Molecular Psychiatry (2002) 7, 706–711  2002 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 1359-4184/02 $25.00 www.nature.com/mp

ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE

Higher scores of self reported schizotypy in healthy young males carrying the COMT high activity allele D Avramopoulos1, NC Stefanis1, I Hantoumi1, N Smyrnis1,2, I Evdokimidis2 and CN Stefanis1 1 University Mental Health Research Institute, Argyrocastrou and Ionias St, Papagou, Athens 15669, Greece; 2Department of Neurology, National and Kapodestrian University of Athens, School of Medicine, Eginition Hospital, Vas Sofias 72, Athens 11528, Greece

The gene for COMT is located on chromosome 22q11, an area that has been implicated in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia through linkage studies and through the detection of deletions in schizophrenics and velocardiofacial syndrome patients that often present psychotic symptomatology. Additionally catechol-O-methyl transferase activity has been found increased in schizophrenia and a functional polymorphism in the COMT gene itself has been associated with the disease, as well as with aggression in patients. We tested the hypothesis that COMT genotype for the functional Val158Met might contribute to the variance of self reported schizotypy and aggression scores in the normal population. We genotyped 379 healthy 18- to 24year-old male individuals who had completed the PAS, SPQ and AQ questionnaires. Our results showed that self-reported schizotypy scores in both questionnaires were significantly related to COMT genotype (P = 0.028 for the PAS and P = 0.015 for the SPQ) with individuals homozygous for the high activity allele showing the highest scores. No significant differences were detected for AQ scores. We conclude that the COMT genotype for the functional Val158Met polymorphism is correlated to self-reported schizotypy in healthy males. This finding is in the same direction as reported findings on schizophrenia and it adds to the list of evidence that COMT or a nearby gene in linkage disequilibrium is involved in the pathogenesis of the disease. Molecular Psychiatry (2002) 7, 706–711. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001070 Keywords: catechol O-methyltransferase; genes; chromosomes; human; pair 22; questionnaires; schizotypy; aggression; schizophrenia

Introduction There are many lines of evidence suggesting the presence of a schizophrenia-related gene on the long arm of chromosome 22. Several linkage studies for schizophrenia have reported positive findings in the 22q area.1–6 Additionally velocardiofacial syndrome (OMIM No. 192430), a clinical entity characterized by cleft palate, cardiac anomalies, typical facies, learning disabilities, and a high rate of psychosis among patients and their relatives,7,8 is caused by a deletion at 22q11. Furthermore. Karayiorgou et al9 have identified 22q deletions in schizophrenic patients. 22q11.2 harbors the gene for catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), an enzyme that plays an important role in the metabolism of catecholamines including dopamine.10 The dopaminergic pathways have long been thought to play an important role in the pathogenic mechanisms of schizophrenia. Erythrocyte COMT activity has been studied in schizophrenic

Correspondence: CN Stefanis, University Mental Health Research Institute, Argyrocastrou and Ionias St, Papagou, Athens 15669, Greece. E-mail: iumhri얀compulink.gr Received 7 June 2001; revised 22 November 2001; accepted 17 December 2001

patients and was found elevated in some,11–13 though not all studies.14–16 COMT activity has been found to have a normal variability, with a trimodal distribution of low, intermediate and high levels.17,18 A G to A transition at codon 158 of the COMT gene, resulting in a valine-to-methionine substitution, has been identified and found to be associated with differential COMT activity19,20 and the two alleles have been termed low (Met158) and high (Val158) activity. Several studies have shown the high activity allele to be preferentially transmitted to the schizophrenic offspring,21–23 although other studies have found no association with the disease.24–26 Schizophrenia is a complex disorder with variable phenotypic expressions. Several studies confirm a strong genetic component but it appears most likely that many different genes are involved and environmental factors also contribute to the expression of the disease in those susceptible.27 Schizotypal personality often precedes the development of schizophrenia28 and it is considered by some to be a low end of the schizophrenia spectrum. It has also been found to be present in high frequency in schizophrenics’ relatives (reviewed by Webb et al29), suggesting a common genetic background with the disease. Although many studies have shown aggregation of schizotypal personality

