Instant Messaging Social Networks: Individual, Relational, and Cultural Characteristics Gustavo S. Mesch, Ilan Talmud (presenter) University of Haifa, and Anabel Quan-Haase University of Western Ontario,

Abstract Prevalent research on information and communication technologies (ICTs) tends to focus on either individual or group level characteristics, while neglecting to investigate the effect of relational variables on communication. We collected survey data in Israel (N = 492) and Canada (N = 293) to investigate the effect of individual, relational and cultural variables on frequency and topic multiplexity of Instant Messaging communication among dyads of university students.

Online communication and time spent online on socializing activities Seems to increase the closeness of existing social ties, supplementing face-to-face communication with close friends, distant friends, and family Links intimate friends who are temporarily geographically separated, as they take jobs or enroll in college Provides an alternative channel, filling intervals between face-to-face meetings and facilitating the coordination of social gatherings (QuanHaase, 2008).

Instant Messaging Most studies have focused on the effects of individual-level variables. The average IM session is longer for females than for males, because in part females use IM to establish, nurture, and develop personal relationships, while males use it for providing information (Baron, 2004).

IM use has a narrower range of purposes. It promotes rather than hinders intimacy, with frequent IM conversations encouraging the desire to meet face-toface with friends (Hu et al., 2004). Main uses of IM are for socializing, event planning, task accomplishment, and meeting new people (Flanagin, 2005; Grinter & Pallen, 2002; Walther, 1996).

IM for Relations Bringing Relations Back in Little relational or comparative research is available in Israel, IM is their most frequent Internet activity, with 77% of high school students reporting using IM on a daily basis. IM use was more popular with girls than boys, 70% and 64% respectively (Lemish, Ribak, & Alony, 2009).

IM differs from other digital textual communication channels such as email, SMS, and chat rooms. First, IM users generally engage in messaging with others whom they know. One-to-one and small group chats characterize IM's use in peer groups and the workplace, where IM is considered an essential communication tool (Grinter & Palen, 2002). IM communication was mostly restricted to one's "real space friends," people who first met face-to-face in physical settings such as schools or summer camps (Grinter & Palen, 2002). IM use was associated with the perceived intimacy between friends (Hu et al., 2004).\ The amount of IM use was also positively associated with verbal, affective, and social intimacy. Indeed, frequent conversation via IM actually encouraged the desire to meet face-to-face.

Studies have shown that because of its inherent positive network externalities, the rate of adoption among peers has grown exponentially by sheer in-group influence. Hence, IM is well suited for the study of social ties.

Two Cases Comparison: Students in Canada and Israel Immigration absorption Size Federated polity State involvement and cultural legacy Ethnic and immigrant segregation Civil Society State of War + conscription

Type of Relationship RQ1: Does the communicators' type of relationship have an effect on the frequency and content of IM communications? H1: A tie’s perceived closeness will be positively associated with more frequent communication and with the inclusion of more topics of conversation.

Tie Homophily H2: Same-sex relationships will display more frequent communication, and on a larger number of topics, than cross-sex relationships.

Propinquity RQ2: Does propinquity affect the frequency and multiplicity of topics of IM communications?

Tie Duration H3: As engagement duration between communicators increases, relational strength intensifies. Hence, the longer the relationship duration, the higher the frequency of IM communication and the greater the number of topics discussed.

Method The data collection took place in Canada and Israel between October 2005 and August 2006 . Size: Canada N=293. Israel N= 492; Mean age: Ca = 21; ISR = 25. Female: Ca=68%, ISR = 51% Majority Social sciences. Across grade levels

Name Generator for two ties Dependent Variables (dyadic): Communication frequency for each pair (i, j) Topic multiplexity

Independent Variables Relational: Tie duration Perceived Closeness Relationship Type Individual (ego): Age, gender, marital status.

Because we used slightly different model specifications, we used the BIC as a modelselection test to complement our analytic strategy, and to examine the extent to which we lost parsimony at the expense of specificity. The BIC indicates the relative likelihood that a model fits a given data set. The calculation of BIC for the Ordinary Least Squares models is: BIC(q) = N [ln(1 - R2(q)] + p(q) [ln (N)] ; where N is the number of cases, and p is the

Relationship type with IM contacts Israel and Canada (N=293) First IM Contact

Second IM Contact

Israel _______

Canada _______

Israel ______

Canada _______

Family

11%

16%

10%

20%

Close Friend

48%

42%

44%

49%

Distant Friends

23%

12%

35%

16%

Romantic Partner

14%

21%

5%

6%

Online Tie

4%

1%

6%

1%

Conclusion Individual level characteristics, such as: age, gender, and marital status proved not to be associated with communication frequency in either Canada or Israel. The extent of IM contacts was positively associated with communication frequency for both contacts in Israel and Canada. The extent of daily IM interaction was also positively associated with communication frequency for both contacts in both countries. The larger the IM network of respondents, and the more frequently they interact via IM, the more likely they are to communicate frequently with their first and second contacts.

