Golden Years or Financial Fears? How Plans Change After Retirement Seminars Steven G. Allen Robert L. Clark Jennifer Maki Melinda Sandler Morrill This Version: October 2015* Abstract: Many organizations provide retirement planning seminars to their employees as a benefit to help them make more informed retirement plans. This study examines 85 pre-retirement planning seminars conducted by five companies in 2008 and 2009 to determine whether participants improved their financial knowledge and learned important facts about retirement programs and, on the basis of this potential change in knowledge, adjusted their retirement plans. Using surveys conducted before and after the seminars, we find that financial literacy and knowledge of retirement program parameters were significantly higher after the seminar. Employees with the largest increases in knowledge were most likely to change their planned retirement age and planned age of claiming Social Security benefits. (JEL Codes: J26, J14)

*

(Corresponding Author) Allen, Professor of Economics and Management, NC State University and Research Associate, National Bureau of Economic Research. Email: [email protected]. Clark, Stephen Zelnak Professor of Economics and Management, NC State University and Research Associate, National Bureau of Economic Research. Email: [email protected]. Maki, FTI Consulting. Email: [email protected]. Morrill, Associate Professor of Economics, NC State University. Email: [email protected]. This research was supported by a grant from the FINRA Investor Education Foundation. All results, interpretations and conclusions expressed are those of the author(s) alone, and do not necessarily represent the views of the FINRA Investor Education Foundation or any of its affiliated companies. No portion of this work may be reproduced, cited, or circulated without the express written permission of the author(s).

Golden Years or Financial Fears? How Plans Change After Retirement Seminars

I. Introduction As retirement approaches, an older worker faces a series of important and irreversible choices including when to retire from her current job and when to start collecting Social Security. These decisions have become much more complicated as defined contribution plans largely replaced defined benefit plans and as financial options facing investors become more complex. These actions will strongly affect individuals’ income levels in retirement, the sensitivity of income to economic fluctuations, and the ability to maintain consumption throughout retirement. To make the transition into retirement successfully, workers must rely on their financial knowledge and obtain information about retirement programs offered by employers and the government. Without accurate information and sufficient financial literacy, some older workers will make suboptimal employment and investment choices that will have adverse consequences. Most lifecycle models of economic behavior assume that individuals have basic financial literacy and make resource allocation decisions to develop saving/consumption plans along with work/retirement choices to maximize lifetime utility. Economists and other social science researchers have recently examined the level of financial literacy and its role in economic decision-making. The general conclusion of this research, recently surveyed by Hastings, Madrian and Skimmyhorn (2013) and by Lusardi and Mitchell (2014), is that individuals have quite low levels of financial literacy. Individuals with low levels of financial literacy have difficulty with paying bills on time, saving, and diversifying (see Hastings, Madrian, and Skimmyhorn, 2013, and references therein). Low levels of financial 2

literacy are also associated with lower wealth (see Lusardi and Mitchell, 2007, 2011; Gustman, Steinmeir and Tabatabai, 2012; van Rooij, Lusardi and Alessie, 2012). This paper addresses two key questions. First, can financial literacy programs increase knowledge? And second, if steps are taken to increase financial literacy and pension understanding, will more knowledgeable individuals change their retirement plans? While it is interesting to see an association between low literacy levels and certain types of decisions, this does not necessarily imply that improving financial literacy will lead to a revision in retirement plans. Low literacy levels could reflect other variables (such as intelligence, attitudes, or preferences) that do not show up in household surveys but more truly determine choices. A few studies have attempted to determine whether changes in knowledge base can lead to changes in decisions. For instance, Liebman and Luttmer (2015) conducted a field experiment where a sample of older workers received information about Social Security provisions in the mail; these workers were 4 percentage points more likely than the control group to be in the labor force one year later. Financial literacy programs can take many forms and can occur in many different settings. This study focuses on financial education and retirement planning programs in the workplace. Since individuals spend substantial time at work and since much of what they need to know to develop retirement plans is related to employer-provided benefits, the workplace is well suited for financial education programs. The programs that we have studied are provided by the employer at no cost to the employee and are conducted during the workday on company time. While individuals are not required to attend the seminars, employers in this study indicated to us that most eligible workers do attend a retirement planning seminar prior to retiring.

3

In this study, we examine the retirement planning programs offered by employers to their older employees and assess their impact on financial literacy and retirement plans. There is a sizable literature on retirement planning (recent examples include Benitez-Silva and Dwyer, 2005, and Chan and Stevens, 2008) and the choice of when to claim Social Security benefits (see Behaghel and Blau, 2012, and references therein), but the role of financial literacy is underexplored. A related literature finds that those with access to seminars save more and have higher wealth (e.g., Bayer, Bernheim, and Scholz, 2009; Bernheim and Garrett, 2003; Lusardi, 2005), but does not explicitly consider how seminars alter retirement plans. We study how worker knowledge changes after completion of a retirement seminar and whether the changes in knowledge actually lead to changes in retirement plans. We build upon previous work by Clark, Morrill and Allen (2011, 2012a, 2012b) that examined retirement plans of workers in three companies that did not have retirement planning seminars. That study finds employees who mistakenly think that Social Security or pension eligibility is later than it really is plan to retire later than well informed workers. The results are more mixed for those who mistakenly thought they were eligible earlier than they really were; this type of mistake on full Social Security benefit eligibility is associated with earlier retirement, whereas mistakes regarding early Social Security eligibility and private pension eligibility were unrelated to planned retirement age. This paper demonstrates that employees’ knowledge about pension plan parameters and financial matters increased as a direct result of attending a pre-retirement planning workshop. We then explore whether attendees made changes in their plans for retirement and receipt of Social Security benefits. Our findings indicate that participants increase their financial and pension literacy, and on the basis of new knowledge many alter their retirement plans.

4

II. Financial Literacy and Retirement Decisions When individuals enter the labor force, they immediately begin making important choices about their lifetime consumption and saving profiles. Lifecycle theory suggests that individuals set retirement goals and targets early in their careers. In order to achieve these retirement goals, workers select labor supply, saving and investment behavior consistent with their goals. As new information becomes available, rational decision-makers will re-optimize their consumption and saving patterns, and they may alter their retirement expectations using this new knowledge (Benitez-Silva and Dwyer, 2005). In an uncertain world, there is a distribution of possible outcomes that arise from each retirement decision. The primary retirement goals that workers must set include the age of retirement and their levels of retirement income. A fundamental principle in retirement planning is that younger retirement ages and higher retirement incomes require more saving and less consumption throughout one’s working life. Greater financial knowledge allows individuals to make more feasible retirement plans and to increase the likelihood of meeting consumption goals in retirement. While considerable attention has been paid to the undersaving of American workers, much less attention has been focused on how older workers make decisions concerning the allocation of their resources as they enter into retirement. Workers must decide when and how to enter into retirement, and how to best use the resources available to them. Limited available evidence suggests that older workers do not have sufficient financial literacy needed to make the many choices that must be made as they transition from work to retirement (Bernheim 1995, 1998; Hilgert and Hogarth 2002; Lusardi 2008; Lusardi and Mitchell 2006, 2007, 2014). Incorrect or insufficient knowledge can lead to suboptimal choices. For this reason, programs that increase financial literacy and retirement program knowledge have the potential to improve

5

retirement decisions and produce better retirement outcomes (Clark and d’Ambrosio 2003; Clark, et al., 2006; Lusardi 2008). Pre-retirement planning seminars held in the workplace can efficiently address the numerous questions that individuals approaching retirement share, thereby reducing human resources costs. Lusardi and Mitchell (2014) provide a theoretical framework in which savings, investments and financial knowledge are determined within a lifecycle context. Individuals invest in financial knowledge up until the point where the marginal value equals marginal cost. Those who save and invest the most will obtain the greatest payoff from financial knowledge. Thus, we expect those with higher levels of wealth at retirement to also have higher levels of financial literacy. Likewise, firm-provided retirement planning seminars reduce the cost to employees of obtaining financial knowledge and should thus be expected to increase their financial literacy. Some of the most important decisions older workers must make include: (1) What age to retire from a career jobs? (2) When retiring from a career job, whether to request a lump sum distribution from a defined benefit plan or accept a life annuity from the plan? (3) What age to start receiving Social Security benefits? (4) Whether to annuitize the account balances in a 401(k) plan? In making these important decisions, individuals must rely on their own financial literacy and understanding of financial mathematics and have accurate knowledge about their employers’ and national retirement programs. Workers can acquire the needed knowledge to make these key decisions in various ways, and one resource often available is employersponsored pre-retirement planning programs.1 1

Clark, Maki, and Morrill (2014) find that newly hired workers use a variety of sources to gain information about retirement planning including employer HR offices, colleagues, family and friends, and financial planners, as well as the internet and the popular press.

6

Many large employers offer some type of planning seminar for retirement eligible employees. Sabelhaus, Brogdan, and Holden (2008) report that 46 percent of pension participants covered by defined contribution plans work for companies that provide resources to assist participants in retirement choices. Thirty percent of participants have the opportunity to attend employer seminars and workshops, and almost eighty-five percent of these attendees rely on this information to ‘some’ or a ‘great’ extent in making their retirement decisions. In a plan sponsor survey, Wray (2008) finds that 31 percent of employers offer seminars focusing on retirement assets and income planning. While not universal, employer-provided retirement planning programs are common, accessible to perhaps one-third of the labor force. Although many business leaders and analysts believe these programs are beneficial to employees and increase their financial knowledge, relatively little formal analysis of employerprovided programs has been conducted. Thus, not much is known about the effectiveness of workplace education and its ability to alter the retirement decisions of employees. To address these important issues, we examine the pre-retirement programs of several large employers.

III. Research Methodology This study examines the impact of retirement seminars on the retirement plans of older workers at five large organizations based in the United States. The retirement income system in the United States differs from those in other developed countries by its greater reliance on employer pension plans and individual saving decisions. While virtually all employees are covered by the national Social Security system, benefits from this program provide relatively low

7

income replacement rates for middle and higher income earners.2 Because of this, for higherearning career workers, the benefits from employer-provided pensions can exceed those from Social Security. Historically most private employer-provided pension plans were defined benefit plans; however, over the last four decades, there has been a significant shift toward defined contribution plans. In 2013, approximately half of private sector employees participated in an employersponsored retirement plan, with 26 percent of all workers participating in a defined benefit plan and 38 percent participating in a defined contribution plan (USBLS 2013).3 Defined contribution plans typically require individuals to enroll in the plan, decide how much to contribute each year, and how to invest the retirement funds. For most workers, achieving the level of retirement income needed to sustain one’s standard of living in their “golden years” necessitates participation in and contributions to retirement saving plans. The multifaceted retirement system in the United States places considerable responsibility on individuals to save for their own retirement. All of the workers in our sample are included in Social Security. In addition, all of the employers provide a mandatory defined benefit plan and an optional supplemental retirement saving plans that requires individuals to determine their level of saving. Employers recognize the need for additional financial knowledge and provide the type of educational programs examined in this article.

