One distinct characteristic of the polyadic dominance competitions was that males avoided the cost of engaging in severe aggression: polyadic aggression never developed into physical aggression, even when the lowerranking males had numerical superiority when attacking the dominant male (N and M attacking GN or R7: example 2), most coalitions were conservative and incurred little cost for the coalition partners, and lower-ranking males stopped forming revolutionary coalitions once the higher-ranking males had formed conservative coalitions (examples 1 and 3). Via series of experimental studies, Chapais and colleagues who created subgroups of female Japanese macaques to induce rank reversals, noted the importance of alliances with kin (Chapais, 1988) and non-kin (Chapais et el., 1991, 1994; Chapais & St-Pierre, 1997) in the process of rank inheritance. Subordinate female Japanese macaques challenged the dominant individual only when the power asymmetry was obviously in favor of the subordinates (Chapais, 1992). Thus, it appears that female Japanese macaques follow a minimumrisk strategy, i.e., they avoid and reduce the risk of aggression when the expected it is high. The female behaviors seem similar to the low-cost strategy by males in this study group, which indicates that males also followed a minimum-risk strategy during dominance instability. Theoretically, cooperation can evolve through several mechanisms, such as mutualism, reciprocal altruism, and kin selection (Dugatkin, 1997). Although we had few cases of coalition formation and agonistic support to analyze the ultimate factor quantitatively, it is likely that the behaviors were based on mutualism because most of the coalitions were conservative with little associated cost. Moreover, stabilizing the dominance relationship offers immediate benefits to the participants of the coalition. Several researchers have suggested that male coalitions may be mutualistic (Bercovitch, 1988; Noe, 1992; Watts, 1998; Widdig et al., 2000).

Behavior of the Alpha Female, TG TG was the only female that participated to the dominance turnover (supporting R7 against GN). Supporting a more closely related individual (son) than a more distant one (brother) may be reasonable for TG. However, TG’s aggression towards R4 during the cooperative aggression towards GN is difficult to understand because TG did not need to attack R4 to defeat GN. TG also threatened R7 after the submission signal by GN to R7, though TG had supported R7 in the defeat of GN. Further, TG was the only female that participated in the polyadic interactions among

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

795

the males (example 3), which suggests that the alpha female held a special position in the group, and possibly influenced male dominance ranks to some extent (Gouzoules, 1980; Chapais, 1983; Chapais and Lecomte, 1995; Nakamichi et al., 1995; see also Raleigh & McGuire, 1989). Given the influence of the alpha female, it may be beneficial for natal males that are born to a high-ranking matriline to postpone emigration and to attain the dominant position in their natal group with the help of the females (Chapais, 1983). In this study, TG increased proximity to deposed alpha male GN rather than proximity to R7. Nakamichi et al. (1995) observed a similar case of turnover in another troop of Japanese macaques and found that females, e.g., the alpha female, increased their proximity to the new alpha male after turnover. In Shiga A-1, the alpha female was a sister of GN, which have reduced the total C-score for R7, as compared to the situation observed by Nakamichi et al. (1995). The relatedness between TG and all of the highest-ranking males may have made the influence of the alpha female much stronger in this study group.

Changes in Females: Fragile Friendship and Flexible Receptivity Our study showed that proximity between friendly adult females and the deposed alpha male temporarily disappeared following turnover. Moreover, the total C score to GN per scan decreased after the turnover. The results agree with Perry’s (1997, 1998b) observations that proximity scores of adult females and defeated previous alpha males decreased after the male’s defeat. In contrast, the proximate relationship of R7 and adult females did not appear to be affected by the turnover, except for the 3 receptive females that increased proximity to R7, which disagrees with previous studies that showed increases in female proximity to the new alpha male (Nakamichi et al., 1995; Raleigh and McGuire, 1989). We supposed that avoidance of aggression from R7 was one of the factors explaining the short-term change in relations between GN and friendly females for the following 3 reasons. First, frequent aggression from R7 to GN occurred soon after the turnover. The aggression may have prevented friendly females from accessing GN, because interacting with the target of aggression incurs the cost of becoming involved in the aggression. In Japanese macaques, previously uninvolved third parties affiliate significantly less with victims of aggression as compared to affiliation under normal conditions (Aureli et al., 1993). Second, females with a Cscore increase after the turnover were related to R7. In general, related females showed tolerance, irrespective of dominance position. Finally, R7

