University of Nairobi Department of Linguistics and Languages

16 December 2009

Luyia Tonal Dialectology Michael R. Marlo University of Maryland [email protected] This talk presents an overview of the diverse tonal systems of Luyia, a Bantu language of western Kenya and eastern Uganda, which has at least 23 dialects. 1.

Luyia dialect situation

The Luyia people, distributed across the Kenya-Uganda border, are the 2nd largest ethnic group in Kenya, behind the Kikuyu, with population numbers roughly equivalent to their Luo neighbors. There could be as many as 7-8 million Luyia in Kenya and Uganda. The CIA Factbook estimates the Kenyan population to be 39,000,000 in July 2009, with the Luyia ethnic group constituting 14% of the population: 5.5 million.1 The 2002 Ugandan census adds about 1.75 million Luyias. As is common throughout Africa, ‘Luyia’ refers to a cluster of closely related dialects or languages, though the Luyia situation is especially heterogeneous. There are at least 19 Luyia dialects in Kenya and at least 4 in Uganda. Thanks to the efforts of Kenyan linguists, the most recent version of the Ethnologue (16th ed., Lewis 2009) has reclassified Luyia as a “macrolanguage”, the various former “dialects” now promoted to the status of “languages”. (1)

Luyia dialects and their population figures, as reported in the Ethnologue 15th ed. (Gordon 2005) 16th ed. (Lewis 2009) Masaba (Uganda) 751,253 (1991 census) 1,120,000 (2002 census) Buya, Dadiri, Gisu, Syan Bukusu 565,000 (1987 BTL) 565,000 (1987 BTL) Nyole (Uganda) 228,918 (1991 census) 341,000 (2002 census) Menya, Hadyo, Sabi, Wesi Logoori

135,000 (SIL 1980)

No estimate available.

Nyore

120,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980)

120,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980)

Kabaras

105,000 (SIL 1980)

No estimate available.

100,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980) 90,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980) 65,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980) 65,000 (SIL 1980) 60,000 (SIL 1980)

100,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980) 90,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980) 65,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980) No estimate available. No estimate available.

Tiriki Isukha Idakho Marama Khayo

2

197,000 (1987 BTL)

Wanga

2

1

197,000 (1987 BTL)

The 1989 Kenyan census reported a population of about 3.5 million Luyias; subsequent Kenyan census figures have not reported statistics in terms of ethnicity. Idakho, Isukha, and Tiriki are (incorrectly, in my view) considered a single language in the 15th and 16th editions of the Ethnologue with a combined population of 306,000 (1987 BTL).

1

Kisa Marachi Saamia (Uganda) Saamia (Kenya) Tachoni Tsotso Gwe (Uganda) Nyala-East Nyala-West Songa (Uganda) Songa (Kenya) Tura (2)

60,000 (SIL 1980) 60,000 (SIL 1980) 225,378 (1991 census) 50,000 (SIL 1980) 47,000 (Heine & Möhlig 1980) 45,000 (SIL 1980) 40,000 (source unclear) 35,000 (SIL 1980) 35,000 (SIL 1980) 10,000 (source unclear) Unknown Unknown

No estimate available. No estimate available. 279,972 (2002 census) No estimate available. No estimate available. No estimate available. 75,257 (2002 census) 35,000 (SIL 1980) Unknown3 Unknown4 Unknown Unknown5

Luyia dialect map (Kenyan varieties, adapted from Heine & Möhlig 1980 and Leung 1991)

Tura

3

4

5

Nyala-East and Nyala-West are treated, incorrectly I believe, as dialects of a single language in the 16th edition of the Ethnologue. Although the 1980 survey indicates 35,000 speakers of Nyala-East and 35,000 speakers of NyalaWest—at least as it is reported in the 14th and 15th editions of the Ethnologue—both East and West varieties of Nyala are reported to have a total of 35,000 speakers in the 16th edition, though the same survey is cited. The 16th edition of the Ethnologue lists Songa as a dialect of Saamia spoken in Uganda. I have seen no evidence for a Ugandan variety of Songa and believe the 10,000 speakers of Songa in Uganda reported in the 15th edition of the Ethnologue may be a mistake. A 2004 survey by SIL Uganda provided to me by Prossy Nannyombi does not even mention Songa in its results. The 10,000 figure may apply to the Songa variety spoken in Kenya, though there may actually be fewer speakers, as the dialect, now found in Nyanza province is evidently under heavy pressure from Luo, and the few Songas I was able to interview in 2006 seemed to speak the Nyala West dialect. The Tura dialect is not reported in the Ethnologue. See Marlo (to appear, a).