2

Goss (2014) reports that replacement rates for individuals who start benefits at age 62 and have median and high earnings ranged between 25 and 35 percent. He notes that about half of all Social Security retirees begin benefits at age 62. Munnell (2013) describes how the replacement rates from Social Security vary with the age the benefit is started, over time as the full retirement age is increased, and by the levels of income. 3

Some employers provide both types of plans and workers can participate in both plans. http://www.bls.gov/ncs/ebs/benefits/2013/ownership/civilian/table02a.pdf

8

To evaluate employer-provided pre-retirement planning programs, we assembled a team of five large employers ranging in size from 8,000 to 40,000 employees. Four of the firms have sites throughout the United States; their home offices are in New Jersey, North Carolina, Oklahoma, and Washington. Each of the employers offers defined benefit plans (three employers have cash balance plans4), each offers health insurance to active and retired workers, and each offers supplemental defined contribution plans (all but one have an employer match). The employee populations of these companies vary by gender, education, earnings, and geographic location. We worked with each employer in the spring of 2008 to develop an evaluation process for their pre-retirement planning programs. Our methodology included the development of two surveys. The first survey was to be completed by each participant prior to the start of the program. The objective was to obtain baseline socioeconomic data about the individual and his/her household, as well as information concerning retirement plans and investment strategies. Employees also answered questions about their financial literacy and their knowledge of employer and national retirement programs, described in detail in Section IV. At the conclusion of the seminar, participants were asked to complete a second survey. This time, participants answered additional questions concerning the program, the employee’s assessment of the seminar, and its value. The knowledge and literacy questions were repeated, to see if the participants’ overall knowledge of retirement programs and financial markets had improved. Seminar participants were also again asked about their retirement plans, so we can

4

A cash balance defined benefit plan defines the expected benefit in terms of a stated account balance at retirement, rather than a monthly income stream. The employer still bears the investment risk, but the framing of the benefit is more in-line with a defined contribution. For more information, see: http://www.dol.gov/ebsa/faqs/faq_consumer_cashbalanceplans.html, [accessed August 10, 2014].

9

observe any changes in plans that individuals made due to the seminar. Three of the employers used hard-copy surveys. The program leaders at each of these companies extended the length of their programs to allow participants 15 to 20 minutes prior to the start of the seminar to complete Survey One and similar time at the end of the program to complete Survey Two. The other two employers used electronic surveys; in this case, a link to the electronic Survey One was sent via email to participants about a week prior to the seminar and a link to Survey Two was e-mailed to the participants immediately following the seminar. Attendees were given approximately two weeks to complete Survey Two. This research is based on participant surveys from 85 seminars that incorporated our surveys into their programs between June 2008 and December 2009. We merged the responses from the five employers into a single data set that we call Participants Attending Retirement Seminars (PARS). Table 1 presents basic statistics about each employer, their retirement planning seminars, and the sample size in PARS. We do not conduct analysis at the employerlevel and have kept the identities of the employers masked. Although it is interesting to note the similarities and differences among the seminars presented by the employers, with such a small sample of employers one should be cautious about attributing differences observed to differing seminar characteristics with only a limited set of employee control variables. [Table 1] Seminars varied in scope and duration across the employers. During the research period, Employer A held 31 seminars that were 4 hours long with approximately 500 total attendees. Employer B and Employer E both had all day seminars. Employer D had 12 seminars that were two and half days in duration and covered 281 employees. In 2008, the Employer C seminars were a full day, but in 2009 the seminars were reduced to a half day. In general, the seminars

10

focused on providing factual information about the employers’ retirement programs, Social Security and Medicare. The central theme of the seminars was to provide information necessary for individuals to make informed choices regarding retirement date, date of Social Security claiming, choice of disposition type of pension benefits, work after retirement, and other retirement-related planning. Most presenters were human resources staff members and were careful not make specific recommendations to attendees concerning their retirement ages or benefit choices. Similarly, often representatives from Social Security and estate planners would make presentations. Many seminars also included panels of current retirees to give advice and perspective on the importance of planning for retirement and retirement preparedness. By gathering data on diverse seminars and employers, we were able to develop a data set that includes a range of seminar types. In addition, our data span 2008-2009, so some seminars took place during a significant economic decline and some in the early stages of the economic recovery.5 Although this diversity provides strong evidence that retirement planning seminars do lead to knowledge gains and changes in retirement plans, one should be cautious in interpreting any aggregate differences observed between the outcomes at each employer. Indeed, many aspects of the seminar, time period, survey, and sample differ, so one should not interpret any pattern between employers as due to one particular aspect of the employer, seminar, or survey. During the 18 month data collection period, over 1,500 individuals attended the seminars of these five employers. We received completed responses from 1,182 (approximately 75 percent) of seminar attendees. In the analysis, we restrict the sample to participants with valid 5

Goda, Shoven, and Slavov (2012) find a weak negative correlation between the stock market decline in 2008 and planned retirement age, but suggest that the relationship is not driven by wealth shocks. Although our data do span the start of The Great Recession, because the employers held seminars at different times and the population of attendees may have been affected by the macroeconomic conditions at the time of the seminar, our data are not well suited to a study of the effects of the recent recession on retirement planning.

11

responses for age, gender, education, and tenure (dropping 13, 11, 8, and 9 observations, respectively). Next, we restricted the sample to include only those born between 1943 and 1959; thus participants were approximately age 50 to 65 at the time of the seminars (dropping an additional 39 observations).6 We also restrict the sample to those respondents that had a valid response for at least five out of the ten knowledge questions (dropping 104 observations). The final sample size for our analysis is 998 employees (64 percent of those attending the seminars), as shown in Table 1. We examine the impact of pre-retirement planning programs offered between June 2008 and December 2009.7 The usual process followed by the employers is that retirement-eligible employees are invited individually to participate in these programs by their employer. The employers issue invitations and track attendance, and there are usually high participation rates among employees nearing retirement. Because of the high demand by employees to participate in the programs, several employers limit participation to once every five years or so. These programs are on-going and the employers expect that over a number of years most eligible employees will attend one of these programs. Thus, the participants in the programs we observe should roughly reflect the population of older workers at these employers, on average. While our results clearly demonstrate the effectiveness of financial education at the five employers studied, there are two important caveats to the external generalizability of our findings. First, there is a possibility of selection issues associated with who decides to participate 6

The age-50 restriction was applied to limit the sample to individuals approaching retirement decisions. Most of the employers only invite retirement eligible employees to these programs so, in fact, this results in only a small number of observations being deleted from the sample. The upper age limit was applied to limit the sample to those who had not yet attained the normal retirement age for Social Security. In addition, we felt that workers over age 65 had already made the decision to delay retirement and that they would most likely have very different responses to these programs than workers age 50 to 65. 7 At least one of the authors attended a seminar offered by each of the five employers.

12

in various types of educational events and who completes the surveys. In contrast to past studies of voluntary seminars or benefit fairs, the programs we examine are more structured. While attendance is voluntary, employees receive specific invitations from their employers and attendance is recorded. When questioned, the leaders of the various programs in our study reported to us that nearly all workers invited to participate actually attend the programs and, over time, all retirement eligible workers are invited to attend one of these programs. 8 Without any knowledge about the attendees who did not complete the surveys, it is impossible to assess the bias associated with survey completion. However, our data do include over half of all seminar attendees. The second reason our results may not be generalizable to the full population of workers is that our sample necessarily is employed at larger firms that offer formal preretirement planning seminars and have generous pension and retirement benefits.9

IV. Survey Design and the PARS Sample The basic framework of each of the surveys was similar across the firms; however, some components of the surveys were customized for each employer. The surveys mentioned the specific employer by name, where appropriate, and people were asked about their own employer-specific retirement benefits by name. Questions concerning retirement saving accounts differed somewhat between private-sector versus public/nonprofit employers (i.e., the questions concerning 401(k) plans in the survey given to private sector employees were adapted to include references to 403(b) and 457 plans in the survey given to public sector employees). In addition, 8

The programs offered by three of the employers have been presented over a number of years. In contrast, the programs by two of the employers were implemented during our study period and thus a full cycle of retirees has not had a chance to attend one of the programs. 9 Appendix Table A-1 presents a comparison of our sample (described below) to the nationally representative Health and Retirement Study (HRS) data. As expected our sample has higher levels of education, more years of service, higher earnings, and more financial literacy than the HRS sample.

13

several employers requested that specific questions be added to the survey to help them better understand how their employees were using the human resource programs and accessing the help lines offered by their 401(k) providers. The objectives of Survey One were to determine the level of employees understanding of their employers’ pension and health benefits, their knowledge of national retirement plans such as Social Security and Medicare, their financial literacy, and their current retirement plans. Survey One was also developed to obtain economic and family characteristics of the respondents. These questions were guided by the literature review presented earlier and were then used in the development of our models of knowledge productions and the impact of increased literacy on retirement plans. To assess the current level of knowledge regarding national retirement plans, the survey asked about Social Security early and normal retirement ages, as well as early retirement penalties, cost of living increases, and the age of eligibility for Medicare. In addition, participants were asked benefit and eligibility questions concerning their employers’ defined benefit plans and their own 401(k), 457, or 403(b) accounts. This survey included several questions related to basic financial literacy.10 Two of the financial literacy questions, the ones dealing with inflation and stock risk, were adapted from the 2004 Financial Planning module of the HRS (see Lusardi and Mitchell, 2014 for a description of the data and references for articles using these variables).11 Survey One also asked participants the age that they expected to retire, when they expected to start Social Security benefits, what the expected level of benefits would be, what benefits they expected to receive from their employer-

10

These questions are very similar to those used in Clark, Morrill, and Allen (2012b).

11

Appendix Table A-1 compares the responses on these two variables to the HRS sample. In order to make the samples more comparable, we include summary statistics for the subset of the HRS who were between ages 50-65, actively employed, and had a pension.