Schizotypy scores associated with COMT genotype D Avramopoulos et al

in schizophrenics’ families, when positive or negative symptoms are examined there is lack of consistency in the findings.29–32 Nevertheless there have been reports of significant heritability for schizotypal personality33,34 and it has been suggested that positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia correlate with the corresponding symptoms in schizotypal relatives.35 It therefore seems likely, that schizotypal personality represents a condition attributable to some (but possibly not all) of the genes that predispose to schizophrenia, as it is a common but not a consistent premorbid finding. Schizotypal personality also appears to have a higher penetrance since it is often found in nonaffected family members. Both these characteristics, if true, may considerably augment the power of linkage and genetic association studies when schizotypy, which is the related continuous measurable trait, is the phenotype used and not the disease itself. Although there are definite criteria for the clinical diagnosis of schizotypal personality disorder, schizotypy is a trait present in the general population in a more or less continuous manner. Several questionnaires have been developed that assess self-reported schizotypy as a quantitative trait. Using a quantitative variable for an association study is an advantage, as it allows the study of its variation in relationship to the genotype, as opposed to measuring genotype frequencies in subgroups defined by arbitrary cut-off points. On the other hand ‘pencil and paper’ methods are often not considered very reliable, certainly not as reliable as clinical evaluation. Nevertheless the nature of these methods makes them very objective as scores are not assigned by the investigator but they are derived by a common for all subjects scoring system. In addition to this, they can be administered to large numbers of individuals far easier than clinical evaluation. The possibility of errors in the assessment due to the use of questionnaires is present and it could introduce random noise in the results, which could mask possible findings. This is balanced though by the potential to use larger samples. Taking into account these observations, we set out to test whether the genotype for the Val158Met functional polymorphism in the COMT gene on chromosome 22q11.2 is related to the schizotypy scores of normal subjects in two well established questionnaires, the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ)36 and the Perceptual Aberration Scale (PAS).37 The SPQ was used to measure overall schizotypy without accounting for both positive and negative symptoms. The PAS was used because it is a widely accepted scale and it has been shown to present a significant heritability and to give higher scores among schizophrenics’ relatives.33,38,39 Other scales were not used due to examination time limitations. Additionally, because a correlation between the COMT genotype and violence in Schizophrenic patients has been reported,40,41 we analyzed for the same subjects their scores for the Buss– Perry Aggression Questionnaire,42 in order to study possible associations of the COMT genotype with aggression in our non-clinical sample.

707

Materials and methods Subjects Our 379 unrelated subjects were a random portion of 2075, 18- to 24-year-old male airforce recruits, who participated in a larger study (the ASPIS, Stefanis et al, submitted for publication) which, among other cognitive and occulomotor tests, involved administration of the Perceptual Aberration Scale, the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire and the Aggression Questionnaire. The selection of only part of the sample was necessary for technical reasons and it was done in a random fashion, according to the last digit of an alphabetically assigned identification number. The Greek airforce recruits are an age-defined male population of Greek citizens. No other criteria except their personal preference and the needs of the airforce are used for their selection, although there is an observed tendency for them to be of a slightly higher socioeconomic status than ground forces recruits. The male only sex was a necessary restriction. This possibly limits the application of our findings, but it might also provide additional power in case there is a sex-specific effect, a possibility that is consistent with various findings in the schizophrenia literature. Our goals and procedures were explained to the subjects and written informed consent was acquired. Questionnaires The Perceptual Aberration Scale, the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire and the Aggression Questionnaire were translated into Greek by two independent bilingual translators and back translated by two other bilingual translators. The back translations were communicated to the authors of the questionnaires for review and were accepted as accurate. In addition four validity questions from the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI)43 were included and individuals found to respond unreliably were excluded from the analysis. DNA extraction DNA was extracted from mouthwash as follows: 15 ml of sterile saline solution was supplied and the subjects were instructed to perform a rigorous mouthwash for approximately 20 seconds. The sample was centrifuged at 1440 g for 15 min at 4°C. The pellet was resuspended in 2 ml Lysis buffer (2% SDS, 0.1 M NaCl, 0.05 M trisHCl (pH 8.0) 1 mM EDTA) and incubated at 37°C for 16 h with 2 mg of Proteinase K. Proteins were precipitated and removed with the addition of 1 ml of 6 M NaCl, and centrifugation at 2160 g for 20 min at 4°C. DNA was precipitated from the supernatant with ethanol. Mouthwash was chosen in order to obtain a better procedure acceptance rate and to avoid possible denials related to psychological profile (eg fear of the needle). It has been shown in large samples that there are no genotyping discrepancies between DNA obtained from blood and mouthwash.44

Molecular Psychiatry

Schizotypy scores associated with COMT genotype D Avramopoulos et al

708

COMT typing Typing was performed by PCR amplification, digestion with the restriction enzyme NlaIII and 3% agarose electrophoresis as described by Kirov et al.45 Agarose gels were scored independently by two researchers (DA and IH) blind to the schizotypy and aggression scores of the subjects. Alleles were assigned as L (low activity, NlaIII site present) and H (high activity, NlaIII site absent). Statistical analysis Basic statistics and regression analyses were performed for the three quantitative variables. The subjects were then assigned into three groups according to their genotype (Val/VAl (HH), Val/Met (LH) or Met/Met (LL)) and one way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), using the genotype as the grouping variable and the scores in the PAS, SPQ and AQ as dependent variables, was performed. Additionally, a chi-square test between genotypes of low and high 10% scorers was performed. Log transformation the Wilks test was also used in order to investigate the general effect of genotype on schizotypy. Statistical analyses were performed using the STATISTICA software (STATSOFT Inc, 1993).

