Dynamic Decision Support for Emergency Responders 2009 Technologies for Homeland Security Conference

Dr. Jill L. Drury (The MITRE Corp.), Dr. Gary L. Klein (The MITRE Corp.), Dr. Mark Pfaff (Indiana U. Indianapolis), and Loretta More (Penn State U.)

Core Idea Let emergency responders visualize more futures and save more lives through Robust Decision Making (RDM)

http://www.surfcityhb.org/images/users/ fire/cedar_fire3a.jpg Photos from Huntingdon Beach (CA) Fire-rescue; right: http://www.surfcityhb.org/images/users/fire/fire_rescue.jpg 2

Problem Decision-making processes break down under complex emergency response situations such as major hurricanes and earthquakes

Hurricane Katrina devastation Photo from http://www.ci.huntington-beach.ca.us/images/users/fire/Hurricane%20Katrina%20 Response2.jpg 3

Robust Decision Making (RDM) 

Use simulations to project a landscape of futures Vary things we control  Vary things we don’t control 

 

Identify the most robust course of action (COA) COAs evaluated in terms of an aggregated cost measure

Optimal “Optimal” solutions can be so sensitive to small changes in the environment that they can often be poor choices

Robust Robust solutions may not be the best choice under all conditions but are good under most 4

RDM bridges the “Situation Space” and “Decision Space” gap Photo: Jill Drury



Situation space (SS) consists of the facts such as raw sensor data, mapbased information, or alerts



Decision space (DS) involves information fusion or analysis

5

Robust DecisionMaking (RDM) Analysis

  

Simulation model generates plausible futures for each course of action (COA) and calculates range of costs Decisions involve choosing the most robust COA based on the upper/lower bounds of cost metrics A COA with a tight cost range indicates it is relatively stable even when worst case conditions occur 6

Costs computed by underlying model  

Decision-aid software runs a model in the background For each action alternative: 

The model looks at everything that could happen in the situation… Will the wind come up or die down? Will it start raining soon?



…and computes the cost for each of those possible futures assuming a given COA

A Huntington Beach (CA) Fire responder surveys the Cedar Fire in 2003. See http://www.surfcity-hb.org/images/users/fire/cedar_fire3a.jpg 7

Example cost measure 

Penn State’s NeoCITIES emergency response simulation model adds costs that represent: The number of resources used  Property damage  Injuries, and/or deaths that might result from a decision 



Penn State’s Info Sciences and Technology building

Assigns costs based on the current situation plus extra costs that might be incurred for future incidents if too few resources are conserved to handle them 8

Starting simple: Tukey’s box-plot Depicts a range of costs for each alternative:      



The highest cost of all possible futures that might occur The cost of 25% of all futures fall between here and the median The median cost (half cost more & half cost less) The cost of 25% of all futures fall between here and the median The lowest cost of a future under this alternative The costs of 50% of all futures fall within the box

The median cost and range of cost values depend on the likelihood of each possible future and how it will interact with the chosen alternative

Cost



Alternative 1 9





Sending one fire truck will preserve more resources for the future and cost less now So it is has a lower median cost than sending two, and that could be even lower if all external conditions are favorable

Cost Likelihood Decision Will be Regretted

Allows comparison of alternatives’ possible outcomes

1

2 Fire Trucks

10

 

 

But there is also an argument for two Sending two fire trucks costs more now but deals better with the worst cases (e.g., high winds); it’s maximum cost is much lower Sending two fire trucks has less downside risk Decision-maker’s role is to choose which alternative is “better” for the situation at hand

Cost Likelihood Decision Will be Regretted

Comparison revisited

1

2 11

Experiment purpose and design 

Evaluate the… Impact of RDM information on decision making  Use of box-plots for conveying that information  Principled design of unambiguous and ambiguous decision-making test situations 



Mixed design

“DS group”

“SS group”

Between subjects for box-plots AND text vs. text only  Within subjects for ambiguous vs. unambiguous test cases  41 participants 

 

Independent variable: presentation type Primary dependent variables: Chosen course of action  Time to make decision  Confidence level of decision 

12

Post-session questionnaires  

Primarily designed to control for possible confounders Three parts drawn from standard instruments: Risk taking versus risk aversion (Blaise and Weber, 2006)  Visual versus verbal information processing (Childers et al., 1985)  Vivid versus non-vivid imaging (Sheehan, 1967) 



Plus… Demographic information, including past experience with emergency response  Overall reactions to the degree of decision support provided 

13

Test case manipulations 

Unambiguous cases Ambiguous cases of type 1: robustness conflicts 



Best case vs. worst case vs. median case costs

Cost



Ambiguous cases of type 2: cost function conflicts Type 2A: Magnitude vs. current costs  Type 2B: Current costs vs. future costs 

14

Test environment

15

Hypotheses      

H1: The DS group will make decisions that will result in more positive outcomes H2: The DS group will be more confident in their decisions H3: Non-ambiguous decisions will be made faster than ambiguous decisions by both groups H4: In the case of ambiguous decisions, the DS group will take longer to make the decision H5: The DS group will give higher scores for the degree of decision support provided to them H6: Participants in both groups who believe there is a large chance of future events occurring will under-allocate resources