In Canada and Israel, if the residence of the contact was at the university dorms or in the same city, communication frequency was greater than in the cases of contacts residing in another city or in another country.

Distance clearly exerts a significant effect on communication frequency, despite IM’s negligible cost in comparison with land lines and cell phones. Relationship type influenced communication frequency: with a romantic partner it was greater than with a close friend. In contrast, IM communication with an online friend or family member was less frequent than with a close friend. IM communication with a distant friend was also less frequent than with a close friend. Results for relationship type showed a similar pattern for Israel and Canada. Surprise: gender homophily was not founded to have significant net effect in any model.

In both countries, we found that university students living in geographical proximity communicated more frequently and on more topics than students living at geographical distance did. Relationship type had an effect on patterns of IM communication: communication with close friends and romantic partners was more frequent than with distant friends and family ties. Perceived closeness was positively associated with comm. frequency and topic multiplexity. IM communication patterns reflect the type and nature of existing relationships between contacts rather than individual attributes and cultural characteristics of the

Future Research Same Relational patterns on two countries, but: Needs multi-cases, multi-level studies Needs real records rather then self reported evidence. Single source bias. From tie approach into whole network or partial network approach Needs comparison of various relational media Problem of media integration: multi-purpose

Table1. Relationship type with IM contacts Israel and Canada (N=293) First IM Contact

Second IM Contact

Israel _______

Canada _______

Israel ______

Canada _______

Family

11%

16%

10%

20%

Close Friend

48%

42%

44%

49%

Distant Friends

23%

12%

35%

16%

Romantic Partner

14%

21%

5%

6%

Online Tie

4%

1%

6%

1%

Table2. OLS regression predicting communication frequency with tie—Canada First Contact Model 1 ________________ -.03 -.066 (.026) -.079 -.032 (.122) .071 .031 (.124) -.171 -.075 (.140) .545** .237 (.116) .436** .319 (.067) .154* .135 (.060) .160 .017 (.470) .035 .012 (.162) -.918** -.286 (.173) .987** .354 (.195) -----------

Model 2 ____________ -.031 -.058 (.026) -.054 -.021 (.120) .087 .038 (.122) -.157 -.069 (.141) .555** .241 (.121) .418** .305 (.067) .148* .129 (.059) .220 .023 (.486) .162 .054 (.169) -.707** -.220 (.220) .876** .314 (.196) ---------

Constant

5.381** (.655)

-.106* (.054) .338* (.134) 4.106** (.950)

Adjusted R Square

.390

BIC F-Ratio

-82.261

Age Gender(1=male) Single Gender similarity Propinquity Frequency of IM use # of IM regular contacts Online Family Distant friend Partner Close friends (omitted category) Relationship duration Closeness to tie

**p < .01 *p < .05

Second Contact Model 3 ____________ -.014 -.028 (.028) -.125 -.052 (.129) -.038 -.017 (.120) -.026 -.011 (.138) .675** .305 (.124) .422** .320 (.072) .147* .134 (.064) .100 .011 (.496) .303 .111 (.163) -.488** -.175 (.169) .804** .173 (.279) ------------

Model 4 ____________ -.007 -.013 (.028) -.114 -.048 (.128) -.098 -.044 (.120) -.038 -.017 (.137) .706** .318 (.133) .418** .317 (.071) .138* .125 (.063) .407 .045 (.500) .320 .117 (.175) -.194 -.069 (.191) .676* .145 (.282) -------------

4.587** (.691)

-.033 (.061) .343** (.109) 3.015 (.860)

.407

.265

.287

-79.104 4.047

-27.47

-25.192 4.4

-.121 .169

-.038 .212

Table3. OLS regression predicting communication frequency with tie—Israel

Age Gender(1=male) Single Gender similarity Propinquity Frequency of IM use # of IM regular contacts Online Family Distant friend Partner Close friends (omitted category) Relationship duration Closeness to tie