Results Of the 379 individuals, 366 had valid scores (answered more than 90% of the questions) for the PAS, 365 for the SPQ and 334 for the AQ. The distribution of the scores was very close to normal for SPQ and AQ while it was skewed to the right for the PAS (in accordance to the distributions other groups have obtained for this questionnaire). The variances of the three groups were homogenous (Levene’s test). The means and standard deviations for the whole sample as well as for the genotype subgroups are shown on Table 1. Correlation analysis showed highly significant correlations between the three variables (PAS vs SPQ r = 0.64, PAS vs AQ r = 0.33, SPQ vs AQ r = 0.48, P ⬍ 0.001 in all cases). COMT genotyping showed that the two alleles, H and L were present at overall frequencies of 53.4% and 46.6% respectively. The three genotype frequencies were LL: 21.3% (81/379), LH: 50.4% (191/379), HH:

28.2% (107/379) not deviant from that expected for genotypes in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium. Higher mean scores were observed for all variables in individuals homozygous for the high activity allele (see Table 1 for means and SD for the three groups). Analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed that the differences observed for the PAS and SPQ scores between the three possible genotypes were statistically significant, while the differences observed in the AQ were not. More specifically HH homozygotes had significantly higher scores than LH heterozygotes (PAS: P = 0.005, SPQ: P = 0.024) and combined non-HH individuals (PAS: P = 0.012, SPQ: P = 0.006) in both questionnaires. HH homozygotes also had significantly higher scores for the SPQ than LL homozygotes (P = 0.006) while significance limits were not reached for the PAS, possibly due to the small number of individuals in the homozygosity groups. The differences between LL and LH individuals were not statistically significant. In order to investigate the global effect of genotype on schizotypy we also used a general linear model analysis. This analysis allowed us to examine the effect of genotype on both PAS and SPQ scores in a multivariate analysis. The rationale for this approach was that both PAS and SPQ actually measure a single underlying trait of schizotypy and they are highly correlated (Pearson r = 0.65; P ⬍ 0.001). By using this analysis we could get an estimate of the total effect of genotype on schizotypy (measured with two independent variables, PAS and SPQ) and also estimate the specific effects of genotype on each one of these variables. We used a general linear model analysis where genotype was the single categorical predictor variable and logPAS and SPQ were the dependent variables. Log PAS was used in order to normalize the distribution of PAS scores which was, as mentioned above, skewed. Four subjects’ scores could not be used as they were 0 for PAS and could not be log transformed. It must be noted that two of those were in the HH group and two in the HL group and therefore their omission had a small positive effect on the reported values for this analysis. The Wilks test for the general effect of genotype in this model was highly significant (F = 4.20, P ⬍ 0.002). The model r for the specific effect of genotype on logPAS was 0.18, and the r2 was 0.034, indicat-

Table 1 Descriptive data and ANOVA for the three questionnaires for the whole sample and in relation to COMT genotype (LL, LH, HH) Mean ± SD (n)

Mean LL ± SD (n)

Mean HL ± SD (n)

Mean HH ± SD (n)

F (P)

PAS

7.55 ± 5.07 (366)

7.61 ± 5.74 (77)

6.95 ± 4.8 (186)

8.61 ± 4.88 (103)

F = 3.60 (P = 0.028)*

SPQ

28.06 ± 12.07 (365)

25.81 ± 12.37 (77)

27.48 ± 12.02 (185)

30.77 ± 11.57 (103)

F = 4.22 (P = 0.015)*

AQ

81.84 ± 16.09 (334)

79.72 ± 15.27 (74)

81.77 ± 16.95 (166)

83.65 ± 15.07 (94)

F = 1.23 (P = ns)

SD, standard deviation; n, number of observations; *difference significant below the 0.05 level (ANOVA); ns, not significant. Molecular Psychiatry