796

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

attacked GN-affiliative females soon after the turnover. Because the females were not spatially close to GN at the time of attack, it appears that R7 targeted the females selectively. The attacks also suggest that R7 recognized the affiliative relationships between GN and his friendly females. Not all females friendly to GN, behaved similarly. Notably, TW actively copulated with and maintained proximity to R7 after the turnover, and she was not attacked by R7. TW became receptive on the day of the turnover. TW’s behavior may have functioned actively to cultivate interactions with R7 and thus avoid R7’s attacks. In addition, the long-term effects of the dominance turnover on malefemale friendship varied; M females resumed their proximate relationship to GN, but TM and TW did not. For M females, it may have been beneficial to remain friendly with the beta male GN because the M was historically subordinate to the other kin groups and the strong bond with GN might have improved their dominance rank over other matrilineal groups (Kutsukake, 2000). Conversely, TM and TW were from the high-ranking T kin group and a loss of benefits resulting from ending friendship with GN may have been less important for them. We found that 6 adult females became receptive on the day of the turnover. Simulation showed a significance level indicating that this would not occur coincidentally. Although several studies have shown that the female receptive condition changes in reaction to exposure to a new male (Wilson and Gordon, 1979; Okayasu et al., 1992), no study has shown that changes in intra-group social situations induced immediate receptivity in female primates. The benefits of the onset of receptivity by social stimulation within a group are unclear. It is possible that the physiological mechanism that led to the onset of receptivity when exposed to a new male was a response to the unusual intra-group dynamic event, i.e., the dominance reversal between the alpha and beta-male. However, we cannot draw conclusions about the adaptive significance of the phenomenon from our data, and we need more studies to reveal the adaptive significance of immediate receptivity.

Generality of the Study and Importance of N == 1 Observation Because our results are based on a single case we hesitate to conclude that the behavior we observed represents or is typical of the species. For example, regarding male-male interaction, there is little evidence that malemale coalitions play an important role in dominance relationships among males in unprovisioned wild Japanese macaques (Furuichi, 1983, 1985).

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

797

Male-male coalitions may reflect a high frequency of aggression and antagonistic relationships among males under provisioned conditions (Hill, 1999). Moreover, there is much dissimilarity with the study by Nakamichi et al. (1995), in which the males did not form a coalition and struggle for rank. The high relatedness among the highest-ranking males, which is unusual in a society with female philopatry and male dispersal, also might have facilitated the formation of coalitions among males. Therefore, dominance turnover and its influences vary with the case. What kind of general biological conclusions can we draw from case studies of a rare event? As Chapais (1985) and de Waal (1991) noted, they are valuable to provide new insight into behavioral flexibility and social strategy. As long as researchers use systematic observation methods when observing rare cases, the results can lead to new hypotheses, which can be tested in controlled conditions. For example, experimental manipulation of group composition can address questions on the power dynamics of the dominance relationship among females (Chapais, 1992) and malefemale friendship in Japanese macaques (Chapais and Lecomte, 1995). Such paradigms are very useful to test the phenomena that we have reported. In addition, captive studies can reveal hormonal changes under conditions of varying access to males (Mitsunaga et al., 1994), which is useful to reveal the physiological aspects of flexible female receptivity. Therefore, we emphasize the usefulness of single-case studies based on systematic observations, like this one, to provide new testable questions. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Duncan Castles and Ichiro Tanaka for support over the course of this study. We especially thank Eishi Tokida, Sogo Hara, Haruo Takefushi, Toshio Hagiwara and other staffs of Jigokudani Monkey Park for permitting the study and supporting every stage of my fieldwork. C. Hemelrijk kindly provided the program of Matrixtester, for which we are grateful. Reviewers provided critical comments on the manuscript, for which we are grateful. The study was supported by JSPS Research Fellowships and The 21st Century COE Program (The Center for Evolutionary Cognitive Sciences at the University of Tokyo). REFERENCES Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227– 265 Aureli, F., Cords, M., and van Schaik, C. P. (2002). Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. Anim. Behav. 64: 325–343