14

provided retirement plans, and what their work plans are after retirement from their current employer. Additional questions dealt with annuitization of pension assets. Finally, Survey One contained a series of economic and demographic questions concerning current income, wealth, age, marital status, and the work, income, and retirement benefits of any spouse or partner. The primary objectives of Survey Two were to determine how participants evaluated the seminar, whether they enhanced their knowledge of retirement programs, and whether the new information changed their retirement plans. To assess the employees’ impression of the seminars, the first section of Survey Two asked respondents if the program provided useful information, if the information was presented at the right level for them, if the presenters were of high caliber, if they felt better able to make retirement decisions after completing the program, and whether they valued the program as an employee benefit. The next two sections of Survey Two repeated many Survey One questions concerning retirement intentions and knowledge. By comparing the answers given across both surveys, we assess the change in the respondent’s knowledge about retirement programs, financial literacy, and whether participants changed their retirement plans. Means of various demographic, economic, and retirement plans characteristics are shown in Table 2. The sample is composed of 998 workers age 50-65 with a mean age of 57.7 years. Males represented 56 percent of the employees, while 76 percent of respondents are married, and they have an average of 25 years of service with their current employer. In general, these are relatively high earners with above-average retirement wealth. [Table 2]

15

V. Did Participant Financial Literacy Rise? The primary objective of our research is to answer two key questions concerning workplace financial education programs. First, do employer-provided retirement planning programs increase financial literacy and knowledge about company and national retirement programs? Second, if financial literacy is increased, how does this enhanced knowledge affect workers’ retirement plans? To examine the impact of financial education programs, we use the PARS data described above, which includes merged survey responses from the five employers. First, we investigate whether individuals gain knowledge about retirement-related concepts during the seminar. We assume that the individuals’ reported retirement plans before the seminar are made through individuals rationally attempting to maximize their well-being. But, individuals must make these plans under sometimes limited knowledge about financial markets and retirement institutions. They also face uncertainty regarding own and spouse’s future earnings, own and spouse’s health, and financial and labor market conditions. Thus, while we assume individual rationality, we interpret the retirement plans prior to the seminar as a ‘best guess’ that the individual uses to make decisions now about saving, wealth accumulation, retirement date, and work after retirement. During the seminar, individuals will were exposed to information regarding the retirement process, retirement institutions, and general financial concepts. The first question addressed in this study is whether individuals learn during these seminars. This is approximated by considering responses on a series of ‘financial literacy’ questions. After the seminars, the attendees should have improved knowledge about key aspects of retirement. Based on this improved information set, individuals can assess whether their preseminar retirement plans were optimal and whether their savings will be adequate. For example,

16

individuals might reassess the odds that they have saved enough to maintain their desired standard of living during retirement. We hypothesize that the change in knowledge is a function of attendees’ characteristics. First, individuals’ level of education can be considered a proxy for the ability or propensity to learn, so it is expected to be positively associated with knowledge gain. Second, the closer the expected retirement date, the higher the individuals’ motivation to learn and, thus, proximity to retirement should be positively associated with gains in knowledge. Third, the initial level of knowledge may be associated with knowledge gain through several channels. Higher initial knowledge is likely associated with a greater ability to understand the information presented in the seminar. However, knowledge gains, as measured by improvement along our knowledge index, are ‘right censored’ in the sense that those that already score highly are not able to demonstrate as much learning on our quiz. To examine the level of financial literacy and knowledge of retirement programs, both before and after the seminar the participants were asked 10 financial literacy and retirement program questions. The questions, along with a summary of correct answers and percent answering correctly are reported in Table 3.12 As reported in the bottom row of Table 3, the average number of correct responses prior to the seminar is 6.1; afterwards the average number of correct answers rose to 7.5, indicating a substantial level of learning among program participants. In general, the questions on which the lowest percentage of respondents gave the correct answers (P2 to P5) are those questions that relate to Social Security eligibility ages and how Social Security benefits vary with age and over time.13 Note that given our sample of

12

We coded blanks as incorrect answers as long as the respondent provided an answer to five out of ten of the knowledge questions on each survey. 13

It should not be surprising that many older workers do not know the basic eligibility and plan characteristics of national retirement programs. Key parameters of Social Security, Medicare, and pension

17

individuals with relatively generous employer-provided retirement benefits, Social Security may be less important to the respondents compared to others in the labor force. [Table 3] The average knowledge score after the seminar for the entire sample is significantly higher than the average score prior to the seminar. Figure 1 shows the distribution of participants’ knowledge scores (the number of correct answers) before and after the seminar. Prior to the seminar, 42 participants score a perfect 10 while 527 participants have scores of six or lower. After the seminar, 153 participants have correct answers for all 10 questions and only 243 have a score of six or fewer.14 [Figure 1] Figure 2 shows the change in knowledge achieved during the seminar. Overall, 681 participants (68 percent of the sample) improved their knowledge scores, while the knowledge score is unaffected for 194 individuals (19 percent of the sample). Although knowledge scores increased for most of the sample, there were 123 cases where the scores actually fell. We believe the measured decrease in knowledge score is not due to actual loss of knowledge, but rather is largely a case of misreporting in the survey. Respondents were asked to provide a best guess if they did not know the answer, so even random responses would lead to some gains and some

regulations are based on legislation passed at different points in time and have different objectives. As a result, many alternative ages are specified in these programs that determine access to retirement income. A recent study (US GAO 2007) summarized these many different retirement age-related rules ranging from age 55, the age of eligibility for drawing certain pensions without penalties if leaving an employer, to age 70 ½ which is the age for mandatory withdrawals from pension plans to avoid tax penalties. While confusion over these ages is understandable, the retirement income of workers depends on their knowledge of these ages and the timing of their retirement decisions. 14 Some critical variables in the index changed between Survey One and Survey Two. In results not shown, 31 percent of the responses in Survey One on the age at which one can begin receiving normal Social Security benefits were too low and 20 percent were too high. Both of these percentages dropped considerably in Survey Two; 20 percent were too low and 10 percent were too high.

18

losses. Results are not sensitive to the exclusion of respondents whose knowledge score dropped or who left one or more knowledge questions blank (results available upon request). [Figure 2] Figure 2, Panel B illustrates the knowledge gain sorted by the base level of financial knowledge from Survey One. Most importantly, those with initial low scores achieve substantial increases in their knowledge of retirement plans. Of the 527 individuals with low pre-seminar knowledge scores (6 or fewer correct answers), 438 (83 percent) improve their knowledge scores with 43 percent achieving increases of 3 or more additional correct answers. For this reason, we conclude that the pre-retirement planning seminars did increase financial literacy for almost all participants, with large gains among those with relatively little knowledge prior to the event. It should be emphasized that the gains in knowledge are observed across all economic and demographic characteristics of the participants we considered and at all employers. Table 4 shows the knowledge index by subsets of the sample based on various employee characteristics. Note that the knowledge gain reported in each row is statistically significant at the 1% level. There is a 1.4 point gain on a 10 point scale in the mean score of the entire population. Younger participants, those aged 50-61, have a greater increase in their knowledge scores than the older participants. In fact, the gain is so large that it virtually eliminates the difference in knowledge between the two age groups observed prior to the program. Although women have slightly larger gains in knowledge than men, women still have a lower knowledge score than men after the program.15

15

In results not shown, but available upon request, a regression of knowledge score in Survey One on demographic characteristics illustrates a similar pattern whereby men have a 0.276 point higher knowledge score before the seminar and married individuals have a 0.312 point higher score. Interestingly, when regressing the change in knowledge score after the seminar, the estimated coefficients on male and married are small and not statistically significant (0.002 and -0.100, respectively). This

19

[Table 4] Interestingly, people with lower self-assessed knowledge scores prior to the seminar have a much larger gain in mean scores than those with a higher initial self-assessed level of knowledge (1.60 compared to 1.10). This is another indication that the programs are beneficial to those with relatively little knowledge of their retirement plans; although, we should note that mechanically individuals with high scores before the seminar are not able to gain as much on the knowledge index scale. Pre-existing levels of literacy may reflect the amount of attention that respondents have devoted to retirement planning. If true, this might explain why knowledge gains are linked to the changes in retirement plans that we observe below. There also is evidence that those with some college learn more from the seminars than those with no more than a high school education. Workers with some college start with a higher initial level of knowledge than those who have less education (6.39 to 4.89) and the gap widens even further (7.83 to 6.11) after the workshops. This implies that education and retirement planning programs have complementary effects on learning.16 Similarly, higher earners have both higher knowledge scores overall and experience more learning during the seminar. Finally, we consider whether the respondent reports having sought financial advice prior to the seminar. Those that had previously sought advice from any source experience a larger gain in knowledge during the seminar than those who had not previously sought advice (1.43 to 1.39), although the respondents who had not sought advice had much higher knowledge scores both before and after the seminar. In this sample, 300 respondents report having contacted their indicates that within each demographic category represented in Table 4 individuals have statistically significantly higher knowledge after the seminar, but that learning is not different by gender or marital status. 16

In results not shown, but available upon request, those with some college have a 0.505 point higher knowledge gain.

20

employers’ benefit offices for advice prior to attending the seminar. These employees have a lower knowledge score than average before attending the seminar (5.63 to 6.12), but experience a larger overall gain in knowledge of 1.51 questions. Overall, these data provide important statistical support for the conclusion that workplace education is effective in increasing financial literacy for older workers and the gains are often greater for those that enter the event with the lowest levels of financial literacy. The bottom portion of Table 4 shows how knowledge scores changed in each of the five companies and for different groupings based on employer characteristics. Employer D had the largest increase in knowledge scores (1.77 points), followed closely by Employer E (1.76 points) – the employer that introduced the seminars during our study period and invited all employees age 50 and older to attend. Scores increased by 1.38 points or more at employers A and B. The score increase was lowest at Employer C (0.91 points), but that may be partly attributable to a relatively high score in Survey One.17 Next, in Table 4 we see that knowledge increases varied somewhat by seminar characteristics. Scores increased by 1.59 points in seminars that lasted one day or more, whereas scores increased by 1.20 in half day seminars. Seminars that employed outside speakers had larger knowledge gains (1.52) than those that relied on in-house personnel (0.74). While these patterns are in the anticipated direction, with only five firms in the sample one should be cautious of drawing too strong a conclusion. It is likely that other seminar or firm characteristics

17

To test whether the amount of learning across the different seminars was uniform, we regressed the change in knowledge score on a full set of demographic variables and organizational indicator variables. In results not shown, but available upon request, we easily reject the null that learning was identical across all five organizations.

21

that are correlated with an employer’s willingness or ability to have longer programs, for example, influence the relative knowledge gains we see. Finally, we consider whether the survey technique influenced the measurement of knowledge gains. One might think that surveys conducted on paper immediately after the seminar would show larger knowledge gains than those conducted on the Internet up to two weeks later. However, this conjecture is not borne out in the data. The knowledge gain is actually slightly larger in the companies that used Internet surveys. Again, we cannot determine the effect of survey method while holding all else constant, since we only have five employers in our sample.