Schizotypy scores associated with COMT genotype D Avramopoulos et al

ing that genotype accounted for 3.4% of logPAS variance (regression ANOVA, F = 6.3, P ⬍ 0.002). The model r for the specific effect of genotype on SPQ was 0.17 and the r2 was 0.028 indicating that genotype accounted for 2.8% of SPQ variance (regression ANOVA, F2 = 5.2, P ⬍ 0.006). Thus the multivariate analysis using the general linear model confirmed that both PAS and SPQ were independently and equally affected by genotype and these effects were highly significant. In order to further test our result, we also used the reverse approach, comparing frequencies of homozygotes for each allele among high and low scoring groups. SPQ high scorers were found to be significantly more often homozygous for the HH allele (HH:15, HL:20, LL:4) than low scorers (HH:7, HL:20, LL:14) (␹2 = 8.38 between homozygotes, P = 0.004; ␹2 overall = 8.42, P = 0.015). The same was true for PAS (high scorers: HH:14, HL:15, LL:9; low scorers: HH:5, HL:26, LL:12; ␹2 = 3.88 between homozygotes, P = 0.049; ␹2 = 7.4 overall, P = 0.025). The subgroup of high scorers in the PAS or SPQ did not show any significant differences for AQ scores among genotypes. This analysis was performed because of the reported findings of higher aggression associated with the L allele in schizophrenics. This result should be considered inconclusive, as the number of individuals for this analysis was very small.

Discussion It has been proposed by many that in order to solve the schizophrenia puzzle, it might be fruitful to examine phenotypes that are strongly related to the disease and appear in high frequencies in relatives of the patients. These traits, termed endophenotypes might be associated to fewer genes and seem to present a higher penetrance than the disease itself, providing better targets for genetic studies. A number of such endophenotypes have been proposed, such as differences in cognitive and occulomotor functions, inhibition of the P50 evoked potentials etc.46 Some of these, like eye tracking deficits and P50 evoked potentials inhibition deficits, have already been used for linkage analysis and provided positive results (chromosome 6p21–23,47 and chromosome 15: alpha-7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor gene locus,48 respectively). Also Myles-Worsley et al,49 using both antisaccade and P50 as a combined inhibitory phenotype, found evidence of linkage on chromosome 22q, close to the COMT gene locus. Interestingly positive linkage findings in all these regions have also been reported for schizophrenia.50,51 Schizotypal personality often precedes schizophrenia and is also present in high frequency in schizophrenics’ relatives.29 Therefore schizotypy which is the relevant quantifiable personality trait shares some of the same properties with endophenotypes. Nevertheless, so far there are no reported studies attempting to associate schizotypy with candidate genes, or to use it for linkage analysis. There are many studies linking schizophrenia with

the COMT gene locus, the COMT functional polymorphism, and COMT activity as we mentioned in more detail in our introduction. We report here an association between the COMT functional polymorphism and self-reported schizotypy as measured by two wellestablished questionnaires. Our result is in agreement with the previous positive findings on schizophrenia, adding to the list of positive findings associating COMT with the disease. Nevertheless other studies have failed to find association and linkage between COMT and schizophrenia. The differences we observe here as well as the reported linkage and association results of others with schizophrenia indicate that the effect of the COMT genotype although present is not very strong. A weaker effect is easy to miss in association studies and hard to detect in linkage studies, depending greatly on the family panel that is used. The most likely scenario for schizophrenia is that more than one gene with variable contributions is involved in its pathogenesis.27 The large number of positive findings to which we contribute here, make it likely that COMT, or a nearby gene in linkage disequilibrium with this polymorphism, is one of them. Further verification of these results will be necessary. Multiple well studied established associations of genes with schizophrenia could lead to a better understanding of the disease and the candidate pathways for pharmacological intervention and possibly the development of a polymorphism panel with a significant predictive value. Our data indicate that the COMT genotype is associated with schizotypy scores in both questionnaires used. The SPQ, a questionnaire that shows a normal distribution in the sample, shows higher scores for HH homozygotes, intermediate scores for HL heterozygotes and lower schizotypy scores for LL homozygotes. Additionally, the top 10% scorers in both questionnaires have a significantly higher frequency of the HH genotype than the bottom 10% scorers. Although the PAS questionnaire shows higher scores for HH homozygotes, heterozygotes show lower scores (yet not significantly) from LL homozygotes. The sample is not large enough to clearly establish the significance of this unexpected inter-group variation, and the two questionnaires are not measuring exactly the same parameter, therefore conclusions cannot be drawn from this observation. It appears that PAS scores behave in a somewhat more complicated way than SPQ scores in relation to phenotype, a finding that could be related to the fact that PAS scores demonstrate a skewed distribution towards low scores. Nevertheless it is a consistent finding in both questionnaires, that HH homozygotes show higher schizotypy scores. We report here performing ANOVA for three different ‘paper and pencil’ phenotypes using the COMT genotype as an independent variable. Additional comparisons have also been performed without positive results using our data from cognitive and occulomotor tests. The significance level of our finding cannot withstand correction for multiple comparisons and should therefore be interpreted with caution and as an adjunct to the existing literature on the subject. Caution is also