2

2.

Previous research on Luyia

There is a growing literature on the linguistic properties of the Luyia dialects, but there is much work to be done. In particular, none of the dialects has a comprehensive reference grammar or a dictionary that accurately marks tone and vowel length. (3)

Tone references Bukusu Khayo Logoori Nyala-West Nyore Saamia Tachoni Tiriki Tura

de Blois 1975, Mutonyi 1996, 2000 Marlo to appear, b, c Leung 1991 Ebarb et al. in prep, Ebarb & Marlo 2009, Marlo 2007, Onyango 2004, 2005, 2006 Key & Bosire 2003 Chagas 1976, Poletto 1998 Odden 2009 Kim & Paster 2007 Marlo to appear, a

(4)

Other phonology references Bukusu Austen 1974a, b, de Blois 1975, Downing 2000, Mutonyi 1992, 1996, 2000, Wasike 2004 Kisa Sample 1976 Masaba Brown 1972 Nyala-East Ochwaya-Oluoch 2003 Nyole Schadeberg 1989 Saamia Marlo 2002, 2004, 2006, Marlo & Brown 2003 Tsootso Dalgish 1974, 1975a, b, c, 1976a, b, 1986

(5)

Dictionaries / wordlists Bukusu “Luhya” (central varieties) Logoori Masaba

de Blois 1975, KWL 1998 Appleby 1943 Friends Africa Mission 1940, Ndanyi & Ndanyi 2005 Siertsema 1981

Grammatical descriptions “Luyia” Masaba Saamia

Appleby 1961, Williams 1972 Purvis 1907 Botne et al. 2006

(6)

(7)

Morphology, syntax, semantics references Bukusu Austen 1974a, Diercks in prep, to appear, Sikuku in prep, Wasike 2007 Kisa Donohew 1973 “Luyia” Frajzyngier 1977, Gary 1977 Logoori Sample 1974

3

Nyole Saamia Tsootso

Namulemu 2004 Botne 2004 Dalgish 1977

(8)

Historical linguistics / classificatory references Bennett 1973, Hinnebusch et al. 1981, Kisembe 2005, Möhlig 1985, Mould 1981, Tucker & Bryan 1975, Williams 1973

(9)

Other references “Luyia” Masaba Nyole

Itebete 1974, Kanyoro 1983, Myers-Scotton 1982, 2000 Brown 1968, Huntingford 1965, Siertsema 1968 Eastman 1972, Morris 1963, SIL 2004

(10)

My research (see https://sites.google.com/site/michaelrmarlo/) Interviews with native speakers of various Luyia dialects since 2001, beginning with a field methods course on Saamia. Fieldwork in Kenya in 2006 (Busia: May – September, Nairobi: October – December) on the verbal tonal systems of Khayo, Marachi, Nyala-West, Tura, and Wanga. My dissertation (Marlo 2007): description and analysis of the Marachi and Nyala-West tonal systems Recent publications on tonal systems of Khayo (Marlo to appear, b, c) and Tura (Marlo to appear, a) Preparing a more definitive study of the Marachi tonal system, as well as: Current collaborations with Henry Nandelanga (former Ph.D. student at University of Southern California) on Bukusu; with Kris Ebarb (Indiana University), Jennifer Fischer (UCLA), and Peter Otiato (University of Illinois) on Nyala-West; and with Robert Botne (Indiana University) and various students on Wanga.

(11)

Luyia dictionary project (see http://sites.google.com/a/indiana.edu/luyiadictionaryproject/) Goals: 2000-6000 word dictionaries for all Luyia dialects, with accompanying sound files demonstrating the pronunciation of each word. Method: digitized Appleby’s (1943) Luluhya-English Vocabulary; distributed copies to native speakers of 17 Kenyan Luyia dialects, who re-translated the dictionary into their dialect; 7 speakers were recorded pronouncing their dictionary entries; Alfred Anangwe has assisted in typing the hand-written materials, and I am soliciting other volunteers to assist in the further development of the dictionaries. Results to date: Bukusu: typed, edited, and tone-marked (thanks to Aggrey Wasike) Wanga: typed and edited (not yet tone-marked) Idakho: typed (not yet edited or tone-marked) Kabaras: typed (not yet edited or tone-marked) Kisa: typed (not yet edited or tone-marked) Tsotso: typed (not yet edited or tone-marked) Tura: typed (not yet edited or tone-marked) Other dictionaries (Khayo, Isukha, Logoori, Nyala-West, Nyore, Saamia, Tachoni, and Tiriki) remain hand-written.