First Contact Model 1 Model 2 ____________ ______________ -.011 -.028 -.011 -.028 (.020) (.020) -.075 -.030 -.069 -.027 (.123) (.121) .084 .033 .054 .021 (.123) (.121) .197 .078 .221 .088 (.135) (.133) .498** .196 .500** .197 (.122) (.120) .225** .185 .222** .183 (.062) (.061) .340** .263 .330** .256 (.066) (.065) -.421 -.058 -.126 -.017 (.364) (.369) -.316 -.077 -.325 -.079 (.223) (.220) -.631** -.213 -.434** -.146 (.154) (.162) .519** .147 .365* .103 (.195) (.198) -------------------

Constant

5.382** (.586)

-.118* (.060) .380** (.113) 3.818 (.741)

Adjusted R Square

.296

BIC F-Ratio

-104.02

**p < .01; *p < .05

Second Contact Model 3 Model 4 _____________ ____________ -.026 -.060 -.031 -.071 (.022) (.022) .001 .001 .021 .009 (.127) (.123) .177 .070 .156 .062 (.127) (.124) .011 .004 -.026 -.010 (.123) (.120) .507** .302 .505** .203 (.125) (.122) .196** .166 ..202** .170 (.064) (.062) .341** .261 .314** .240 (.069) (.068) -.456 -.070 -.032 -.005 (.344) (.349) -.101 -.025 -.126 -.031 (.216) (.210) -.577** -.221 -.269 -.103 (.142) (.156) .220 .044 .139 .028 (.258) (.252) ----------------------

-.110

5.744 (.640)

-.168* (.059) .444** (.104) 4.045 (.739)

.318

.258

.297

-107.364 7.857

-78.433

-92.604 13.57

.190

-.157 .254

Table 4. OLS regression predicting topic multiplexity of IM communication—Canada

Age Gender(1=male) Single Gender similarity Propinquity Frequency of IM use # of contacts Online Family Distant friend Partner Close friends (omitted category) Duration

First Contact Model 1 Model 2 _____________ ____________ -.071 -.057 -.082 -.066 (.072) (.70) -.096 -.560 -.599 -.102 (.334) (.326) .287 .053 .355 .066 (.340) (.332) -.118 -.633 -.338 -.063 (.385) (.382) -.078 -.421 .031 .006 (.319) (.329) .410* .128 .426* .133 (.185) (.181) .060 .023 .098 .037 (.164) (.160) -1.515 -.068 .076 .003 (1.293) (1.318) -.732 -.104 -1.138* -.161 (.446) (.459) -1.686** -.225 -.369 -.049 (.477) (.596) 1.799** .276 1.925** .295 (.537) (.532) ----------------------

Second Model 3 _____________ -.007 -.006 (.070) -.626* -.114 (.318) -.110 -.022 (.295) -.413 -.079 (.338) -.493 -.097 (.304) .716** .236 (.178) .088 .035 (.158) -2.835* -.135 (1.223) -1.32** -.210 (.402) -1.59** -.248 (.417) -.026 -.002 (.687) -----------

Contact Model 4 _____________ .005 .004 (.069) -.634* -.115 (.314) -.227 -.045 (.294) -.383 -.073 (.336) -.247 -.048 (.327) .701** .231 (.175) .055 .022 (.155) -1.948 -.093 (1.229) -1.49** -.238 (.430) -.831 -.129 (.470) -.182 -.017 (.691) --------------

4.303* (1.700)

.109 (.150) .829** (.267) -.088 (2.113)

Constant

6.996** (1.803)

.461** (.146) .841* (.364) .832 (2.579)

Adjusted R Square

.157

.202

.153

.183

BIC F-Ratio

12.671

7.917 7.857

13.843

14.656 5.172

Closeness to tie

**p < .01; *p < .05

.226 .180

.055 .222

Table 5. OLS regression predicting topic multiplexity of IM communication—Israel

Age Gender(1=male) Single Gender similarity Propinquity Frequency of IM use # of contacts Online Family Distant friend Partner Duration Closeness to tie Constant Adjusted R Square BIC F-Ratio

First Contact Model 1 Model 2 _______________ ___________ -.099 -.111 -.098* -.111 (.049) (.049) -.468 -.082 -.453 -.079 (.304) (.300) .151 .026 .085 .015 (.304) (.301) -.006 -.001 .046 .008 (.334) (.330) -.122 -.021 -.120 -.021 (.302) (.298) .521** .188 .514** .186 (.153) (.151) .319 .109 .294 .100 (.162) (.160) -1.976* -.118 -1.325 -.079 (.903) (.919) -1.276* -.136 -1.296* -.138 (.553) (.546) -1.065* -.158 -.627 -.093 (.378) (.402) 1.090* .135 .752 .093 (.484) (.491) .006 .003 (.148) .838** .184 (.281) 6.828** (1.454) .166 .186