798

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

Aureli, F., Veenema, H. C., van Panthaleon van Eck, C. J., and van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1993). Reconciliation, consolation, and redirection in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Behaviour 124: 1–21. Belisle, P., and Chapais, B. (2001). Tolerated co-feeding in relation to degree of kinship in Japanese macaques. Behaviour 138: 487–509. Bercovitch, F. B. (1988). Coalitions, cooperation and reproductive tactics among adult male baboons. Anim. Behav. 36: 1198–1209 Chapais, B. (1983). Matriline membership and male rhesus reaching high ranks in the natal troops. In: Hinde, R. A. (ed). Primate social relationships: an integrated approach. Oxford, Blackwell Scientific Publications. pp. 171–175. Chapais, B. (1985). An experimental analysis of a mother-daughter rank reversal in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Priamtes 26: 407–423. Chapais, B. (1988). Experimental matrilineal inheritance of rank in female Japanese macaques. Anim. Behav. 36: 1025–1037. Chapais, B. (1992). The role of alliances in social inheritance of rank among female primates. In: Harcourt, A. H., and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds). Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals, New York, Oxford University Press, pp. 29–59. Chapais, B. (1995). Alliances as a means of competition in primates: evolutionary, developmental, and cognitive aspects. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 38: 115–136. Chapais, B., Girard, M., and Primi, G. (1991). Non-kin alliances, and the stability of matrilineal dominance relations in Japanese macaques. Anim. Behav. 41: 481–491. Chapais, B., and Lecomte, M. (1995). Induction of matrilineal rank instability by the alpha male in a group of Japanese macaques. Amer. J. Primatol. 36: 299–312 Chapais, B., Prud’Homme, J., and Teijeiro, S. (1994). Dominance competition among siblings in Japanese macaques: constraints on nepotism. Anim. Behav. 48: 1335–1347. Chapais, B., and St-Pierre, C. G. (1997). Kinship bonds are not necessary for maintaining matrilineal rank in captive Japanese macaques. Int. J. Primatol. 18: 375–385. Chapais, B., Gauthier, C., Prud’Homme, J., and Vasey, P. (1997). Relatedness threshold for nepotism in Japanese macaques. Anim. Behav. 53: 1089–1101. Colmenares, F., and Gomendio, M. (1988). Changes in female reproductive condition following male take-overs in a colony of hamadryas and hybrid baboons. Folia Primatologica 50: 157–174. Cords, M. (1997). Friendships, alliances, reciprocity and repair. In: Whiten, A., and Byrne, R. W. (eds). Machiavellian intelligence II –extentions and evaluations-. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, pp. 24–49. Dugatkin, L. E. (1997). Cooperation among animals. Oxford, Oxford University Press. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate social systems. London & Sydney, Croom Helm. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1999). Male mating strategies: an optimal foraging problem. In: Kappler, P. (ed) Primate males–causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Ehardt, C. L., and Bernstein, I. S. (1986). Matrilineal overthrows in rhesus monkey groups. Int. J. Primatol. 7: 157–181. Furuichi, T. (1983). Interivdividual distance and influence of dominance on feeding in a natural Japanese macaque troop. Primates 24: 445–455. Furuichi, T. (1985). Inter-male associations in a wild Japanese macaque troop on Yakushima Island, Japan. Primates 26: 219–237. Gouzoules, H. (1980). The alpha female: Observations on captive pigtail monkeys. Folia Primatologica. 33: 46–56. Grewal, B. S. (1980). Social relationships between adult central males and kinship groups of Japanese monkeys at Arashiyama with some aspects of troop organization. Primates 21: 161–180. Gygax, L., Harley, N., and Kummer, H. (1997). A matrilineal overthrow with destructive aggression in Macaca fascucularis. Primates 38 149–158. Haucourt, A. H, and de Waal, F. B. M. (1992). Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals. Oxford, Oxford University Press.