4

3.

Verbal tone in Luyia

(12)

The Luyia dialects have a number of common cross-Bantu tonal properties. a. Privative tonal contrast (Hyman 2001, Stevick 1969): H (v́) vs. Ø (v, “toneless”) As we see below, some dialects have a three-way contrast: H vs. L vs. Ø.

b. Tone-Bearing Unit (TBU) = µ, (the “mora”, i.e., the vowel) c.

Tonal contrasts on long and short vowels: µ́µ́ vs. µµ vs. µ́µ vs. µµ́ vs. µ́ vs. µ Contour tones only on long (bimoraic) vowels: one tone per mora.

(13)

Tonal melodies Tone plays a significant role in the grammatical system, marking tense-aspect-mood-negation and clause-type (matrix vs. relative) distinctions in verbs. Underlyingly floating H tones (“melodic Hs”) are assigned by rule to different positions of the verb [stem].

(14)

Tonal melodies in Nyala-West (Ebarb & Marlo 2009)6 Pattern 1a H from σ2 to final Indefinite Future

xu-ri[paangúlúl-á]

‘we will disarrange’

βa-ná[paangúlúl-á]

‘they will disarrange’

Pattern 1b Near Future Pattern 2a Present Negative

H on µ1 of [σ2] si-i[mbaangúlul-a]

‘I am not disarranging’

si-β-a-xá[paangúlul-a]

‘they did not just disarrange’

Pattern 2b Immediate Past Neg. Pattern 3 ImperativeSg Pattern 4 Remote Past Pattern 5 Hodiernal Perfective Pattern 6 Conditional Past

6

H on final [paangulul-á]

‘disarrange!’

H on µ1 βa-á[paangulul-a]

‘they disarranged’

H on µ1 and final xu[sáambuluul-é]

‘he de-roofed’

all toneless xu[paanguluul-e]

‘if we could disarrange’

Single-underlining indicates an underlying H; double-underlining indicates the surface position of a melodic H. The verb [stem] is indicated in square brackets.

5

4.

Three types of tone systems in Luyia

The tonal systems of the Luyia dialects can be classified into three types: ‘conservative’, ‘reversed’, and ‘predictable’ (Marlo 2008). These categories reflect differences in the nature of lexical tone contrasts in verb roots and whether melodic H suffixes are found in all verbal contexts in the language. (15)

Map of Luyia tone systems

Predictable

Tura

Conservative

Reversed

4.1

Conservative tone systems

The known ‘conservative’ dialects of Luyia are found in the eastern part of Luyialand: Idakho, Logoori (Leung 1991), Tachoni (Odden 2009), and Tiriki (Kim & Paster 2007). These dialects maintain the historical distinction between two lexical classes of verbs: /H/ (“H-toned”) verbs have an underlying H on V1 (the stem-initial vowel/mora). /Ø/ (“toneless) verbs are underlyingly toneless. The underlying /H/ vs. /Ø/ contrast is seen clearly in tenses such as the Infinitive, which lack grammatical tonal suffixes (Goldsmith 1987’s “Simple Stem” pattern). (16)

Tachoni lexical tonal contrast in the Infinitive Infinitive H on V1 /H/ o-xu[káraang-a] all Ø /Ø/ o-xu[purux-a] Note: the infinitive prefix xu- is toneless.

6

‘to fry’ ‘to fly’

Other tenses are inflected with melodic H suffixes, which target different positions of the stem. The tonal suffixes can interact with lexical tones, often resulting in opaque surface forms. (17)

Tachoni tonal melodies Indef Fut H from σ3 to penult /H/ βa-li[karaang-án-íl-a] H from σ2 to penult /Ø/ βa-li[βal-án-íl-a] Subjunctive /H/ /Ø/ Imp Neg /H/ /Ø/

Imperative

/H/ /Ø/

H on σ2 xa-βa[βek-án-il-e] H on σ2 xa-βa[yoombóól-an-il-e] all Ø o-xa[karaang-a] H on σ of V2 o-xa[láámbaal-a]

‘they will fry for e.o.’ ‘they will count e.o.’

‘let them shave for e.o.’ ‘let them spill for e.o.’