709

Molecular Psychiatry

Schizotypy scores associated with COMT genotype D Avramopoulos et al

710

necessary as positive association results are often due to population admixture and stratification. Our population is one of Greek nationals selected with the criterion of age. The vast majority of male Greek nationals serve in the Greek armed forces and there are no set criteria for assignment in the airforce. Therefore our sample represents well the Greek population of this age stratum. Although as with most European populations, a certain extent of admixture is possible, it is unlikely that it is large enough to account for our results. Regarding the reported association between the low affinity allele and aggressive and antisocial behavior in schizophrenics, our study did not detect any significant differences in aggression scores between genotypes in our sample. The small, but not significant difference that was present, was in the opposite direction than that reported. Additionaly no difference was observed in aggression scores between genotypes among high scorers (10%) in schizotypy (although this subgroup is too small to allow for conclusions). Therefore, the reported finding might be confined to patients and not be present in our non-clinical sample. The dopamine hypothesis for schizophrenia suggests that there is an excess of dopamine activity associated with the disease. COMT is an enzyme that is involved in the O-methylation and degradation of catecholamines including dopamine. Finding the high activity allele to be associated with schizophrenia might seem to be opposite to what one would expect. Ours, and others’ experimental results are however consistent regarding the direction of the association. Dopamine regulation does not occur through the same mechanisms in all brain areas. COMT appears to be of greater importance in dopamine activity regulation in the prefrontal cortex,52,53 in areas involved in cognitive functions. Cognitive function is enhanced by dopamine as shown by COMT knockout mice,54 while schizophrenics and their relatives have been consistently shown to perform poorly in cognitive tasks.55–58 Therefore, neurophysiological evidence is also consistent with high COMT activity in schizophrenia.59 In conclusion, our data indicate that the COMT Vall58Met functional polymorphism is associated with self-reported schizotypy scores in the normal male population as those were assessed by two different questionnaires, adding to the long list of findings showing a relationship between COMT and schizophrenic spectrum disorders. Acknowledgements This work was supported by GSRT grant 97EKBAN107 to CNS.

References 1 Pulver AE, Karayiorgou M, Wolyniec PS, Lasseter VK, Kasch L, Nestadt, G et al. Sequential strategy to identify a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia: report of potential linkage on chromosome 22q12–q13.1: Part 1. Am J Med Genet 1994; 54: 36–43. 2 Coon H, Jensen S, Holik J, Hoff M, Myles-Worsley M, Reimherr F et al. Genomic scan for genes predisposing to schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet 1994; 54: 59–71. Molecular Psychiatry