VI. Did Greater Knowledge Change Retirement Plans? After participating in the workplace educational event and increasing their financial literacy, how do workers respond? Does enhanced knowledge result in changes to planned retirement behavior and the management of retirement wealth?

Here we assess the impact of

participation in a retirement planning seminar and the resulting knowledge gain on several aspects of retirement planning. First, we compare responses between Survey One and Survey Two for four key variables: planned retirement age, planned age to begin receiving Social Security benefits, plans for working after retirement, and the decision to annuitize. Then we more directly estimate how knowledge gains impact planned retirement age in a regression framework. Above we have seen that individuals do exhibit gains in financial knowledge, as proxied by a 10-point knowledge score, from attending a pre-retirement planning seminar. We now turn to whether learning leads to changes in retirement plans. We focus on changes in the dates of

22

expected retirement and Social Security claiming, but also briefly consider defined contribution pension disposition choice and work after retirement. The null hypothesis is that changes in knowledge resulting from the seminar have no economic value. Changes in expected age of retirement and age of claiming Social Security benefits are modeled as a function of changes in financial literacy and retirement knowledge, as well as individual characteristics. Results indicate that expected retirement and age for Social Security claiming are significantly altered by knowledge gains, although not always towards delaying. In Table 5, we present sample means for respondents before and after attending the retirement seminar.

Our sample includes 998 respondents from the PARS data, described

above.18 First, in the top panel of Table 5, we see that although mean planned retirement age increases only slightly (and median retirement age is identical), a full 26.2 percent of seminar participants changed planned retirement age. This is because we observe movements in both directions. When we compare the reported planned age of retirement in Survey Two to the reported planned age of retirement in Survey One, 21.5 percent report an older planned retirement age and 9.5 percent report a younger planned retirement age.19

Note that when calculating the “change” variables the data include 158 respondents that did not provide a planned retirement age and 218 respondents that did not provide a planned Social Security claiming age in Survey One and/or Survey Two. However, the means calculation only uses those observations valid ages reported in both surveys. 18

19

The first row of Table 5 includes the full 998 respondents. Of the 158 that did not report a planned retirement age in both surveys, 61 are missing a retirement age in only Survey 1 (i.e., made a plan during the seminar), 28 are missing a retirement age only in Survey 2 (i.e., abandoned their retirement plan during the seminar), and 69 are missing retirement age in both surveys. Of the 165 that did not report a planned social security claiming age in both surveys, 48 are missing a claiming age in only Survey 1 (i.e., made a plan during the seminar), 15 are missing a claiming age only in Survey 2 (i.e., abandoned their retirement plan during the seminar), and 165 are missing retirement age in both surveys. Because of the small sample sizes of these “non-responders”, we do not present further results about these individuals. Appendix Table A-2 details the change in planned retirement age between Survey One and Survey Two. Among those who initially planned to retire at age 62 to 64 in Survey One, 21 percent reported a later planned retirement date in Survey Two whereas only 8 percent report an earlier date. Among those who

23

[Table 5] Next, in Table 5 we observe that the seminars influenced respondents’ planned Social Security claiming age.20 Again, while 29.5 percent of respondents indicated a change in the age they plan to start receiving Social Security benefits, we observe little change in the mean and median due to plans being both delayed and moved earlier as a result of seminar attendance. As shown below, 67.0 percent indicate plans to postpone benefit receipt.21 This variation is reasonable as individuals are constrained in the front end (i.e., it is not possible to claim Social Security earlier than at age 62), while one can theoretically postpone claiming Social Security benefits indefinitely there is no rational reason for delaying the start of benefits after age 70.22,23

planned to retire at age 65 in Survey One, we find that 14 percent plan to retire earlier and 25 percent plan to retire later after Survey Two. At an initial planned retirement age 66 and higher there is slightly more movement toward an earlier retirement (14 percent) than there is movement toward later retirement (11 percent). 20

There is some tendency for retirement and Social Security claiming decisions to be made in tandem as presented in Appendix Table A-3. Among the 163 employees who decided to delay retirement, 53 percent also said that they planned to delay Social Security claiming. Most of the remaining respondents reported no change in planned Social Security claiming, and nine percent reported earlier planned Social Security claiming ages. Among the 66 respondents who said they planned to retire earlier, 36 percent said they would claim Social Security earlier and 18 percent said they would delay Social Security claiming. Appendix Table A-4 illustrates how changes in knowledge regarding Social Security eligibility rules affected retirement plans. 21

Of the 97 individuals that report in Survey Two an earlier planned claiming age than stated in Survey One, the average decline 2.36 years, with the largest change being a drop of 6 years. Of the 197 individuals which reported plans to postpone claiming Social Security, the largest change was by 11 years, with a mean of 2.28 years. 22

The large change in planned age of claiming benefits follows from the observation that learning the age for full benefits in the seminar is rather easy and the finding that Americans still overwhelming believe that the age for full benefits is 65. A recent survey by Mass Mutual found that 71 percent of respondents incorrectly gave 65 as the age for full benefits (Plan Sponsor, June 15, 2015). 23

Because we have only limited information about households, we refrain from categorizing plans as ‘good’ or ‘bad’, since many factors, including preferences and attitudes towards risk, will influence desired retirement. However, there is no financial reason to delay claiming Social Security benefits beyond age 70.

24

In the second half of Table 5, we explore two other retirement planning outcomes: plans for working after retirement and plans for the disposition of pension benefits. Here we observe a large fraction of the population reporting that they have not yet made these plans. Comparing responses before and after the seminar, we observe 10.2 percentage points more respondents reporting planning to work after retirement.

Similarly, about 10 percentage points fewer

respondents indicated uncertainty regarding whether to work after retirement or not. Next, Table 5 explores shifts in plans regarding pension and supplemental retirement saving account distributions. Prior to the seminar, there was considerable uncertainty regarding whether to annuitize account balances in supplemental retirement plans and whether to take lump sum distributions from the defined benefit plans. Post-seminar, fewer respondents indicate uncertainty regarding these important decisions. Before the seminar, approximately 19 percent of the sample planned to annuitize some or all of their supplement plan funds. This increased to 26 percent after the seminar, with the proportion that had not decided declining by 10 percentage points. However, almost half of the participants were still undecided in regards to annuitization. Pre-seminar, approximately 26 percent of the employees were certain that they want to take a lump sum distribution of their defined pension benefit while 63 percent were uncertain. Responses to this set of questions postseminar indicate little change. These results suggest that deciding whether to take a lump sum distribution from one’s pension or to annuitize one’s 401(k) balance are complicated decisions that require substantial consideration. However, the changes observed in age at which to draw on retirement benefits and plans to work after retirement from a career job may be due to a better understanding of retirement income needs and total expected retirement wealth. It should be noted that our data do not allow us to categorize these revised plans as “better” in an objective

25

sense. We assume that changes that are made post-seminar reflect better information, and thus are more closely aligned with the individuals’ goals for consumption in retirement, but such changes may also reflect the influence of peers or leadership at the seminar and therefore might not actually result in higher utility in retirement. We have thus far shown that retirement seminars increase knowledge and that retirement plans are revised during the seminars. Next, we directly test whether the changes in retirement plans are associated with improved financial knowledge and better understanding of pension plan parameters.

Before attending the retirement planning seminar, each worker has a planned

retirement age based on a host of factors including expected longevity, wealth, expected future earnings potential, features of public and private pensions, and spousal retirement plans. During the seminar, the employee obtains information from the various presentations, has a chance to ask questions, and discusses retirement issues informally with other attendees. In response to this expanded set of information, the worker will either confirm his or her original choice of retirement age or make an adjustment toward earlier or delayed retirement. Formally, we model this thought process with a linear equation where the planned retirement age (R) is a function of knowledge (Info) at a given point in time and personal characteristics (X) related to retirement decision making (e.g., age, gender, earnings and marital status will be reasonable proxy variables for longevity, future earnings, wealth and household decision making). Before the seminar (t = 1), we write that the planned retirement age in period 1, R1, is a linear function of demographic characteristics, X, and the information set, Info. 𝑅𝑖1 = 𝛼1 + 𝛽1 𝑋𝑖1 + 𝛾1 𝐼𝑛𝑓𝑜𝑖1 + 𝜖𝑖1

(1)

26

The planned retirement age after the seminar, R2, is the planned retirement age in period 1 plus any revision to the retirement plan that is made as a function of demographic characteristics, X, and the change in knowledge. 𝑅𝑖2 = 𝑅𝑖1 + 𝛼2 + 𝛽2 𝑋𝑖2 + 𝛾2 (𝐼𝑛𝑓𝑜𝑖2 − 𝐼𝑛𝑓𝑜𝑖1 ) + 𝜖𝑖2

(2)

In the short time between Survey One and Survey Two, we assume that Xi2=Xi1=Xi. Rearranging equation (2), we find that the change in planned retirement age is a function of demographic characteristics and the change in the information set. Equation (3) is our main regression equation: Δ𝑅𝑖 = 𝑅𝑖2 − 𝑅𝑖1 = 𝛼2 + 𝛽2 𝑋𝑖 + 𝛾2 (𝐼𝑛𝑓𝑜𝑖2 − 𝐼𝑛𝑓𝑜𝑖1 ) + 𝜖𝑖2

(3)

The change in planned retirement age, R, will depend on: (a) how people with given values of X reevaluate their retirement decisions (α2 and β2); (b) the effect of changes in the information set, (γ2 ∆Info); and (3) random noise that we cannot capture in our model or data (ε2). In practice, workers have a probability density function for expected retirement age instead of a 100 percent probability at a single age. We assume that workers report the age at which they are most likely to retire. Changes in the probability density function would still be a function of X and changes in Info. Empirically we estimated three closely related probit models: the probability that R2 does not equal R1, the probability that R2>R1, and the probability that R1>R2. To estimate, the impact of the learning that took place at the seminar on planned retirement age, this study focuses on the participant knowledge scores in the PARS data. We start with an aggregate index of knowledge based on the ten items in Table 3; this variable is scaled to be between zero and ten. Because our focus is on planned retirement age, we also break down this index into its four major components: (1) knowledge about Social Security and

27

Medicare eligibility, (2) general knowledge of public programs, (3) overall financial knowledge, and (4) company-specific knowledge. Finally, we examine the role of each individual question in the knowledge score index as an independent variable. In effect, this means that each knowledge variable can have its own coefficient, whereas the aggregate index constrains the coefficients of all ten variables to be the same. One should be cautious when interpreting these coefficients individually as they are all highly correlated and may also be related to knowledge about issues that were not included in our knowledge index. Table 4 showed changes in financial and retirement knowledge after the seminars; Table 5 showed sizable changes in retirement plans after attending seminars. Are these two patterns connected? In Table 6, we explore the effect of this increase in financial literacy on planned retirement age by estimating the probability of a change in planned retirement age based on the three different measures of learning.24 Panel A explores the effects of an increase of financial literacy using an aggregate measure of the change in financial knowledge (based on responses to knowledge questions before and after the seminar) while controlling for demographics, wealth, and the individual’s employer. The change in knowledge score variable indicates the change in the total number of correct responses for the ten knowledge score questions. Column (1) presents the average marginal effects from a probit regression on the probability of a change expected retirement age. Results indicate that an increase in the aggregate knowledge score is associated with an increase in the probability of a change in expected retirement age. Here we see that a one question increase in knowledge score is associated with a 2.3 percentage point higher probability of changing one’s planned age of retirement.