‘don’t fry’ ‘don’t lie down’

H from σ3 to penult [βotoox-án-íl-a] all Ø [yoomboox-a]

‘fry’ ‘spill’

In all four of these patterns, the lexical H of /H/ verbs fails to surface, i.e., V1 is [Ø]: the lexical H is deleted before the melodic H—a common phenomenon in Bantu tonal systems. Even though the lexical H is deleted on the surface, it affects the surface position of the grammatical H by limiting its leftward extent, e.g., in the Indefinite Future, the grammatical H begins on σ3 in /H/ verbs, but on σ2 in /Ø/ verbs. If it were not for tenses like the Infinitive, where V1 surfaces [H], we might think V1 is /L/ because it limits the leftward placement of the grammatical H and surfaces [Ø]. 4.2

Predictable tone systems (see Odden 1989 on other predictable Bantu languages)

The known ‘predictable’ dialects of Luyia are found in the western region: Khayo (Marlo to appear, b, c), Nyala-West (Ebarb & Marlo 2009, Ebarb et al. in prep, Marlo 2007, Onyango 2004, 2005, 2006), Saamia (Poletto 1998, Botne et al. 2006), Songa, and Tura (Marlo to appear, a). These dialects have lost the lexical tonal contrast in verb roots; there is only one tonal class of verb: /Ø/. (18)

7

Nyala-West Infinitive *H verbs7 CV o-xú[ry-a]

*Ø verbs o-xú[sy-a]

‘to eat’

‘to grind’

The asterisk indicates the historical tonal value here, i.e., *H refers to historically H-toned roots, and *Ø refers to historically toneless roots.

7

CVCV

o-xú[βek-á]

‘to shave’

o-xú[βal-á]

‘to count’

CVVCV

o-xú[deex-á]

‘to cook’

o-xú[reeβ-á]

‘to ask’

CVCVCV+

o-xú[βukúl-á] o-xú[karááng-á] o-xú[fukíírír-á]

‘to take’ ‘to fry’ ‘to agree’

o-xú[burúx-á] o-xú[laambáál-á] o-xú[βichíkál-á]

‘to fly’ ‘to sleep wildly’ ‘to belch’

Note that the Infinitival prefix xú- is H. All tenses are inflected with a grammatical H suffix. (19)

Tonal patterns in Nyala-West (repeated from (14) above) Pattern 1a H from σ2 to final Indefinite Future

xu-ri[paangúlúl-á]

‘we will disarrange’

βa-ná[paangúlúl-á]

‘they will disarrange’

Pattern 1b Near Future Pattern 2a Present Negative

H on µ1 of [σ2] si-i[mbaangúlul-a]

‘I am not disarranging’

si-β-a-xá[paangúlul-a]

‘they did not just disarrange’

Pattern 2b Immediate Past Neg. Pattern 3 ImperativeSg Pattern 4 Remote Past Pattern 5 Hodiernal Perfective Pattern 6 Conditional Past

H on final [paangulul-á]

‘disarrange!’

H on µ1 βa-á[paangulul-a]

‘they disarranged’

H on µ1 and final xu[sáambuluul-é]

‘he de-roofed’

all toneless xu[paanguluul-e]

‘if we could disarrange’

Note that like the Infinitive xú-, the tense prefixes of the Immediate Past xá- and Remote Past áare /H/. 4.3

Reversed tone systems

The known ‘reversed’ dialects of Luyia are found in the central region, between the conservative and predictable dialects: Bukusu (Mutonyi 2000), Marachi (Marlo 2007), and Wanga.

8

Like the conservative dialects, but unlike the predictable dialects, the reversed dialects have an underlying tonal contrast on V1 between two classes of verbs: a ‘toned’ class and a ‘toneless’ class. Unlike the conservative dialects, but like the predictable dialects, all tenses are inflected with a melodic H, even in tenses like the Infinitive. (20)

Lexical tonal contrast in Marachi Infinitive H on σ1, H from σ2 to final *H o-xu[βó !dóóxán-á] (...) ‘to go around (…)’ H from tense prefix (xu-) to final *Ø o-xú[xwéésúlúl-á] (...) ‘to drag (…)’

Since there is no context which directly reveals the underlying tonal properties of the verb stem, it is difficult to determine the quality of the tone in the ‘toned’ class of verbs. Mutonyi (2000) and Marlo (2007) assume that historically *H verbs are synchronically /H/ in Bukusu and Marachi, respectively, while Austen (1974) and Marlo (2008) assume underlying /L/. The main reason to posit underlying /L/ is that, as in Tachoni, σ1 of *H verbs surfaces [Ø] in most tonal melodies Marachi. (The position of underlying /L/ is marked with a box.) (21)

Marachi tone patterns: σ1 is [L] Indef Fut H on V1 of σ2 and following V *H a-li[karááng-aang-a] H from V1 of σ2 to final a-li[karááng-ír-á] ... H on V1 and V2 *Ø a-li[lómálom-a] H from V1 to final a-li[sáámbúl-ír-á] ... Crast Fut *H *Ø

Imper Neg

*H *Ø

Imperative

*H *Ø

H on V1 of σ2 and following V n-aa[βodóóxan-eeng-e] (...) H on V1 of σ2 and following V ni-βa[lomálóm-an-ir-e] (…) all Ø o-la[βodooxan-a] dá H on V1 and V2 o-la[lómálom-a] dá H on final [karaang-á] (…) H on final [βakal-á] (…)

‘he will be frying’ ‘he will fry for ...’ ‘he will talk’ ‘he will de-roof for ...’