3 Lasseter VK, Pulver AE, Wolyniec PS, Nestadt G, Meyers D, Karayiorgou M et al. Follow-up report of potential linkage for schizophrenia on chromosome 22q: Part 3. Am J Med Genet 1995; 60: 172–173. 4 Gill M, Vallada H, Collier D, Sham P, Holmans P, Murray R et al. A combined analysis of D22S278 marker alleles in affected sibpairs: support for a susceptibility locus for schizophrenia at chromosome 22q12. Schizophrenia Collaborative Linkage Group (Chromosome 22). Am J Med Genet 1996; 67: 40–45. 5 Wildenauer DB, Hallmayer J, Schwab SG, Albus M, Eckstein GN, Zill P et al. Searching for susceptibility genes in schizophrenia by genetic linkage analysis. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 1996; 61: 845–850. 6 Shaw SH, Kelly M, Smith AB, Shields G, Hopkins PJ, Loftus J et al. A genome-wide search for schizophrenia susceptibility genes. Am J Med Genet 1998; 81: 364–376. 7 Pulver AE, Nestadt G, Goldberg R, Shprintzen RJ, Lamacz M, Wolyniec PS et al. Psychotic illness in patients diagnosed with velocardio-facial syndrome and their relatives. J Nerv Ment Dis 1994; 182: 476–478. 8 Murphy KC, Jones LA, Owen MJ. High rates of schizophrenia in adults with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56: 940–945. 9 Karayiorgou M, Morris MA, Morrow B, Shprintzen RJ, Goldberg R, Borrow J et al. Schizophrenia susceptibility associated with interstitial deletions of chromosome 22q11. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1995; 92: 7612–7616. 10 Axelrod J. Methylation reactions in the formation and metabolism of catecholamines and other biogenic amines. Pharmacol Rev 1966; 18: 95–113. 11 Matthysse S, Baldessarini RJ. S-adenosylmethionine and catecholO-methyl-transferase in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1972; 128: 1310–1312. 12 Poitou P, Assicot M, Bohuon C. Soluble and membrane catecholo-methyl transferases in red blood cells of schizophrenic patients. Biomedicine 1974; 21: 91–93. 13 White HL, McLeod MN, Davidson JR. Catechol O-methyltransferase in red blood cells of schizophrenic, depressed, and normal human subjects. Br J Psychiatry 1976; 128: 184–187. 14 Ebstein R, Belmaker RH, Benbenisty D, Rimon R. Electrophoretic pattern of red blood cell catechol-o-methyltransferase in schizophrenia and manic-depressive illness. Biol Psychiatry 1976; 11: 613–623. 15 Groshong R, Baldessarini RJ, Gibson DA, Lipinski JF, Axelrod D, Pope A. Activities of types A and B MAO and catechol-o-methyltransferase in blood cells and skin fibroblasts of normal and chronic schizophrenic subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35: 1198–1205. 16 Baron M, Gruen R, Levitt M, Hunter C, Asnis L. Erythrocyte catechol O-methyltransferase activity in schizophrenia: analysis of family data. Am J Psychiatry 1984; 141: 29–32. 17 Weinshilboum RM, Raymond FA. Inheritance of low erythrocyte catechol-o-methyltransferase activity in man. Am J Hum Genet 1977; 29: 125–135. 18 Spielman RS, Weinshilboum RM. Genetics of red cell COMT activity: analysis of thermal stability and family data. Am J Med Genet 1981; 10: 279–290. 19 Lotta T, Vidgren J, Tilgmann C, Ulmanen I, Melen K, Julkunen I et al. Kinetics of human soluble and membrane-bound catechol Omethyltransferase: a revised mechanism and description of the thermolabile variant of the enzyme. Biochemistry 1995; 34: 4202–4210. 20 Lachman HM, Papolos DF, Saito T, Yu YM, Szumlanski CL, Weinshilboum RM. Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics 1996; 6: 243–250. 21 Li T, Sham PC, Vallada H, Xie T, Tang X, Murray RM et al. Preferential transmission of the high activity allele of COMT in schizophrenia. Psychiatr Genet 1996; 6: 131–133. 22 Kunugi H, Vallada HP, Sham PC, Hoda F, Arranz MJ, Li T et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphisms and schizophrenia: a transmission disequilibrium study in multiply affected families. Psychiatr Genet 1997; 7: 97–101. 23 Li T, Ball D, Zhao J, Murray RM, Liu X, Sham PC et al. Family-

Schizotypy scores associated with COMT genotype D Avramopoulos et al

24 25

26

27 28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36 37 38

39

40

41

42 43

based linkage disequilibrium mapping using SNP marker haplotypes: application to a potential locus for schizophrenia at chromosome 22q11. Mol Psychiatry 2000; 5: 77–84. Wei J, Hemmings GP. Lack of evidence for association between the COMT locus and schizophrenia. Psychiatr Genet 1999; 9: 183–186. Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Galke BL, Wolyniec PS, Nestadt G, Antonarakis SE et al. Identification of sequence variants and analysis of the role of the catechol-O-methyl-transferase gene in schizophrenia susceptibility. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 43: 425–431. Strous RD, Bark N, Woerner M, Lachman HM. Lack of association of a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase gene polymorphism in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41: 493–495. Risch N. Genetic linkage and complex diseases, with special reference to psychiatric disorders. Genet Epidemiol 1990; 7: 3–16. Tyrka AR, Cannon TD, Haslam N, Mednick SA, Schulsinger F, Schulsinger H et al. The latent structure of schizotypy: I. Premorbid indicators of a taxon of individuals at risk for schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 1995; 104: 173–183. Webb CT, Levinson DF. Schizotypal and paranoid personality disorder in the relatives of patients with schizophrenia and affective disorders: a review. Schizophr Res 1993; 11: 81–92. Maier W, Lichtermann D, Minges J, Heun R. Personality disorders among the relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull 1994; 20: 481–493. Kremen WS, Faraone SV, Toomey R, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT. Sex differences in self-reported schizotypal traits in relatives of schizophrenic probands. Schizophr Res 1998; 34: 27–37. Yaralian PS, Raine A, Lencz T, Hooley JM, Bihrle SE, Mills S et al. Elevated levels of cognitive-perceptual deficits in individuals with a family history of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res 2000; 46: 57–63. Lyons MJ, Toomey R, Faraone SV, Kremen WS, Yeung AS, Tsuang MT. Correlates of psychosis proneness in relatives of schizophrenic patients. J Abnorm Psychol 1995; 104: 390–394. Torgersen S, Lygren S, Oien PA, Skre I, Onstad S, Edvardsen J et al. A twin study of personality disorders. Compr Psychiatry 2000; 41: 416–425. Fanous A, Gardner C, Walsh D, Kendler KS. Relationship between positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia and schizotypal symptoms in nonpsychotic relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58: 669–673. Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull 1991; 17: 555–564. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Raulin ML. Scales for physical and social anhedonia. J Abnorm Psychol 1976; 85: 374–382. Coleman MJ, Levy DL, Lenzenweger MF, Holzman PS. Thought disorder, perceptual aberrations, and schizotypy. J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 105: 469–473. Laurent A, Biloa-Tang M, Bougerol T, Duly D, Anchisi AM, Bosson JL et al. Executive/attentional performance and measures of schizotypy in patients with schizophrenia and in their nonpsychotic firstdegree relatives. Schizophr Res 2000; 46: 269–283. Strous RD, Bark N, Parsia SS, Volavka J, Lachman HM. Analysis of a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase gene polymorphism in schizophrenia: evidence for association with aggressive and antisocial behavior. Psychiatry Res 1997; 69: 71–77. Lachman HM, Nolan KA, Mohr P, Saito T, Volavka J. Association between catechol O-methyltransferase genotype and violence in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155: 835–837. Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol 1992; 63: 452–459. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A psychobiological model