16

Experiment participants 

35 participants from a not-for-profit corporation 13 female, 22 male  14 had previous experience in emergency response  5 were 30 years of age or younger, 7 were 31 – 40, 6 were 41 – 50, 11 were 51 – 60, 6 were 61+ 



6 participants from a university All 6 were male  2 had previous emergency response experience  All 6 were 25 years of age or under 

17

Experiment conduct 







Participants trained via selfpaced PowerPoint to act as police or fire/rescue commanders Participants got ten practice situations to become familiar with the interface For the main experiment session…  Participants provided with text information about a situation  One group was also given a graph with box plots for each alternative  They chose one alternative and indicated confidence  Repeated for 40 situations Then participants answered questions about the experiment and themselves

18

Results: H1 

H1: The DS group will make decisions that will result in more positive outcomes. SUPPORTED



DS group made the correct resource allocation 68% of the time, compared to 40% in the SS group (Χ2(1) = 122.99, p < .001) Based on the odds ratio, individuals with the decision support were 3.15 times more likely to get the correct answer than those without



19

Results: H2 

H2: The DS group will be more confident in their decisions. SUPPORTED



A one-way Kruskal-Wallis test showed that participants with the decision aid reported much higher confidence (M = 5.41) than those without (M = 4.95), H(1) = 24.11, p < .001 A subsequent Kruskal-Wallis test showed that DS group members also reported much higher confidence for nonambiguous events (M = 5.55) over the other three types (M = 5.07, 5.11, and 4.99), H(3) = 34.12, p < .001



20

Results: H3 





H3: Non-ambiguous decisions will be made faster than ambiguous decisions by both groups. SUPPORTED A mixed factorial ANOVA for decision time (R2 = .48) showed that decisions about nonambiguous events were made fastest (M = 41.21 sec) Means for all four types differ significantly at α = .05

60

55 51

50

46 41

40 30 20 10 0

21

Results: H4 and H5 

H4: In the case of ambiguous decisions, the DS group will take longer to make the decision. NOT SUPPORTED



A mixed ANOVA showed no significant difference in decision time with respect to condition for any event type (p = .30)



H5: The DS group will give higher scores for the degree of decision support provided to them. SUPPORTED



The DS group rated the system as more highly supportive (M = 5.3) than the SS group (M = 4.5), t(38) = 2.14, p < .05

22

Results: H6 

 



H6: Participants in both groups who believe there is a large chance of future events occurring will under-allocate resources. NOT SUPPORTED Analyzed using one-way withinsubjects ANOVA (R2 = .09) Those reporting a “more than usual” chance of future events over-allocated by .10 resources Those answering “same as usual” or “less than usual” under-allocated by .11 and .09 resources, respectively

0.2 0.1 0 -0.1 -0.2

23

Results: Covariates 

No significant effects were found for covariance of any of the following:  Prior experience with box-plots  Emergency response experience  Vivid vs. non-vivid imaging  Risk taking versus risk aversion  Visual versus verbal information processing A Huntington Beach (CA) fire department member battles the Yorba Linda fire in 2008 (http://www.surfcityhb.org/images/users/fire/berkeley_camera_071.jpg) 24

Conclusions 

 

The decision space information did positively impact decisions made using the box-plot decision aid DS group had a higher confidence in decisions than the SS group DS group felt they had greater decision support than SS group 

Participants interact with the NeoCITIES testbed at Penn State (photo courtesy of PSU)



Participants did not appear to have difficulty in understanding or making use of the plots

We successfully introduced decisionmaking conflict based on decision space trade-offs

25

Future work Participants interact with the NeoCITIES testbed at Penn State (photo courtesy PSU) 

Further human-subjects experiments: Have participants interact with model’s equation parameters  Allow participants to interact with visualizations of results  Explore different visualization approaches 

26

Dynamic Decision Support for Emergency Responders

Fire-rescue; right: http://www.surfcity- hb.org/images/users/fire/fire_rescue.jpg ... facts such as raw sensor data, map- based information, or alerts. ▫ Decision ...

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 22 Views

Recommend Documents

Dynamic Decision Support for Emergency Responders
for Simulation-Based COA Planning, Laboratory for AI. Research, The Ohio State ... http://www.cs.bath.ac.uk/hiins/acceptedpapers.html. 7. Furnas, G. W. ...

PDF Counter-Terrorism for Emergency Responders ...
the 1990’s brought the reality of terrorism home and the Sept 11th tragedy let us know that the United States is a high priority target. The goal of ...

INTELLIGENT SYSTEMS FOR DECISION SUPPORT ...
small gap at T = 0 means that rules with T = 0 are excluded from being .... Uncertainties are emphasized here because according to Harvard Business .... identical situations and understand the same phrases differently when hearing or reading .... car

Clinical Decision Support
... noted Luke Hunter the president of Panthera at the organization’s blog “I know of ... Support: The Road to Broad Adoption Online , Read Best Book Clinical ... Clinical Decision Support, 2nd Edition explores the technology, sources of

Dynamic Dissimilarity Measure for Support-Based ...
Abstract—Clustering methods utilizing support estimates of a data distribution ... (For a mathematical ..... Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, vol. 27, no.