24

Rather than implementing a full factor analysis, we include the components of the knowledge score separately and present estimated coefficients on each item.

28

[Table 6] Columns (2) and (3) further define the change by presenting the average marginal effects of a regression on the probability of a delay in expected retirement and the probability of an earlier expected retirement, respectively. In Column (2), the change in the knowledge score variable is positive and statistically significant which indicates that a one question increase knowledge score is associated with a 1.7 percentage point higher probability of delaying retirement. About 21.8 percent of respondents delayed their planned retirement age, so the effect size of 1.7 percentage points is about 8 percent of the mean. In Column (3), the change in knowledge score is small and statistically insignificant, indicating that a change in the aggregate knowledge score did not lead to an increase in the probability of an earlier planned retirement.25 Table 6, Panel B presents a similar analysis, but disaggregates the change in knowledge score to four distinct groups to better understand which areas of learning impact retirement plans. Results presented in Table 6, Column (1) indicate that both learning about public program eligibility and company-specific retirement plans lead to an increase in the probability of a change in planned retirement age. Knowledge about the age at which an individual is eligible to receive Social Security may have a significant impact on retirement plans if one’s prior understanding of the eligibility for these programs was incorrect. Accordingly, a better understanding of company specific retirement guidelines including pension plan distributions and retiree health benefits may also influence retirement plans.

25

Appendix Table A-5 presents the full set of estimated coefficients for Table 6, Panel A. We see that, relative to participants ages 64 and 65, controlling for gains in knowledge younger workers are more likely to change their retirement plans and plan to retire later after the seminar. Married individuals are less likely to report a different planned retirement age after the seminar. Holding all else equal, participants at Employer E are significantly more likely to report a later planned retirement age relative to those at Employer A.

29

In Table 6, Column (2), Panel B, we see that learning about public program eligibility is associated with an increase in the probability of a delay in planned retirement. In the specification presented in Table 6, Column (3), we see that learning about investment diversification and inflation is associated with decrease in the probability of earlier retirement. A better understanding of inflation could influence the employee’s perception of retirement wealth which might make it less likely he would move up his planned retirement age. Table 6, Panel C further disaggregates the change in knowledge score by presenting the change in response to each of the ten questions. By doing so, we can determine how learning about each individual item influenced retirement plans. However, one should be cautious in interpreting these results as learning about one aspect of retirement planning is likely correlated with other aspects that may not be measured. In Column (1), we see that estimated coefficient on learning about public program eligibility is driven by learning about the age at which the individual can receive full Social Security benefits and the age at which an individual will be eligible for Medicare. As Social Security provides a significant source of retirement income for the individual and health insurance is costly, a better understanding of eligibility for these two important programs is likely to influence retirement plans.26 The results in Column (1) also show that learning about employer-provided health insurance eligibility is positively associated with a greater probability of a change in planned retirement age. The Social Security eligibility coefficients for the delayed retirement probit in Column (2) are similar to those in Column (1). They have the same sign as in Column (1), but are of smaller magnitude. The similarity between

26

Supporting the claim that Medicare eligibility influences planned retirement behavior, Kopczuk and Song (2008) use administrative files from the Master Beneficiary File to show that there are a relatively large number of individuals who claim retirement benefits around their 65th birthday.

30

the results presented in these two columns is due to the fact that of those that do change expected retirement age, they tend to do so by delaying retirement. In Column (3), Table 6, Panel C, we see that two of the three questions included in the “Public Program – General” index are independently statistically significant at the ten percent level but of opposite sign. Learning that the reduction in Social Security benefits due to claiming early retirement is permanent is, paradoxically, associated with an increase in the probability of earlier retirement. Learning that Social Security benefits are increased by the rate of inflation is associated with a decrease in the probability of earlier retirement. For those who thought that benefits increased more rapidly than inflation, this information would have a negative impact on the individual’s perceived retirement wealth which could dissuade him from considering an earlier date of retirement than currently planned. This is somewhat consistent with the results for the financial knowledge question on inflation and retirement income. Those who learned how inflation can erode real income were less likely to change their planned retirement age to be earlier. The relationship between knowledge and retirement plans is complex and depends on the type of knowledge errors. By disaggregating the knowledge score index and exploring each component individually, we see how each piece of knowledge influences the individuals’ planned retirement age. Another key decision facing individuals is when to take up Social Security benefits. Although the life-cycle framework can be used to model retirement decision making, some behaviors are inconsistent with this approach. One such example is the spike in Social Security claiming which occurs at the full retirement age (NRA). Behaghel and Blau (2012) explore the reasons for the spike in benefits claiming that occurs at age 65 by examining the increase in the

31

full retirement age.27 They note that the spike may be due to an endorsement effect. As full retirement age had been 65 since the program began and has only recently been changed,28 individuals may interpret this age as the “recommended” retirement age. It may also be that individuals are loss averse and are unwilling to move away from the full retirement age. Both of these behavioral explanations for the spike at age 65 highlight the potential impact of learning on planned Social Security claiming behavior. Table 7 replicates the models of Table 6 for a different retirement decision – the age at which one begins receiving Social Security benefits. In Table 7, Panel A, we see that changes in the aggregate knowledge score are associated with higher probability of changing one’s Social Security claiming age, higher probability of delaying that age and higher probability of an earlier claiming age. When the aggregate index is broken down into four major components, it is clear that Social Security knowledge is driving the Panel A results. Table 7, Panel C confirms the role Social Security learning about Social Security eligibility. Improved knowledge for the “normal retirement age” for Social Security is associated with greater probability of changing the Social Security claiming.29 [Table 7] Looking across Tables 6 and 7, misperceptions about the ages of eligibility for normal Social Security and Medicare are strongly related to changes in retirement plans.30 The means

27

In 1983, Social Security reform increased the full retirement age from 65 to 66, in two month increments, for cohorts born from 1943-1954. 28

The first cohort to experience the increase in full retirement age resulting for the 1983 Social Security were those reaching their full retirement age in 2004-2009. 29

This is consistent with findings by Song and Manchester (2007) showing that changes in the full retirement age resulted in a shift of the spike to the new full retirement age among affected cohorts. 30

Appendix Table A-4 details the planned retirement and Social Security claiming ages associated with learning about normal Social Security eligibility. There were 113 people in the sample who

32

presented in Table 5 indicated that plans for working after retirement and plans for annuitizing pension wealth also shifted in response to the seminar. However, for both of these categories the major shift was in a reduction of those reporting “have not yet decided.” In fact, for the choice of whether to annuitize one’s pension, in Survey Two the modal response is “have not yet decided.” We did examine how changes in financial literacy and pension knowledge affected plans to work after retirement. In results not reported (available upon request), we find again that changes in knowledge are related to changes in retirement plans, but the patterns are somewhat mixed. While an increase in the aggregate knowledge score reduced the probability of planning not to work after retirement by a very small 1.2 percentage points, when disaggregating there is no consistent pattern. A caveat to our results is that it is possible that the deployment of Survey One before the seminars tipped off respondents to focus on certain parts of the discussion. In this case, individuals might have “learned” more about the specific questions we were asking. But, if it were the case that this learning was not meaningful, then we would expect to see no effect of learning on retirement plans in Tables 6 and 7.31

underestimated the eligibility age for normal Social Security in Survey One but correctly reported the age in Survey Two. Within this group, 12 percent changed their planned retirement age, 27 percent changed their planned Social Security claiming age and 26 percent changed both. Of the 83 who overestimated this eligibility age in Survey One, 8 percent changed their planned retirement age, 28 percent changed their planned Social Security claiming age, and 30 percent changed both. In other words, over half of those who corrected misperceptions about normal Social Security eligibility changed their retirement plans. 31

In results not shown, we confirm that our results in Tables 6 and 7 are robust to excluding individuals that have a decrease in knowledge score. Results are also robust to excluding any individuals that left one or more of the knowledge questions blank on either survey. Results are available upon request.

33

VII. Conclusions Many older workers have a rather low level of financial literacy and understanding of their retirement programs. Limited or inaccurate information may lead them to make poor retirement decisions, undermining their ability to achieve retirement income adequacy. We show how pre-retirement planning programs offered by five large employers improved financial and pension literacy and how this changed employees’ reported retirement plans. How closely to statements about retirement intentions correspond to actual retirement decisions? Using data from the Employee Benefits Research Institute’s Retirement Confidence Survey, Helman, Copeland, and VanDerhei (2015) show that aggregate data on retirement decisions and actual retirement dates are moving in the same direction. The share of employees retiring at age 66 or later has increased from 8 percent in 1991 to 14 percent in 2015, whereas the share of employees intending to retire at age 66 or later has increased from 11 percent to 36 percent.32 At the micro level, Bernheim (1989) found that modal predictions of retirement age correspond closely to actual retirement dates in the Retirement History Survey. Because wealth at retirement and financial literacy are jointly determined, previous studies have struggled with disentangling whether increased financial literacy can influence retirement plans or whether the observed links can be attributed to some other variable that is common to decision-making about wealth accumulation and financial literacy. Before the seminar, participants have retirement plans based on both their knowledge at that time, as well their previous decisions on savings and investments. In other words, their initial retirement plan reported before the seminar is a function of their earnings, wealth, and financial knowledge.

32

Keep in mind that the populations answering these two questions are quite different; one group has already retired whereas the other consists of workers age 25 and up.