4.738** (1.666) .206

-20.869

-44.977

**p < .01; *p < .05 

-20.28 5.882

Second Model 3 ____________ -.061 -.064 (.052) -.836** -.152 (.290) -.099 -.018 (.292) .072 .013 (.289) .072 .013 (.289) .295* .112 (.149) .406* .140 (.184) -2.06** -.144 (.787) -1.84** -.207 (.494) -1.59** -.276 (.333) -.602 -.054 (.604)

Contact Model 4 _____________ -.072 -.075 (.051) -.802** -.146 (.285) -.123 -.022 (.287) .083 .015 (.279) .068 .012 (.284) .324* .123 (.146) .375* .130 (.181) -1.307 -.091 (.803) -1.90** -.214 (.485) -1.059 -.183 (.362) -.726 -.065 (.594) .227 .095 (.139) .858** .223 (.248)

.234 -50.292 8.75

IM Social Networks: Individual, Relational, and Cultural ...

spent online on socializing activities. Seems to increase the ... between face-to-face meetings and facilitating .... Needs comparison of various relational media.

2MB Sizes 3 Downloads 141 Views

Recommend Documents

Individual and Cultural Diversity 300:401, Section 04 ...
ABOUT THE COURSE. This course is designed to provide an overview of the ways that individual identity, cultural practice, social power relationships, ...

Cultural Dynamics, Social Mobility and Urban Segregation∗
Feb 2, 2017 - their child benefits from larger peer effects in education, provided that ...... technology, socialization cost and peer effects, there exists a set of ..... Resurgence of Immigrant Segregation”, Review of Economics and Statistics, vo

Generalised Blockmodelling of Social and Relational ...
Mar 28, 2014 - and abnormal communications between communities and external actors. This allows normal and abnormal external communication patterns ...

Occupational mismatch and social networks
May 13, 2013 - high, networks provide good matches at a higher rate than the formal .... and for a sufficiently high homophily level social networks pay a ...... The discount rate δ is set to 0.988 which corresponds to a quarterly interest rate.

Social Networks and Research Output
Empirical strategy: role of social networks. • How much can prediction be .... 4.54% .14∗∗. 2-Coauthors prod. .32 .731. 3.62% .10∗∗. Top 1% coauthor .31 .738.

Social Networks and Research Output
Aim: Assess whether networks have explanatory power? Doe they ... Two roles for the network. • Conduit for ideas: Communication in the course of research ...

Individual bounded response of social choice functions
May 30, 2016 - On the universal domain of preferences, it is hard to find a nonmanipulability condition which leads to a possibility result. ∗. This research is ...

Social Status in Small Groups: Individual-Group Similarity and the ...
Social Status in Small Groups: Individual-Group. Similarity ... requisite for high social status and is predicted to be consistently correlated with status across peer.

Individual-Group Similarity and the Social "Misfit"
Mary Giammarino. Harry W. Parad. Harvard University ... Gottman et al., 1975; Green, Forehand, Beck & Vosk, 1980), yet the relation between negative ...

Optimal Taxation and Social Networks
Nov 1, 2011 - We study optimal taxation when jobs are found through a social network. This network determines employment, which workers may influence ...

Individual-Group Similarity and the Social "Misfit"
Wediko Children's Services, Boston, Massachusetts ... studies, employing both acceptance and rejection measures of status, provide support for the proposed.

gender, social networks and performance
Oct 20, 2017 - 1 rule of business. Sallie Krawcheck ... In turn, earnings of both executives and financial managers are largely based on ..... focuses on dyadic relationship in the classroom with 5 classes of 25-35 students each. Benenson ...

Individual Giving to Support Cultural Heritage
available to the arts and culture sector. Therefore, from a policy .... giving, in addition to caring about the public ..... ally low (European Commission, 2009). We.

Blockchain meets Social Networks - Longcatchain
Jan 15, 2018 - The platform also provides custom analytic and monitoring capabilities for blockchain operations and enterprises. Users can run custom queries on social network data to get desired insights, find influencers, see the reach of their pos

social and cultural anthropology the key concepts pdf
File: Social and cultural anthropology. the key concepts pdf. Download now. Click here if your download doesn't start automatically. Page 1 of 1. social and cultural anthropology the key concepts pdf. social and cultural anthropology the key concepts

Social Presence and Cultural Presence In Oblivion
clouds, wild plants and animals, rocks and minerals, and landforms” ([35] ..... music could gradually change (slow down or stop playing) this may also add to the ...