‘he will be going around (…)’ ‘they will talk for each other (…)’

‘don’t go around!’ ‘don’t talk!’

‘fry (…)!’ ‘spread to dry (…)!’

There are other tenses, such as the Infinitive, where σ1 surfaces [H], but there is evidence that this is not the lexical H.

9

(22)

Marachi tone patterns: σ1 is [H] Infinitive H on σ1, H from σ2 to final *H o-xu[βó !dóóxán-á] (…) ‘to go around (…)’ H from tense prefix (xu-) to final *Ø o-xú[xwéésúlúl-á] (...) ‘to drag (…)’ Imm Pst *H



Rem Pst 1 *H *Ø

H on σ1, H on V1 of σ2 and following V y-a-xa[ká !rááng-aang-a] H on σ1, H from σ2 to final y-a-xa[ká !rááng-ír-á] … H on σ1, H on V1 of σ2 and following V y-a-xa[xwéé!súlúl-a] H from tense prefix (xa-) to final y-a-xá[sáámbúl-ír-á] … H from tense prefix (a-) to σ1 y-aá[káraang-aang-a] (…) H from tense prefix (a-) to σ1 y-aá[lómalom-a] (…)

The H on σ1 originates synchronically on the preceding tense prefix, i.e., /xú-/, /xá-/, /á-/, and shifts/spread from there to the right. (23)

Prefix Hop H L H x x o-xu[βó !dóóxán-á]

Supporting evidence for this position comes from the facts that: Prefix Hop is a regular process affecting object prefixes in Marachi and Tachoni The same tense prefixes are unambiguously /H/ in Khayo and Nyala-West, as we have seen. The main complication for this analysis is the pattern of /Ø/ verbs in the Infinitive and in phrasemedial Immediate Past forms, where the H of the tense prefix does not shift to σ1 (presumably because it is deleted in this position). 5.

How did the predictable and reversed dialects develop from the conservative systems?

The key difference between the conservative tone systems and the reversed and predictable systems is that all tenses in the reversed and predictable systems are inflected with a melodic H suffix. How did this innovation come about? One possibility: A pattern of phrasal H-Tone Anticipation found in conservative dialects such as Logoori and Tiriki was reinterpreted as a melodic H.

10

(24)

H-Tone Anticipation in Logoori /H/ verbs va-rá[záázam-a] va-rá[záá!zám-á] gáráha /Ø/ verbs va-ra[mɔrɔm-a] va-rá[mɔ́rɔ́m-á] gáráha

H ‘they will taste’ ‘they will taste slowly’ ‘they will talk’ ‘they will talk slowly’

H

va-rá[záá!zám-á] gáráha H va-rá[mɔ́rɔ́m-á] gáráha

Once all tenses possess a melodic H, there is no context that directly reveals the quality of the underlying tone in the ‘toned’ class of verbs, so determining the underlying tone of *H verbs becomes an analytical challenge. The reversed dialects reanalyzed *H as /L/, making tonal derivations more transparent than in the conservative dialects (though not without certain analytical complications). The [H] on σ1 of *H verbs was attributed to a prefix, which is possible since conservative dialects like Tachoni already have a rule of Prefix Hop (Odden 2009). How did the predictable systems develop? The Nyala-West tonal patterns correspond almost perfectly to (phrase-medial) /L/ verbs in Marachi.8 (25)

Comparison of predictable Tense Infinitive Indef Fut Imm Pst Rem Pst 1 Crast Fut Imp Neg Imperative

(Nyala-West) and reversed (Marachi) tone patterns Predictable (Nyala-West) Reversed (Marachi /L/ …) o-xu[βó !dóóxán-á] o-xú[karááng-á] xu-ri[paangúlúl-á] a-li[karááng-ír-á] βa-xá[deex-ér-án-á] y-a-xa[ká !rááng-ír-á] β-aá[paangulul-a] y-aá[káraang-aang-a] na-βa[βodóxan-e] n-aa[βodóóxan-eeng-e] o-ra[lomálom-a] o-la[βodooxan-a] dá [paangulul-á] [βodooxan-á]