34

Because we can assume that all factors except financial knowledge are constant between the surveys given immediately before and after the seminars, we believe that any change in reported retirement plans can be directly attributed to knowledge gains during the seminar. Although this paper focuses on retirement intentions rather than actual retirement decisions, the results are able to illustrate that gains in financial literacy do affect retirement plans.33 Employers who offer pre-retirement planning seminars as an employee benefit are naturally seeking to ensure that these programs are effective and valued by employees. The feedback we received in Survey Two, conducted immediately after attending a seminar, indicated participants had a very positive experience and believed that the seminars are helpful. Some 95 percent of respondents state that the programs provided all or most of the information needed for them to make important retirement decisions. Eighty-six percent of respondents rate the programs very good or excellent, and 93 percent find the presenters and program leaders to be very good or excellent. The seminars also may be creating value for employers by reducing the transaction costs of managing pension plans. Many employees will have the same questions which can be answered more efficiently in a seminar environment than in one-on-one meetings with human resource staff. Consequently, employers can facilitate the transition into retirement by providing the means for workers to increase their understanding of key retirement concepts. The results of this study should provide encouragement to employers considering whether to adopt preretirement planning programs. Based on our assessment of these initial data, employer provided financial education programs can increase employee knowledge of retirement programs and In this “high-stakes” environment our employer-partners were not willing to randomly assign individuals to receive education, so a controlled experiment is not feasible. Thus, while theoretically one could track eventual retirement decisions many years out, even then there is no good comparison group. 33

35

assist them in making better retirement choices. This enables their employees to achieve a more desirable retirement. Also, to the extent that the firm has designed its pension plan to optimize employee retention and the age structure of its workforce, the seminars enhance the probability that these corporate goals will be achieved.

REFERENCES Bayer, Patrick J., B. Douglas Bernheim, and John Karl Scholz. 2009. “The Effects of Financial Education in the Workplace: Evidence from a Survey of Employers.” Economic Inquiry 47(4): 605-624. Behaghel, Luc, and David M. Blau. 2012. “Framing Social Security Reform: Behavioral Responses to Changes in the Full Retirement Age.”American Economic Journal: Economic Policy 4(4): 41-67. Benitez-Silva, Hugo, and Debra S. Dwyer. 2005. “The Rationality of Retirement Expectations and the Role of New Information.” The Review of Economics and Statistics 87(3): 587592. Bernheim, Douglas. 1989. “The Timing of Retirement: A Comparison of Expectations and Realizations,” in David Wise (ed.), The Economics of Aging (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1989), pp. 335-355. Bernheim, Douglas. 1995. “Do households appreciate their financial vulnerabilities? An analysis of actions, perceptions, and public policy,” in Tax Policy and Economic Growth, American Council for Capital Formation, Washington, DC, pp. 1-30. Bernheim, Douglas. 1998. “Financial illiteracy, education and retirement saving,” in Olivia Mitchell and Sylvester Schieber (eds.), Living with Defined Contribution Pensions, University of Pennsylvania press, Philadelphia, pp. 38-68. Bernheim, B. Douglas, and Daniel M. Garrett. 2003. “The Effects of Financial Education in the Workplace: Evidence from a Survey of Households.” Journal of Public Economics 87(78): 1487-1519. 36

Chan, Sewin, and Ann Huff Stevens. 2008. “What You Don't Know Can't Help You: Pension Knowledge and Retirement Decision-Making.” The Review of Economics and Statistics 90(2): 253-266. Clark, Robert and Madeleine d’Ambrosio. 2003. “Ignorance is Not Bliss,” TIAA-CREF Research Dialogue. Clark, Robert L., Madeleine B. d'Ambrosio, Ann A. McDermed, and Kshama Sawant. 2006. “Retirement Plans and Saving Decisions: The Role of Information and Education.” Journal of Pension Economics and Finance, 5(1): 45-67. Clark, Robert, Jennifer Maki, and Melinda Morrill. 2014. “Can Simple Informational Nudges Increase Employee Participation in a 401(k) Plan?” Southern Economic Journal, 80(3): 677-701. Clark, Robert, Melinda Morrill, and Steven Allen. 2011. Evaluating Employer-Provided Financial Education Programs for Pre-Retirees, Report prepared for the FINRA Investor Education Foundation. Available online at: http://www.finrafoundation.org/web/groups/foundation/@foundation/documents/foundati on/p124011.pdf Clark, Robert L., Melinda Sandler Morrill, and Steven G. Allen. 2012a. “Effectiveness of Employer-Provided Financial Information: Hiring to Retiring.” American Economic Review 102(3): 314-318. Clark, Robert L., Melinda Sandler Morrill, and Steven G. Allen. 2012b. “The Role of Financial Literacy in Determining Retirement Plans,” Economic Inquiry, 50(4): 851-866. Goda, Gopi Shah, John B. Shoven, Sita Nataraj Slavov. 2012. “Does Stock Market Performance Influence Retirement Intentions?” Journal of Human Resources, 47(4): 1055-1081. Goss, Stephen. 2014. “Testimony Before the Subcommitte of Social Security, Pensions, and Family Policy, Senate Finance Committee, May 21. Gustman, Alan L., Thomas L. Steinmeier, and Nahid Tabatabai. 2012. “Financial Knowledge and Financial Literacy at the Household Level.” American Economic Review 102(3): 309-313. 37

Hastings, Justine S., Brigitte C. Madrian, and William L. Skimmyhorn. 2012. “Financial Literacy, Financial Education and Economic Outcomes.” Annual Review of Economics, 5:347-373.. Helman, Ruth, Craig Copeland, and Jack VanDerhei. 2015. “The 2015 Retirement Confidence Survey: Having a Retirement Savings Plan a Key Factor in Americans’ Retirement Confidence,” EBRI Issue Brief, no. 413 (Employee Benefit Research Institute, April 2015) Hilgert, Marianne and Jeanne Hogarth. 2002. “Financial Knowledge, Experience and Learning Preferences: Preliminary Results from a New Survey on Financial Literacy,” Consumer Interest Annual 48. Kopczuk, Wojciech, and Jae Song. 2008. “Stylized Facts and Incentive Effects Related to Claiming of Retirement Benefits Based on Social Security Administration Data.” Michigan Retirement Research Center Working Paper 2008-200 Liebman, Jeffrey B., and Erzo F. P. Luttmer. 2015. “Would People Behave Differently If They Better Understood Social Security? Evidence From a Field Experiment.” American Economic Jurnal: Economic Policy, 7(1): 275-299. Lusardi, Annamaria. 2005. “Saving and the Effectiveness of Financial Education.” Journal of Financial Transformation, 15: 159-169. Lusardi, Annamaria.(ed.). 2008. Overcoming the Saving Slump: How to Increase the Effectiveness of Financial Education and Saving Programs. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2006. “Financial Literacy and Planning: Implications for Retirement Wellbeing.” Pension Research Council Working Paper n. 1. Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2007. “Baby Boomer Retirement Security: The Roles of Planning, Financial Literacy, and Housing Wealth.” Journal of Monetary Economics 54(1): 205-224. Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2011. “Financial Literacy and Planning: Implications for Retirement Wellbeing.” National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper No. 17078, May 2011. 38

Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2014. "The Economic Importance of Financial Literacy: Theory and Evidence." Journal of Economic Literature, 52(1): 5-44. Munnell, Alicia. 2013. Social Security’s Real Retirement Age is 70. Center for Retirement Research at Boston College. http://crr.bc.edu/wp-content/uploads/2013/10/IB_13-15.pdf PlanSponsor. 2015. “Social Security Quiz Stumps Most Americans,” http://www.plansponsor.com/Social-Security-Quiz-Stumps-Most-Americans/, June 15. [Accessed July 24, 2015]. Sabelhaus, John, Michael Brogdan, and Sarah Holden. 2008. Defined Contribution Plan Distribution Choices at Retirement: A Survey of Employees Retiring Between 2002 and 2007. Washington, DC: Investment Company Institute. http://www.ici.org/pdf/rpt_08_dcdd.pdf Song, Jae, and Joyce Manchester. 2007. “Have People Delayed Claiming Retirement Benefits? Responses to Changes in Social Security Rules.” Social Security Bulletin 67(2): 1-23. U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (USBLS), 2013. "National Compensation Survey: Employee Benefits in the United States, March 2013," September 2013, Bulletin 2776, http://www.bls.gov/ncs/ebs/benefits/2013/ebbl0052.pdf United States Government Accountability Office (USGAO). 2007. Federal Policies Offer Mixed Signals on When to Retire, Washington, DC: USGAO. van Rooij, Maarten C. J., Annamaria Lusardi, and Rob J. M. Alessie. 2012. “Financial Literacy, Retirement Planning and Household Wealth.” Economic Journal 122(560): 449-478 Wray, David. 2008. Testimony Before the ERISA Advisory Council Working Group on Spend Down of Defined Contribution Plan Assets at Retirement. Chicago, IL: Profit Sharing/401k Council of America, July 16. Available online at: http://www.psca.org/Portals/0/pdf/July%2016%202008%20 testimony.pdf

39

Figure 1. Knowledge Score Pre and Post- Seminar Knowledge Score Number of Respondents

250 200

160

210 185 170 165

207 153

136

150

103

95

100

66

51 50 5 3

14 10

0

1

14

20

39

94

54

42

0 2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

Number Correct (Out of 10) Before

After

Notes: The knowledge score is calculated for those responding to both Survey One and Survey Two. The data is limited to those who were born between 1943 and 1959 and who had valid responses to at least five out of ten knowledge questions, gender, age, tenure, and education. Respondents that did not indicate planned retirement age in Survey One or Survey Two are included. Aggregate knowledge scores for respondents in the PARS data who answered at least five out of ten questions in both Survey One (before the seminar) and Survey Two (after the seminar). Missing or blank responses are treated as incorrect.

40

Figure 2. Changes in Knowledge Scores Pre-Post Seminar

Panel A. All Participants Change in Knowledge Score

Number of Respondents

300 241

250

196

194

200 150

120 85

100

64

50

40

31 1

2

4

-5

-4

-3

18

1

1

7

8

0 -2

-1

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

Panel B. Separately by High and Low Pre-Seminar Knowledge Change in Knowledge Score Number of Respondents

160

141

140 120

138 103

100

101

86

80

60

60

64

53

40

40 20

110

1

2

1 3

10

21 25

19

18 1

1

0 -5

-4

-3

-2

-1

0

1

Low Score Before Seminar

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

High Score Before Seminar

Notes: Panels A and B present aggregate knowledge scores for respondents in the PARS data who answered at least five out of ten questions in Survey One (before the seminar) and Survey Two (after the seminar). Missing or blank responses are treated as incorrect. In Panel B, low scores were 6 or below, high scores were 7 or more questions answered correctly.