Dominance Turnover Between Male Japanese Macaques

799

Hemelrijk, C. K. (1990). Models of, and tests for, reciprocity, unidirectionality and other social interaction patterns at a group level. Anim. Behav. 39: 1013–1029. Hill, D. A. (1999). Effects of provisioning on the social behaviour of Japanese and rhesus macaques: implications for socioecology. Primates 40: 187–198. van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., and van Schaik, C. P. (1992). Cooperation in competition: the ecology of primates bonds. In: Harcourt, A. H., and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds). Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. New York, Oxford University Press. pp. 357– 389. van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., and van Schaik, C. P. (1994). Male bonds: Affiliative relationships among nonhuman primate males. Behaviour 130: 309–337. Itoigawa, N. (1993). Social conflict in adult male relationships in a free-ranging group of Japanese monkeys. In Mason, W. A. and Mendoza, S. P. (eds). Primate social conflict, Albany, State University of New York Press, pp. 145–169. Kawamura, S. (1958). The matriarchal social order in the Minoo-B group -A study on the rank system of Japanese macaque-. Primates 1: 149–156. (in Japanese) Keddy Hector, A., and Raleigh, M. J. (1992). The effects of temporary removal of the alpha male on the behavior of subordinate male vervet monkeys. Amer. J. Primatol 26: 77– 87. Koyama, N. (1967). On dominance rank and kinship of a wild Japanese monkey troop in Arashiyama. Primates 8: 189–216. Kummer, H. (1978). On the value of social relationships to nonhuman primates: A heuristic scheme. Soc. Scie. Info. 17: 687–705. Kurland, J. A. (1977). Kin selection in the Japanese monkey. Contributions to Primatology 12. Kutsukake, N. (2000). Matrilineal rank inheritance varies with absolute rank in Japanese macaques. Primates. 41: 321–335. Kutsukake, N., and Castles, L. D. (2001). Reconciliation and variation in postconflict stress in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata fuscata): testing the integrated hypothesis. Animal Cognition. 4: 259–268. Mitsunaga, F., Nozaki, M., and Shimizu, K. (1994). Suppressed copulatory behavior and ovarian function in lactating Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata fuscata) during the mating season. Primates 35: 79–88. Mizuhara, H. (1964). Social changes of Japanese monkey troops in the Takasakiyama. Primates 5: 27–52. Nakamichi, M., Kojima, Y., Itoigawa, S., Imakawa, S., and Machida, S. (1995). Interactions among adult males and females before and after the death of the alpha male in a freeranging troop of Japanese macaques. Primates 36: 385–396. Nishida, T. (1983). Alpha status and agonistic alliance in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Primates 24: 318–336. Nishida, T., and Hosaka, K. (1996). Coalition strategies among adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. In McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T., (eds). Great Ape Societies. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. pp. 114–134. Noe, R. (1992). Alliance formation among male baboons: Shopping for profitable partners. In: Harcourt, A. H., and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds). Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. New York, Oxford University Press. pp. 285–321. Nunn, C. L. (1999). The number of males in primate social groups: a comparative test of the socioecological model. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 46: 1–13. Okayasu, N. (1992). Prolonged estrus in female Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) and the social influence on estrus: with special reference to male intertroop movement. In: Itoigawa, N., Sugiyama, Y., Sackett, G. P., and Thompson, R. K. R. (eds). Topics in Primatology, Vol 2 Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation, Tokyo, University of Tokyo Press. pp. 163–178. Pusey, A. E., and Packer, C. 1997. The ecology of relationships. In Krebs, J. & Davies, N.B. (eds). Behavioural Ecology, Fourth edition, Blackwell, Oxford. pp. 254–283. Perry, S. (1997). Male-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Behaviour. 134: 477–510.

800

Kutsukake and Hasegawa

Perry, S. (1998a). Male-male relationships in wild white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus. Behaviour 135: 139–172. Perry, S. (1998b). A case report of a male rank reversal in a group of wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Primates 39: 51–70. Raleigh, M. J., and McGuire, M. T. (1989). Female influences on male dominance acquisition in captive vervet monkeys, Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus. Anim. Behav. 38: 59– 67. Schnell, G. D., Watt, D. J., and Douglas, M. E. (1985). Statistical comparison of proximity matrices: Applications in animal behaviour. Anim. Behav. 33: 239–253. Silk, J. B. (1987). Social behavior in evolutionary perspective. In: Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., Struhsaker, T. T. (eds). Primate Societies. Chicago, University of Chicago Press. pp. 318–329. Silk, J. B. (1993). Does participation in coalitions influence dominance relationships among male bonnet macaques? Behaviour 126: 171–189. Smuts, B. B. (1999). Sex and friendship in baboons - a version with a new preface-. Cambridge, Harvard University Press. Sprague, D. S., Suzuki, S., and Tsukahara, T. (1996). Variation in social mechanisms by which males attained the alpha rank among Japanese macaques. In: Fa, J. E., and Lindburg, D. G., (eds). Evolution and ecology of macaque societies, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, pp. 444–457. Swedell, L. (2000). Two takeovers in wild hamadryas baboons. Folia Primatologica 71: 169– 172. Takahata, Y. (1980). The reproductive biology of a free-ranging troop of Japanese monkeys. Primates 21: 303–329. Takahata, Y. (1982). Social relations between adult males and females of Japanese monkeys in the Arashiyama B troop. Primates 23: 1–23. de Waal, F. B. M. (1982). Chimpanzee Politics -Power and Sex among Apes. London, The Johns Hopkins University Press. de Waal, F. B. M. (1986). The brutal elimination of a rival among captive male chimpanzees. Ethology and Sociobiology 7: 237–251. de Waal, F. B. M. (1991). Complementary methods and convergent evidence in the study of primate social cognition. Behaviour. 118: 297–320. Walters, J. R., Seyfarth, R. M. (1987). Conflict and cooperation. In: Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., Struhsaker, T. T. (eds). Primate Societies. Chicago, University of Chicago Press. pp. 306–317. Watanabe, K. (1979). Alliance formation in a free-ranging troop of Japanese macaques. Primates 20: 459–474. Watts, D. P. (1998). Coalitionary mate guarding by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 44: 43–55. Widdig, A., Streich, W. J., and Tembrock, G. (2000). Coalition formation among male Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Amer. J. Primatol. 50: 37–51. Wilson, M. E., and Gordon, T. P. (1979). Sexual activity of male rhesus monkeys introduced into a heterosexual group. Amer. J. Phys. Anthrop. 50: 515–524. Witt, R., Schmidt, C., and Schmidt, J. (1981). Social rank and Darwinian fitness in a multimale group of Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvana Linnaeus, 1758) -Dominance reversals and male reproductive sucess-. Folia Primatol 36: 201–211. Zinner, D., and Deschner, T. (2000). Sexual swelling in female hamadryas baboons after male take-overs: “deceptive” swellings as a possible female counter-strategy against infanticide. Amer. J. Primatol. 52: 157–168.