The predictable tone systems eliminated the analytical ambiguity between /H/ and /L/ and between /L/ and /Ø/, choosing the tone patterns of /L/ verbs. Once the underlying tonal distinction is neutralized, a simpler analysis is that all verbs are /Ø/. The predictable dialects therefore underwent their own ‘reversal’: *H  *L  /Ø/. Why did the /L/ vs. /Ø/ opposition neutralize in the direction of /L/ in the predictable dialects? One possibility: /L/ (*H) verbs are more frequent or an inherently larger set, so their lexical class grew larger. (Unfortunately, there is no existing dictionary that can test this hypothesis.) Whatever the initial trigger, neutralization in this direction is apparent synchronically in Marachi.

8

Aside from the position of the /H/-toned tense prefixes, which predictably move onto σ1 in Marachi, the only other difference in these surface tone patterns is in the Imperative Negative forms o-ra[lomálom-a] (Nyala-West) vs. o-la[βodooxan-a] dá (Marachi). This difference is superficial: the Marachi form derives from an intermediate representation where H is found on σ2 (Marlo 2007).

11

(26)

*Ø  /L/ in Marachi Some *Ø verbs freely vary between /Ø/ and /L/ pronunciations in all contexts. a-lá[léxúúl-á] ~ a-la[lé !xúúl-a] ‘he will release’ Some *Ø roots categorically have the /L/ tonal pattern. a-la[βyéé!tsékál-á] ‘he will belch’ cf. Tachoni /βeečakal-a/ All vowel-initial verb roots longer than VC are /L/, i.e., there are no VCV(V)C+ /Ø/ roots (though such stems can be derived by adding suffixes to /Ø/ VC roots).

6.

Fine-grained distinctions among the predictable dialects

In addition to the three main types of tonal systems within Luyia—conservative, reversed, and predictable—there are also more fine-grained differences among dialects within each of these types. For example, in the Indefinite Future, the predictable dialects Nyala-West, Khayo, and Tura all manifest the same general tone pattern in which the melodic H surfaces from σ2 to the final, but there are differences especially in shorter stems. All three dialects have a different tonal pattern in CVVCV stems in this tense. (27)

Indefinite Future (‘he/they will V’) Nyala-West CV a-ri[fw-a]

Khayo a-li[fw-á]

Tura a-li[fw-á]

CVCV

a-ri[rim-á]

a-li[βék-á]

a-li[βék-á]

CVVCV

a-ri[reeβ-á]

a-li[reéβ-á]

a-li[rééβ-á]

CVCVCV+

a-ri[βukúl-á] a-ri[karááng-á] a-ri[siindíx-á] a-ri[riingáál-á] a-ri[lomálóm-á] βa-ri[deex-ér-án-á]

a-li[βukúl-á] a-li[xalaáng-á] a-li[siindíx-á] a-li[liingaál-á] a-li[lomálóm-á] βa-li[deex-ér-án-á]

a-li[βukúl-á] a-li[karááng-á] a-li[siindíx-á] a-li[saangáál-á] a-li[lomálóm-á] βa-li[teex-ér-án-á]

Thus, tone can be useful not only in identifying major distinctions among the Luyia dialects (eastern/conservative vs. central/reversed vs. western/predictable), as well as more fine-grained differences among dialects from the same area (Nyala-West vs. Khayo vs. Tura). This motivates the further study of the Luyia verbal tonal systems, especially those dialects that straddle the eastern/conservative vs. central/reversed border—many of which have no documentation of their tonal systems (e.g., Kabarasi, Kisa, Marama, Nyala-East, and Tsotso)—and these systems may hold the key for determining exactly how the reversed systems developed from the conservative ones. 7.

References

Appleby, L. L. 1943. An English-LuLuhya Vocabulary. Maseno, Kenya: C. M. S. Appleby, L. L. 1961. A First Luyia Grammar. Nairobi, Kenya: The East African Literature Bureau. Austen, Cheryl. 1974a. Aspects of Bukusu Syntax and Phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, Indiana University.