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

59

of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50: 975–990. de Vries HG, Collee JM, van Veldhuizen MH, Achterhof L, Smit Sibinga CT, Scheffer H et al. Validation of the determination of deltaF508 mutations of the cystic fibrosis gene in over 11 000 mouthwashes. Hum Genet 1996; 97: 334–336. Kirov G, Murphy KC, Arranz MJ, Jones I, McCandles F, Kunugi H et al. Low activity allele of catechol-O-methyltransferase gene associated with rapid cycling bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry 1998; 3: 342–345. Adler LE, Freedman R, Ross RG, Olincy A, Waldo MC. Elementary phenotypes in the neurobiological and genetic study of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46: 8–18. Arolt V, Lencer R, Nolte A, Muller-Myhsok B, Purmann S, Schurmann M et al. Eye tracking dysfunction is a putative phenotypic susceptibility marker of schizophrenia and maps to a locus on chromosome 6p in families with multiple occurrence of the disease. Am J Med Genet 1996; 67: 564–579. Freedman R, Coon H, Myles-Worsley M, Orr-Urtreger A, Olincy A, Davis A et al. Linkage of a neurophysiological deficit in schizophrenia to a chromosome 15 locus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997; 94: 587–592. Myles-Worsley M, Coon H, McDowell J, Brenner C, Hoff M, Lind B et al. Linkage of a composite inhibitory phenotype to a chromosome 22q locus in eight Utah families. Am J Med Genet 1999; 88: 544–550. Schwab SG, Albus M, Hallmayer J, Honig S, Borrmann M, Lichtermann D et al. Evaluation of a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia on chromosome 6p by multipoint affected sib-pair linkage analysis. Nat Genet 1995; 11: 325–327. Riley BP, Makoff A, Mogudi-Carter M, Jenkins T, Williamson R, Collier D et al. Haplotype transmission disequilibrium and evidence for linkage of the CHRNA7 gene region to schizophrenia in Southern African Bantu families. Am J Med Genet 2000; 96: 196– 201. Karoum F, Chrapusta SJ, Egan MF. 3-Methoxytyramine is the major metabolite of released dopamine in the rat frontal cortex: reassessment of the effects of antipsychotics on the dynamics of dopamine release and metabolism in the frontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, and striatum by a simple two pool model. J Neurochem 1994; 63: 972–979. Gogos JA, Morgan M, Luine V, Santha M, Ogawa S, Pfaff D et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998; 95: 9991–9996. Kneavel M, Gogos JA, Karayiorgou M, Luine V. Interaction of COMT gene deletion and environment on cognition. Soc Neurosci Abstracts 2000; 26: 571.20. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zec RF. Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. I. Regional cerebral blood flow evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43: 114–124. Carter CS, Perlstein W, Ganguli R, Brar J, Mintun M, Cohen JD. Functional hypofrontality and working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155: 1285–1287. Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, Langheim FJ, Duyn J, Coppola R et al. Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10: 1078–1092. Park S, Holzman PS, Goldman-Rakic PS. Spatial working memory deficits in the relatives of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52: 821–828. Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Mazzanti CM, Straub RE et al. Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001; 98: 6917–6922.

711

Molecular Psychiatry

Higher scores of self reported schizotypy in healthy ...

used to measure overall schizotypy without accounting for both positive and negative ... the STATISTICA software (STATSOFT Inc, 1993). Results. Of the 379 .... adding to the list of positive findings associating COMT with the disease.