Dominance Turnover Between an Alpha and a Beta Male and ...

Social conflict in adult male relationships in a free-ranging group of Japanese ... Alliance formation among male baboons: Shopping for profitable partners.

430KB Sizes 0 Downloads 227 Views

Recommend Documents

Herding and Male Dominance
Nov 11, 2017 - data to explore the extent to which these measures are associated with historical dependence ... in economically relevant outcomes today (Ashraf et al., 2016; Becker et al., 2017; Chen, 2013; ...... include age, age squared, a dummy fo

The Conditional Relation between Beta and Returns
returns. In periods where excess market returns are negative, an inverse relationship ..... beta and monthly returns, Fama and MacBeth conclude that the SLB model ad? ... model does not describe the last 50 years of average stock returns" (p.

Review;183* Alpha Male Pickup Review - Get a ...
Good day, and thanks for visiting the best rrnternet site. ... Where to buy Alpha Male Pickup online cheap, and we also help it become easy to locate and read through ... Free Download Alpha Male Pickup download ebooks to read on laptop.

A Foundation for Alpha and Beta Portfolios by R. Uppal ...
by R. Uppal and P. Zaffaroni ... Sample covariance matrix (close to) singular, badly estimated ... Here: Jointly tilt covariance matrix and expected return estimates ...

Patterns of alpha, beta and gamma diversity of the ...
been influenced by an insufficient inventory effort and an incomplete database for the study area. (Flores–Villela et ... regarding how the database and GARP binary models were constructed can be consulted in. García (2006). ..... servation of tro

Watch Alpha Beta (1976) Full Movie Online Free (HD 1080P ...
Watch Alpha Beta (1976) Full Movie Online Free (HD 1080P Streaming) DVDrip.MP4.pdf. Watch Alpha Beta (1976) Full Movie Online Free (HD 1080P ...

Aggregate Turnover and Margin Scheme.pdf
Turnover, in common parlance, is the total volume of a business. The term 'aggregate turnover' ... composition levy. Let us dissect the definition in small parts to.

RESEARCH ARTICLES Familiarity and Dominance ...
The goal of this study was to provide the first data on dominance rela- tionships of sooty ... formation, mediated by way of dominance relationships, one can also expect a .... Animal moves slowly forward while visually scanning the forest floor, ...

Monitoring, Moral Hazard, and Turnover
Mar 5, 2014 - than bad policy). 6 Summary and conclusions. Endogenous turnover acts as a disciplining device by inducing the politicians in office to adopt ...

Monitoring, Moral Hazard, and Turnover
Mar 5, 2014 - U.S. Naval Academy. E-mail: ..... There is a big difference between the outcomes in the mixed-strategy equilibria (with ..... exists in the data.

Equilibrium labour turnover, firm growth and unemployment
Mar 27, 2012 - The advantage to this strategy is that hiring (and training) new workers is a ... (auto-regressive) process, posting a high wage today is not only a ...

Biosociology-Of-Dominance-And-Deference.pdf
Page 1 of 2. Download ~-~-~-oo~~ eBook Biosociology Of Dominance And Deference. (eBooks) Biosociology Of Dominance And Deference. BIOSOCIOLOGY OF DOMINANCE AND DEFERENCE EBOOK AUTHOR BY ALLAN MAZUR. Biosociology Of Dominance And Deference eBook - Fre

Equilibrium labour turnover, firm growth and unemployment
Mar 27, 2012 - state of the market s3, and then describes dynamic (Markov) equilibria (e.g.. Mortensen and Pissarides (1994)). In contrast equilibrium wages ...