12

Austen, Cheryl. 1974b. Anatomy of the Tonal System of a Bantu Language. Studies in African Linguistics 5: 21-34. Bennett, Patrick. 1973. A Phonological History of Northeast Victoria. Paper presented at 4th Annual Conference of African Linguistics. Botne, Robert. 2004. Specificity in Lusaamia Infinitives. Studies in Language 28: 137-164. Botne, Robert, Hannington Ochwada & Michael Marlo. A Grammatical Sketch of the Lusaamia Verb. Grammatical Analyses of African Languages 30. Rüdiger Köppe: Cologne. Brown, Gillian. 1968. The dialect situation in Bugisu. Journal of African Languages 7: 58-67. Brown, Gillian. 1972. Phonological rules and dialect variation: a study of the phonology of Lumasaaba. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Chagas, Jeremy. 1976. The tonal structure of Olusamia. In Larry Hyman, ed., Studied in Bantu Tonology (SCOPIL 3), 217-240. Los Angeles: University of Southern California. Dalgish, Gerard. 1974. Arguments for a unified treatment of y-initial and vowel-initial roots in Olutsootso. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 4: 76-90. Dalgish, Gerard. 1975a. The Diachronic Development of Nasal Deletion in OluTsootso. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 5: 25-40. Dalgish, Gerard. 1975b. On underlying and superficial constraints in Olutsootso. Chicago Linguistic Society 11. Dalgish, Gerard. 1975c. Nasal Interactions and Bantu Vowel-Initial Roots; The Morphological or Phonological Solution? Proceedings of the Sixth Conference on African Linguistics: OSU Working Papers in Linguistics 20: 68-74. Dalgish, Gerard. 1976a. The Morphophonemics of the OluTsootso Dialect of (Olu)Luyia; Issues and Implications, Ph.D. dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Dalgish, Gerard. 1976b. Passivizing Locatives in OluTsootso. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 6: 57-68. Dalgish, Gerald. 1977. Towards an Explanation of Diachronic and Synchronic Exceptions in OluTsootso and Kyamutwara (Haya). In P. F. A. Kotey and H. Der-Houssikian, eds., Language and Linguistic Problems in Africa: Proceedings of the VII Conference on African Linguistics. Columbia, SC: Hornbeam Press. Dalgish, Gerard. 1986. /-a-/ Reduction Phenomena in Luyia. Studies in African Linguistics 17: 155-176. De Blois, Keis. 1975. Bukusu Generative Phonology and Aspects of Bantu Structure. Tervuren, België: koninklijk Museum voor midden Afrika. Diercks, Michael. To appear. Null expletives and agreement in Bukusu locative inversion. Lingua (Proceedings of the Bantu Movement and Word Order conference, Leiden University, March 2008). Diercks, Michael. In prep. The morphosyntax of subjects in Lubukusu. Ph.D. dissertation, Georgetown University. Donohew, G. N. 1973. Some verbal extensions in Shisa, Ph.D. dissertation, University of London. Downing, Laura. 2004. Bukusu reduplication. In Chege Githiora, Heather Littlefield & Victor Manfredi, eds. Trends in African Linguistics 5. Lawrenceville, NJ: Africa World Press. Eastman, Carol. 1972. Lunyole of the Bamenya. Journal of African Languages 11: 63-78. Ebarb, Kristopher, Michael Marlo, Jennifer Fischer & Peter Otiato. in prep. Nyala-West verbal tonology. Ebarb, Kristopher & Michael Marlo. 2009. Conditioning factors in the realization of tone: Nyala-West verbs. Ms, Indiana University & University of Maryland. Frajzyngier, Zygmunt. 1977. On the Intransitive Copy Pronouns in Chadic, Luyia, and Mashi. In Martin Mould & Thomas Hinnebusch, eds., Papers from the Eighth Conference on African Linguistics, 73-84. Los Angeles: Los Angeles Pagination. Gary, Judith Olmsted. 1977. Implications for Universal Grammar of Object-Creating Rules in Luyia and Mashi. In Martin Mould & Thomas Hinnebusch, eds., Papers from the Eighth Conference on African Linguistics, 85-95. Los Angeles: Los Angeles Pagination. Goldsmith, John. 1987. Stem Tone Patterns of the Lacustrine Bantu Languages. In David Odden, ed., Current Approaches to African Linguistics IV, 175-185. Dordrecht: Foris. Heine, Bernd & Wilhelm Möhlig. 1980. Language and dialect atlas of Kenya. vol. 1. Berlin: Dietrich Reimer Verlag. Hinnebusch, Thomas, Derek Nurse & Martin Mould. 1981. Studies in the classification of Eastern Bantu Languages. Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag. Huntingford, G. W. B. 1965. The Orusyan language of Uganda. Journal of African Languages 4: 145-169.

