Interpreting motor adaptation results within the framework of optimal feedback control Emanuel Todorov, Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego While motor adaptation experiments provide valuable and potentially very informative data, the interpretation of that data is often more challenging than its gathering. This is because, by definition, adaptation is a change in the state of the underlying sensori-motor control mechanisms. Therefore we cannot talk about adaptation without assuming – either explicitly or implicitly – a model of how those underlying mechanisms function in their “steady state”, irrespective of adaptation. Presently, the most common assumption is that motor control is based on the execution of detailed desired trajectories. Whenever a systematic perturbation is introduced experimentally, the motor system is thought to oppose the perturbation so that the resulting trajectories return to baseline. This is not always the case, but, in the absence of a clear alterative, inconsistencies are usually dealt with by patching the desired trajectory hypothesis with hypothetical internal limitations. We have recently argued that desired trajectory tracking is inappropriate as a general framework of motor control, because it contradicts a wide range of phenomena related to the coordination of redundant actuators, and furthermore results in suboptimal performance. The alternative theory we have proposed is based on stochastic optimal control1. The purpose of this talk is to extend our theory to the process of motor adaptation, and apply it to the task of reaching. We propose that the baseline feedback control law is being adapted so as to become optimal for the new dynamic environment. While complete re-optimization may be impossible within a single training session, and approximatons may be unavoidable, such an idealized model should predict the “direction” of behavioral change (if not its magnitude). Our underlying model of reaching, derived from1, will be introduced first. That model continuously estimates the state of the plant (by optimally integrating delayed noisy feedback, prior control signals, and knowledge of plant dynamics), and maps the instantaneous state estimate into a vector of control signals using the optimal time-varying feedback gains. The feedback gains are optimal for a behaviorally relevant cost, that includes endpoint error and effort terms but does not specify any preferred movement pattern. To validate this underlying model, we will briefly show that it gives rise to the following empirical phenomena: a) bell-shaped speed profiles, that become heavily skewed when target size decreases; b) smooth corrections for target perturbations, that are incomplete when the perturbation occurs late in the movement; c) elongated covariance ellipses, whose shape varies with movement extent. Then we will present models of four adaptation results. 1. Adaptation to curl-viscous force fields has been found to be overcomplete: subjects initiate a movement in a direction opposite to the perturbation, and later the force field pushes the hand back towards the target2. We will show that such overcompensation is indeed the optimal strategy in the presence of curl-viscous forces. This is because subjects have to push against the forces at some point in time, and the earlier that is done, the more time the feedback loop has to cancel the signaldependent noise that pushing against the forces produces. Incidentally, skewed speed profiles in reaching to small targets have the same explanation: although such a pattern is energetically suboptimal, it allows more accurate control of the final hand position because most of the signal-dependent noise is injected early, and the feedback loop has time to cancel it. 2. Adaptation to unstable elastic forces increases impedance selectively in the unstable direction, and produces “aftereffects” that are more straight than normal reaching movements3. Therefore, under normal circumstances, the motor system is not primarily concerned with producing straight movements. Our model shows the same direction-specific increase in feedback gains, and a corresponding decrease in path variability (in aftereffects, compared to baseline conditions). 3. Local visual distortions, that make the perceived path curved but do not affect the endpoint, might appear to distinguish between desired trajectory tracking and optimal control4. The results (about 25% adaptation) have been interpreted as supporting evidence for desired trajectories. However, these results are equally consistent with optimal control, as follows. The motor system is likely to interpret this unusual perturbation, at least in part, as some change in dynamics that causes the physical movements to become curved. Such a change does not affect the average endpoint location, but in general will affect the expected variance and effort, and therefore trigger a change in the optimal control law. We have designed two different dynamic force fields which can account for the perceived curvature, and trigger partial adaptation in agreement with the experimental results. A more systematic approach to identifying what the motor system might learn from this perturbation would be to use system identification methods, which we are in the process of incorporating into the model. 4. Adaptation to inertial force fields is known to be far from complete, while adaptation to viscous and elastic force fields is normally complete5. We have found that this pattern can be reversed by emphasizing temporal rather than spatial accuracy. It is essential to understand the effect of these force fields, because they are the ones experienced most often in the real world, and therefore are likely to cause “perfect” adaptation. Our explanation is the following. When the task allows some freedom in either the duration or extent of the movement, that freedom can be exploited to reduce both variance and effort. It turns out that movements with added inertia benefit more from slowing down than from reducing extent; on the contrary, movements with added visco-elasticity benefit more from reducing extent than from slowing down. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