107KB Sizes 0 Downloads 162 Views

Recommend Documents

Self-reported musculoskeletal problems amongst ...
Department of Human Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough, .... Kuwano and Namba 1985) and musculoskeletal stresses (e.g. McGill and ..... PORTER, J.M., PORTER, C.S. and LEE, V.J.A., 1992, A survey of driver discomfort.

Trends in Self-Reported Spontaneous Abortions: 1970 ...
Growth (hereafter, NSFG), administered by the National Center for Health Statistics in 1988,. 1995 and 2002. ..... which could, in turn, incorrectly lead us to observe a relation between miscarriage and variables correlated .... There is no consisten

Bias, precision and heritability of self-reported and ... - Springer Link
Aug 25, 2006 - or interviews and are often cheaper and more readily available than alternatives. However, the precision and potential bias cannot usually be ...

Schizotypy and handedness in Japanese participants, revisited
Aug 19, 2008 - Although previous studies have suggested a relationship between mixed-handedness and schizotypic symptoms, possibly indicating a predisposition to schizophrenia, the participants involved were exclusively from Western cultures. Only tw

2015 Scores in 30s.pdf
08/04/15 Warren, Mike 4 4 5 5 5 3 4 3 4 37. 08/11/15 Brown, Keith 5 4 4 5 4 4 5 2 4 37. 08/11/15 Krafft, Jay 4 4 4 4 5 6 5 2 4 38. 08/11/15 Rieves, Dave 4 4 3 5 5 ...

Investigation and Treatment of Missing Item Scores in Test and ...
May 1, 2010 - This article first discusses a statistical test for investigating whether or not the pattern of missing scores in a respondent-by-item data matrix is random. Since this is an asymptotic test, we investigate whether it is useful in small

Patient reported outcome measures in hospice ...
symptoms and a 1-item (un)well-being measure on a 0-10 numerical scale. All USD ... using Chi Square for categorical data and independent Students T test or ...

5. REPORTED SPEECH.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. 5. REPORTED ...

pdf-1371\living-a-healthy-life-with-chronic-conditions-self ...
... the apps below to open or edit this item. pdf-1371\living-a-healthy-life-with-chronic-conditions- ... thritis-stroke-diabetes-asthma-bronchitis-emphysema.pdf.

Test Scores, Self Efficacy, and the Educational Plans of ...
scores on the self efficacy of students affects the educational plans of college freshmen. .... higher plans to obtain a graduate or professional degree than their.

Higher Weights and Graded Rings for Binary Self-Dual Codes
5.1 A Gleason-type Theorem. In this subsection, we shall use the previous results to produce a Gleason-type theorem for binary self-dual codes. Let C be a self-dual Type I code and Wr(C;x, y) its symmetric higher weight enumerator. Consider the polyn

Higher Weights and Binary Self-Dual Codes
[email protected] and. T. Aaron Gulliver. Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering. University of Victoria. P.O. Box 3055, STN CSC. Victoria, BC, Canada V8W 3P6 [email protected]. June 22, 2011. Abstract. The theory of higher wei

Higher Weights for Ternary and Quaternary Self-Dual ...
Jun 22, 2011 - C (1,0,0,0,0, y,0,0,0,0, y,0,0,0,0,y). − J. (2). C (1,0,0,0,0,0, y,0,0,0,0, y,0, y,0,0). − J. (2). C (1,0,0,0,0,0,0, y,0, y,0,0,0,0, y,0) + 4). 3 Type III and Type IV Codes. In this section, we consider the higher weights of ternar

Effect of Schizotypy on Cognitive Performance and Its ...
spatial n-back tests; and cognitive performance speed, which reflects a reduction of the ..... scription of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to.

Reported Speech Exercises.pdf
Al Gore said: “As a teenager, one of the books that I read was Silent Spring.” 17. “I was taught information technology in this course,” she said. 18. My aunt said: ...

5. REPORTED SPEECH.pdf
REPORTED SPEECH.pdf. 5. REPORTED SPEECH.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu. Displaying 5. REPORTED SPEECH.pdf. Page 1 of 3.

Reported Statements exercises.pdf
Reported Statements exercises.pdf. Reported Statements exercises.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu. Displaying Reported Statements ...

Incident reported - Islington Council - Democracy
1.1 This is an application by the Metropolitan Police Service for a Review of the .... been meetings between the Management team of Fabric and the Senior ...

Airport self check self check self check-in
then the admin will update its details in the panel and if the flight gets .... Microsoft SQL Server 2005 is comprehensive, integrated data management and ...

Children in reported 'missing' to the police
We are the blue light social services in the fact that we're 24/7 and we are ... 140. 160. 0. 200. 400. 600. 800. 1000. 1200. Jan. Fe b. Ma rch. Ap ril. Ma y. Ju ne.