41

Table 1. Description of Employers and Seminars Employer (survey method)

Industry

Requirements to attend

Seminar length

Seminar description

Number of Seminars

Number attending a seminar

Total Final survey PARS responses sample

Employer A (hard copy)

Medical technology

Retirement-eligible

½ day

Run by Ayco

31

500*

472

389

Employer B (electronic)

University

Immediately prior to retirement

Full day

In house

4

130

71

68

Employer C (hard copy)

Energy

Retirementeligible, only once per 5 years

2008: Full day 2009: ½ day

2008: In-house with outside speakers 2009: In-house only

27

333

295

238

Employer D (hard copy)

Forest products

Retirement-eligible

2 ½ days

In-house with outside speakers

12

281

165

138

Employer E (electronic)

Energy

Any employee over age 50

Full day

In-house with outside speakers

7

314

179

165

85

1,559*

1,182

998

Totals

Notes: The survey response is calculated as those responding to both Survey One and Survey Two. The PARS data is limited to those who were born between 1943 and 1959 and who had valid responses to at least five out of ten knowledge questions, gender, age, tenure, and education. *We were not able to obtain an exact count of seminar attendees for several of the Employer A seminars. The estimated number of attendees reported here is based on discussions with seminar leaders and from projecting the response rate for seminars where attendance was reported.

Table 2. PARS Data Descriptive Statistics Mean or Percent 57.7 56.0% 76.4% 25.0 82.0% 92.8% 93.1% 4.0 4.6 4.9

Age Male Married Years of Service Some College Covered by a Pension Plan Own Home Self-Assessed Knowledge (1-7)* Years from Planned Retirement (Before) * Years from Planned Retirement (After) * Wealth and Earnings Variables: Earnings -- Medium ($50K-$100K) 44.8% Earnings -- High ($100K+) 31.2% 401(k) Account Balance -- Medium (1-5 Years of 62.0% Current Salary) 401(k) Account Balance -- High (More Than 5 18.4% Years of Current Salary) *Sample sizes for these three questions were less than 998 due to missing response; the sample size for each is as follows: Self-Assessed Knowledge: n=971, Years from Planned Retirement (Before): n=868, and Years from Planned Retirement (After): n=901. Notes: Sample size is 998 respondents from the PARS dataset. All variables reported here are measured before the seminar. See Table 1 for a description of the dataset.

Table 3. Participant Knowledge Before and After the Seminar Percent Answering Correctly Before After Seminar Seminar Financial Knowledge Questions F1 True or false? “Buying a single company stock usually provides a safer return than a diversified portfolio.” False F2 Assume that your retirement income increases by 2% per year and that the annual rate of inflation is 4% per year. After one year, will you be able to: a) buy more goods and services? b) buy fewer goods and services? c) buy exactly the same amount of goods and services? d) don’t know Public Programs Questions P1 What is the earliest age that you can start Social Security benefits? 62 P2 What is the age that you can receive a full or unreduced Social Security benefit (“normal retirement age”)? 66 P3 If you start Social Security benefits at the earliest possible age, you will receive a benefit that is __ percent of the benefit you would have received at the normal retirement age. 75% P4 Is the reduction in Social Security benefits for early retirement permanent or does the reduction end when you reach the normal retirement age? Permanent P5 After you start receiving Social Security benefits, these benefits are: a) the same for the rest of my life b) increased annually by the rate of inflation c) increased annually but by less than the rate of inflation d) increased annually but by more than the rate of inflation e) Don’t know P6 What is the earliest age that you will be eligible for Medicare? 65 Company-Specific Questions C1 Can you take a lump sum distribution of some or all of your pension plan (do not include income for your 401(k) account)? Yes (all five companies) C2 Does your company offer you the opportunity to stay in the company health plan after you retire? Yes (all five companies)

85%

90%

82%

86%

73%

81%

43%

67%

24%

42%

63%

79%

35%

53%

66%

82%

71%

83%

69%

89%

6.1 Average Knowledge Score (Out of 10) Notes: Sample is 998 respondents from the PARS dataset, see Table 1 for a description.

44

7.5

Table 4. Knowledge Gains During the Seminars Participant Characteristic Full Sample

Categories

Age

Ages 50-61 62 and Over Male Female Married Unmarried 20 or Less More than 20 Level 1 to 4 Level 5 to 7

Gender Marital Status Years of Service Self-assessed knowledge (scale of 1-7) Education Earnings Sources of Advice

Employer

Seminar Length Speaker Type Survey Method

Number Score Before of Obs Seminar 998 6.12 Participant Characteristics 820 6.05 178 6.43 559 6.65 439 5.44 762 6.34 236 5.43 294 6.01 704 6.17 578 5.62

Gain in Score 1.40

7.53 7.50 7.91 7.03 7.65 7.13 7.52 7.53 7.22

1.48 1.07 1.26 1.59 1.31 1.70 1.51 1.36 1.60

383

7.01

8.11

1.10

180 818 231 758 384 614 300

4.89 6.39 4.73 6.54 5.50 6.50 5.63

6.11 7.83 6.05 7.97 6.93 7.89 7.14

1.22 1.44 1.32 1.43 1.43 1.39 1.51

Employer Characteristics 389 5.48 68 5.38 238 6.63 138 7.23 165 6.27

6.90 6.76 7.54 9.00 8.03

1.42 1.38 0.91 1.77 1.76

HS or Less Some College 50K or Less More than 50K Sought Any Advice No Prior Advice Advice from HR Office Employer A Employer B Employer C Employer D Employer E Half Day

Score After Seminar 7.52

Full Day or Longer In-House Outside Electronic Hard Copy

480

5.74

6.94

1.20

518 159 839 223 765

6.48 6.21 6.11 6.01 6.15

8.07 6.95 7.63 7.66 7.48

1.59 0.74 1.52 1.65 1.33

Notes: Sample is 998 respondents from the PARS data set; see Table 1 for a description of the data. Entries in these columns are the mean number of questions answered by the participants in each category. The mean knowledge scores before and after the seminar were statistically significantly different at the 1% level for each sample reported by row.

45

Table 5. Respondents’ Retirement Plans Pre- and Post-Seminar Survey One Before the Seminar

Survey Two After the Seminar

Planned Retirement Age: 26.2% Changed Median Planned Retirement Age (N=840) Mean Planned Retirement Age (N=840)

62 62.3

62 62.6

Planned Age to Claim Social Security: 29.5% Changed Median Age Planned Social Security Claiming (N=770) Mean Age Planned Social Security Claiming (N=770)

65 64.2

65 64.5

40.4% 35.8%

50.6% 26.1%

Plans for Working after Retirement Expect to Work after Retiring Have Not Decided on Working after Retirement

Annuitization or Lump Sum Distributions Planning to Annuitize Some or all 401(k) Balance 19.0% 26.5% Have Not Decided on Annuitization of 401(k) Balance 57.3% 46.3% Plan to Take Lump Sum Distribution of Entire Pension 25.7% 24.2% Have not Decided on Lump Sum 62.6% 57.9% Notes: Except where indicated, the sample is 998 respondents from the PARS dataset. See Table 1 for a description. Item non-response for planned retirement age and for planned Social Security claiming age includes blanks as well as the response “Don’t Know.”

46

Table 6. Changes in Planned Retirement Age Due to Changes in Respondent Knowledge Any Change Delay Earlier (1) (2) (3) Panel A: Change in Aggregate Knowledge Score (0-10) 0.023** 0.017** 0.001 (0.008) (0.008) (0.006) Panel B: Changes in… Public Program – Eligibility (P1, P2, P6) 0.031* 0.030* 0.010 (0.017) (0.015) (0.011) Public Program – General (P3, P4, P5) 0.006 -0.003 -0.003 (0.016) (0.015) (0.010) Financial Knowledge Score 0.023 0.024 -0.039** (0.032) (0.031) (0.020) Company Specific Knowledge Score 0.040* 0.028 0.014 (0.023) (0.021) (0.014) Panel C: Changes in… P1: Early Social Security eligibility -0.029 -0.039 -0.002 (0.032) (0.030) (0.022) P2: Normal Social Security eligibility 0.056** 0.064** 0.007 (0.028) (0.025) (0.018) P3: Benefit reduction at age 62 -0.036 -0.046* -0.009 (0.029) (0.027) (0.018) P4: Permanence of benefit reduction 0.063* 0.024 0.037* (0.034) (0.032) (0.022) P5: Social Security indexation 0.006 0.020 -0.031* (0.028) (0.026) (0.017) P6: Medicare eligibility 0.066** 0.058** 0.026 (0.029) (0.027) (0.019) F1: Stock diversification and risk -0.003 0.053 -0.040 (0.050) (0.045) (0.035) F2: Inflation and real income 0.034 0.003 -0.050* (0.046) (0.045) (0.029) C1: Eligible for lump sum distribution 0.007 0.009 -0.006 (0.032) (0.030) (0.021) C2: Retiree health insurance 0.069** 0.039 0.033 (0.034) (0.030) (0.021) Mean of Dependent Variable: 0.351 0.215 0.095 Number of Observations 998 840 840 Notes: See Table 1 for a description of the PARS dataset. The sample for columns (2) and (3) is limited to the 840 respondents with a valid planned retirement age in Survey One and Survey Two. Coefficients are estimated from a probit model and average marginal effects are presented. Each panel consists of three regressions with alternative dependent variables as follows: Column (1) probability of a change in planned retirement age; Column (2) probability of a delay in planned retirement age; and Column (3) probability of earlier planned retirement age. All regressions include controls for age, gender, marital status, tenure, education, earnings, 401(k) account balance, and company fixed effects. Robust standard errors are in parentheses. ***indicates statistically significant at the 1% level, ** indicates statistically significant at the 5% level, and * indicates statistically significant at the 10% level.

47

Table 7. Changes Planned Age to Claim Social Security Due to Changes in Knowledge

Panel A: Change in Aggregate Knowledge Score Panel B: Changes in… Public Program – Eligibility (P1, P2, P6) Public Program – General (P3, P4, P5) Financial Knowledge Score Company Specific Knowledge Score Panel C: Changes in… P1: Early Social Security eligibility P2: Normal Social Security eligibility P3: Benefit reduction at age 62 P4: Permanence of benefit reduction P5: Social Security indexation P6: Medicare eligibility F1: Stock diversification and risk F2: Inflation and real income C1: Eligible for lump sum distribution C2: Retiree health insurance Mean of Dependent Variable: Number of Observations

Any Change (1)

Delay (2)

Earlier (3)

0.017** (0.008)

0.021** (0.009)

0.015** (0.007)

0.041** (0.016) -0.007 (0.015) -0.025 (0.031) 0.041* (0.021)

0.041** (0.017) 0.018 (0.017) -0.030 (0.031) 0.017 (0.023)

0.038*** (0.014) -0.004 (0.012) 0.008 (0.027) 0.014 (0.016)

-0.015 (0.031) 0.100*** (0.027) 0.016 (0.027) -0.011 (0.033) -0.024 (0.026) 0.021 (0.028) -0.024 (0.048) -0.009 (0.044) 0.044 (0.031) 0.033 (0.032) 0.358 998

-0.039 (0.033) 0.122*** (0.028) -0.019 (0.030) 0.035 (0.036) 0.040 (0.028) 0.024 (0.030) -0.029 (0.053) -0.016 (0.048) 0.013 (0.033) 0.013 (0.033) 0.256 770

0.028 (0.026) 0.031 (0.025) 0.038* (0.021) -0.024 (0.026) -0.025 (0.022) 0.050* (0.026) 0.039 (0.039) -0.008 (0.036) 0.019 (0.024) 0.008 (0.025) 0.126 770

Notes: See Table 1 for a description of the PARS dataset. The sample for columns (2) and (3) is limited to the 770 respondents with a valid planned Social Security claiming age in Survey One and Survey Two. Coefficients are estimated from a probit model and average marginal effects are presented. Each panel consists of three regressions with alternative dependent variables as follows: Column (1) probability of a change in planned age to claim Social Security benefits; Column (2) probability of a delay in planned age to claim Social Security benefits; and Column (3) probability of earlier planned age to claim Social Security benefits. All regressions include controls for age, gender, marital status, tenure, education, earnings, 401(k) account balance, and company fixed effects. Robust standard errors are in parentheses. ***indicates statistically significant at the 1% level, ** indicates statistically significant at the 5% level, and * indicates statistically significant at the 10% level.