E. Todorov and M. I. Jordan, Nature Neuroscience, in press (2002). Available at http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/~todorov K. A. Thoroughman and R. Shadmehr, Nature 407, 742-747 (2000). E. Burdet, R. Osu, D. W. Franklin, T. E. Milner, M. Kawato, Nature 414, 446-449 (2001). D. M. Wolpert, Z. Ghahramani, M. I. Jordan, Experimental Brain Research 103, 460-470 (1995). G. L. Gottlieb, Journal of Neurophysiology 76, 3207-3228 (1996).

Interpreting motor adaptation results within the ...

Such a change does not affect the average endpoint location, but in general will affect the expected variance ... Available at http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/~todorov. 2.

85KB Sizes 0 Downloads 77 Views

Recommend Documents

Causal inference in motor adaptation
Kording KP, Beierholm U, Ma WJ, Quartz S, Tenenbaum JB, Shams L (in press) Causal inference in Cue combination. PLOSOne. Robinson FR, Noto CT, Bevans SE (2003) Effect of visual error size on saccade adaptation in monkey. J. Neurophysiol 90:1235-1244.

Interpreting the Upanishads
Apr 5, 2015 - tronically on the internet, but only free of cost and without any alteration. The rights to publishing and sale in paper-printed form are vested.

Interpreting focus
... instance of what in the presupposition literature has come to be known as the 'binding problem'. ...... Linguistics and Philosophy 19: 259-294. Geurts, B. 1997: ...

Interpreting Utility Patent Claims
Jul 11, 2016 - other patents within the invention's technology field. An Example of ..... consulting information such as dictionaries, technical trea- tises, other ...

Interpreting focus
c is asserted): the speaker claims M (in c), and for every alternative M ∈ ... the focus-semantic value of VP, i.e. the set of alternatives to the interpretation.

Interpreting Utility Patent Claims
Jul 11, 2016 - The diagram below depicts the scope that may be available ... oppositions, trademark cancellations and domain name disputes; and preparing.

MSCDRC holds LIC cannot deny claim if medical results are within ...
Mumbai 400 021. .....Appellant(s). Versus. 1. Smt. Manju Sharma,. 301, Vrindavan Vrajbhoomi Complex,. Dahanukar Wadi, Kandivali (West),. Mumbai 400 067.

Autocratic Adaptation
Jun 30, 2012 - With the help of new fraud identification techniques, I argue that ..... registration centers where domestic observers were stationed (Ichino and ..... A digit-based measure of election fraud would naturally only capture what we here c

Generic Desired Adaptation Outcomes
Robust policies, programmes and actions for CC adaptation. 3. Accurate weather forecasting, reliable seasonal predictions, climate projections & effective early.

D.C. MOTOR
Learning Objectives. Design for ..... Tutorial Problems 29.1. 1. What do ...... Let us, by way of illustration, calculate the speed and output when motor is running off.

Interpreting Curves Workings.pdf
Sign in. Page. 1. /. 3. Loading… Page 1 of 3. Page 1 of 3. Page 2 of 3. Page 2 of 3. Page 3 of 3. Page 3 of 3. Interpreting Curves Workings.pdf. Interpreting Curves Workings.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu. Displaying Interpreting

The Honey Bee Spree Results
Rock Springs. WY. June challenge. 19.00. Done - Congratulations! Adam. Br. Cayuta. NY. 5K. 3.10. 0.00. Done - Congratulations! Hill. Co. Weare. NH. 10K. 0.00.

Practice Interpreting Graphs.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Practice Inte ... ng Graphs.pdf. Practice Inte ... ng Graphs.pdf. Open.

D.C. MOTOR
Moreover, it is only that part of the input energy which is used ... It may also be noted that motor efficiency is given by the ratio of power developed ..... Alternative to this slight increase in the speed is to increase the field current with the

awaken the giant within
3. High-Jump The Barriers Of Self Limitation. Don't be afraid to dream outside the ..... If a problem seems insurmountable, consider that we live in a galaxy that ...

CHANGING GENDER PRACTICES WITHIN THE ... - IS MU
Keywords: gender relations; household division of labor; time use; social ..... images of a good father,” in contradistinction to the simplistic media image of “the .... influences at the institutional level that promote the development of reflex