48

Supplemental Data Appendix TABLE A-1. PARS and HRS Sample Comparison PARS

Health and Retirement Study Full Sample

Aged 50-65 Currently Employed

Age Male

57.7 56.0%

66.6 44.2%

59.0 46.1%

Aged 50-65 Currently Employed, Has Pension 58.8 42.7%

Married Widowed Years of Service

76.4% 3.0% 25.0

60.6% 15.9% 11.7

63.3% 4.0% 11.7

62.5% 1.7% 12.4

Some College 82.0% 20.1% 28.6% 32.3% Own Home 93.1% 83.9% 86.5% 87.3% Years from Planned 4.6 6.9 7.5 7.2 Retirement (Before) Wealth and Earnings Variables: Earnings -- Medium 44.8% 10.5% 23.4% 34.1% ($50K-$100K) Earnings -- High 31.2% 3.0% 6.5% 11.0% ($100K+) Financial Literacy Variables: Stock Risk 84.6% 59.1% 68.3% 68.8% Inflation 82.3% 79.8 83.3% 84.9% Observations 998 1,077 260 145 Notes: The PARS dataset is described in the text and in Table 1. The Health and Retirement Study sample is from the 2008 RAND HRS Data File. The financial literacy questions were asked in 2004 financial planning module of the HRS. This table presents the mean values for participant response for those that participated in the HRS 2008 survey for comparison to the PARS sample. Some rows vary in sample size due to missing data. Survey weights were used to account for complex survey design. RAND Data File Citation: RAND HRS Data, Version M. Produced by the RAND Center for the Study of Aging, with funding from the National Institute on Aging and the Social Security Administration. Santa Monica, CA (September 2013)

49

Table A-2: Matrix of Planned Retirement Age by Survey Planned Age Before\After

<62

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

Total

<62

208

23

3

0

4

2

1

0

0

1

242

62

13

179 7

4

23

6

1

0

0

1

234

63

0

6

22

3

3

1

1

0

0

0

36

64

0

2

3

7

13

0

0

0

0

0

25

65

2

15

3

2

97

29

5

0

1

4

158

66

0

0

0

0

6

74

3

0

0

2

85

67

0

0

0

0

3

8

16

0

0

5

32

68

0

0

0

1

0

0

1

7

1

5

15

69

0

0

0

0

0

0

0

0

0

0

0

70

0

0

0

0

0

0

0

1

0

12

13

Total

223

225 38

17

149 120 28

8

2

30

840

Notes: The numbers in each cell represent the total number of respondents that had that before/after planned retirement age combination. “Before” ages (Survey One) are by row; “After” ages (Survey Two) are presented in columns. The shaded boxes indicate no change in planned retirement age. The sample is the PARS dataset (998 observations, see the text and Table 1 for a description) further limited to the 840 observations with a valid planned retirement age in survey 1 and survey 2.

50

Table A-3: Changes in Planned Retirement Age and Social Security Claiming Age Planned Retirement Age

Planned Social Security Claiming Age

Earlier

Same

Later

Total

Earlier

24

50

15

89

Same

30

361

62

453

Later

12

88

86

186

Total

66

499

163

728

Notes: The sample is the PARS data (see Table 1 for a description) further limited to those observations with valid planned retirement and Social Security age responses in both Survey One and Survey Two (N= 728). The numbers in each cell represent the total number of respondents that had that planned retirement age and planned Social Security claiming age combination. The shaded boxes indicate movement in sync between Social Security claiming age and planned retirement age. “Earlier” indicates a change to an earlier age (i.e., where age as stated on Survey Two is less than the age stated on Survey One). “Same” indicates that the age on Survey One is the same as age on Survey Two. “Later” indicates a change to a later age (i.e., where the age on Survey Two is greater than the age stated on Survey One). Table A-4: Learning about Normal Social Security Eligibility and Changes in Planned Retirement Age and Social Security Claiming Age

Initial Response

No Change

Change Retirement Age Only

Too Low Too High

40 (35.4%) 28 (33.8%)

13 (11.5%) 7 (8.4%)

Change Social Security Claiming Age Only 31 (27.4%) 23 (27.7%)

Change Both

Total in Category

29 (25.7%) 25 (30.1%)

113 83

Notes: The sample is the PARS data (see Table 1 for a description) further limited to those observations with valid planned retirement and Social Security age responses in both Survey One and Survey Two (N= 728). The numbers in each cell represent the total number of respondents that had the respective planned retirement age and planned Social Security claiming age combination. The row percentages are in parentheses.

51

Table A-5: Full set of coefficients for results in Table 6, Panel A Any Change (1) 0.023*** (0.008) 0.268*** (0.097) 0.223*** (0.078) 0.225*** (0.064) 0.211*** (0.068) 0.099 (0.074) -0.055 (0.036) -0.083** (0.038) -0.001 (0.002) 0.024 (0.043) 0.066 (0.045) 0.059 (0.037) -0.137 (0.159) -0.005 (0.049) -0.002 (0.040) -0.146** (0.058) 0.013 (0.074) 0.018 (0.044) 0.028 (0.052) 0.084* (0.050) 998

Change in Aggregate Knowledge Score (0-10) Age 50-51 Age 52-54 Age 55-57 Age 58-60 Age 61-63 Male Married Tenure Some College Earnings of $50,000 or less Earnings of $100,001 or more Earnings Blank Defined Contribution Balance - Low (Less than 1 year salary) Defined Contribution Balance - High (More than 5 years salary) Defined Contribution Balance - Blank Employer B Employer C Employer D Employer E Observations

Notes: See Table 6.

52

Delay (2) 0.017** (0.008) 0.321*** (0.121) 0.303*** (0.103) 0.298*** (0.073) 0.251*** (0.082) 0.211** (0.092) 0.010 (0.032) -0.052 (0.035) -0.000 (0.002) -0.053 (0.043) -0.012 (0.040) 0.027 (0.035) 0.056 (0.185) 0.035 (0.045) -0.040 (0.035) -0.080 (0.054) 0.058 (0.072) -0.095** (0.037) -0.022 (0.046) 0.176*** (0.048) 840

Earlier (3) 0.001 (0.006) 0.096 (0.111) 0.085 (0.089) 0.119* (0.071) 0.136* (0.082) 0.056 (0.076) -0.067*** (0.025) -0.016 (0.026) -0.001 (0.001) 0.062*** (0.022) 0.091** (0.038) 0.045* (0.027) -0.022 (0.029) 0.015 (0.027) -0.064*** (0.023) -0.037 (0.032) 0.036 (0.035) 0.059 (0.041) -0.051** (0.025) 840

Golden Years or Financial Fears? How Plans Change ...

however, over the last four decades, there has been a significant shift toward defined contribution plans. In 2013, approximately half ... This research is based on participant surveys from 85 seminars that incorporated our surveys into their ...... Employer-Provided Financial Information: Hiring to Retiring.” American Economic.

940KB Sizes 0 Downloads 137 Views

Recommend Documents

Revolution or evolution: how will blockchain technology change ... - PwC
Sep 28, 2016 - Towards a blockchain breakthrough – tackling challenges. While technical challenges remain, rapid progress and investment in the technology has resulted in many problems already being solved. This progress is partly down to the open-

EPUB The Golden Years: 1956 - David Sandison - Book
My The Golden Years: 1956 Grandmother, Lucinda Rena Chandler was the daughter of. DOC Radical Graphics/Graphic Radicals - Laurel Harper - Book,PDF Nina Here Nor There: My Journey Beyond Gender - Nick Krieger - Book,EBOOK The Whole Hog: Exploring the

Read PDF These Happy Golden Years (Little House ...
... Wilder LibraryThing is a cataloging and social networking site for booklovers .... House) ,10 inch ereader These Happy Golden Years (Little House) ,e reader ... Years (Little House) ,top 10 ebook reader These Happy Golden Years (Little ...

1995/2004 Memos: Architect or PE Seal on Plans & Specifications.pdf ...
1995/2004 Memos: Architect or PE Seal on Plans & Specifications.pdf. 1995/2004 Memos: Architect or PE Seal on Plans & Specifications.pdf. Open. Extract.

Do Consumer-Driven Health Plans Drive Behavior Change?
Dec 26, 2013 - EBRI blog: https://ebriorg.wordpress.com/. Sign up for our RSS feeds! © 2013, Employee Benefit Research Institute, 1100 13th St. NW, ...

Do Consumer-Driven Health Plans Drive Behavior Change?
Dec 26, 2013 - Checked the price of a service before getting care (39 percent CDHP and 33 percent HDHP vs. 26 percent traditional). “Findings from the 2013 ...

Editorial Fifty years of change in education - OECD.org
population completes secondary education, one in three young adults has a ..... to attend less prestigious vocational schools, even when their performance on ...

Are Financial or Moral Scandals Worse? - Orion
A number of factors—such as differences in media coverage (Puglisi and Snyder 2008) or the ... presidential campaign included allegations of using campaign funds to buy ..... 10 Although the most grievous abuses of power may be financial (e.g., ...

This Hopes and Fears activity
Hopes and Fears Activity. The following activity is offered as one way to gauge stakeholders' perceptions and opinions of your existing or anticipated technology ...

Forty Years of Vegetation Change on the Missouri River Floodplain
lands, California) to compare pre- and postdam land-cover conditions. Digital ...... Rood SB, Gourley CR, Ammon EM, Heki LG, Klotz JR, Morrison ML,. Mosley D ...