13

Hyman, Larry. 2001. Privative tone in Bantu. In Shigeki Kaji, ed., Cross-linguistic studies of tonal phenomena, 237-257. Tokyo: ILCAA. Itebete, P. A. N. 1974. Language Standardization in Western Kenya: The LuLuyia Experiment. In W. H. Whiteley, ed., Language in Kenya, 87-114. Nairobi: Oxford University Press. Kanyoro, Rachel Angogo. 1983. Unity in Diversity: A linguistic survey of the Abaluyia in Western Kenya. Vienna: AfroPublications. Key, Michael & Mokaya Bosire. 2003. Leftward High tone spread and downstep in Nyore. Ms. University of Albany. Kim, Yuni & Mary Paster. 2007. Downstep in Tiriki. ms. Pomona College and University of California, Berkeley. Kisembe, Evelyne. 2005. A Linguistic Analysis of Luyia Varieties Spoken in Western Kenya. ms. Memorial University of Newfoundland. KWL. 1998. Bukusu wordlist. Comparative Bantu On-Line Dictionary (CBOLD). Leung, Elizabeth. 1991. The Tonal Phonology of Llogoori: A Study of Llogoori Verbs (Working Papers of the Cornell Phonetics Laboratory, 6). Ithaca, NY: CLC Publications. Marlo, Michael. 2002. Reduplication in Lusaamia. Indiana University Working Papers in Linguistics Online 2. Marlo, Michael. 2004. Prefixal reduplication in Lusaamia: evidence from morphology. In Akinbiyi Akinlabi, ed., Proceedings of the 4th World Congress of African Linguistics. Köln: Rüdiger Köppe. Marlo, Michael. 2006. CV- root expansion in three Luyia languages. In Nancy Kula and Rose Letsholo, eds., African Linguistics in the new millennium, Linguistic Analysis 32(3-4): 293-326. Marlo, Michael. 2007. The verbal tonology of Lumarachi and Lunyala: two dialects of Luluyia (Bantu, J.30, Kenya). Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan. Marlo, Michael. 2008. Reversal and Predictability in Luyia Tone. Paper presented at the Annual Conference on African Linguistics 38. University of Georgia, April 17-20. Marlo, Michael. To appear, a. Khayo verbal tonology. Africana Linguistica 15: 77-129. Marlo, Michael. To appear, b. Tura verbal tonology. Studies in African Linguistics 37: 153-243. Marlo, Michael. To appear, c. Tonal melodies and Meeussen’s Rule in Khayo. In Masangu Matondo, ed., Studies in Bantu Linguistics and Languages: Papers in memory of Professor Rugatiri Mekacha. Bayreuth: Bayreuth African Studies. Marlo, Michael & Aaron Brown. 2003. Segmental duration in Lusaamia. Indiana University Working Papers in Linguistics Online 2. Möhlig, Wilhelm. 1985. Luhya Dialectology. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 7: 203-207. Morris, H. F. 1963. A Note on Lunyole. Uganda Journal 27. Mould, Martin. 1981. Greater Luyia. In Thomas J. Hinnebusch, Derek Nurse & Martin Mould, eds., Studies in the Classification of Eastern Bantu Languages, 181-236. Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag. Mutonyi, Nasiombe. 1992. Cs and Vs or Moras: The case of Bukusu Prosodic Structure. OSU Working Papers in Linguistics 41: 46-84. Mutonyi, Nasiombe. 1996. Tense, Aspect, and Bukusu Verb Tones. OSU Working Papers in Linguistics 47: 77-110. Mutonyi, Nasiombe. 2000. Aspects of Bukusu morphology and phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, The Ohio State University. Myers-Scotton, Carol. 1982. Language Use in Kenya: An Urban-Rural Comparison of the Luyia. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 34: 121-136. Myers-Scotton, Carol. 2000. Explaining the Role of Norms and Rationality in Codeswitching. Journal of Pragmatics: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Language Studies 32: 1259-1271. Namulemu, Enoch Wandera. 2004. Tense, Aspect and Mood in Lunyole Grammar and Narrative Discourse, Ph.D. dissertation, Nairobi Evangelical Graduate School of Theology. Ndanyi, Elisha Ugaada & Joseph Olindo Ndanyi. 2005. Amang’ana go Lulimi lwo Lulogooli. Nairobi: Ndanyi Enterprises. Ochwaya-Oluoch, Yvonne. 2003. A Study of the Phonology and Morphology of Lunyala. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Sheffield.

14

Odden, David. 1989. Predictable tone systems in Bantu. In Harry van der Hulst & Norval Smith, eds., Autosegmental studies on pitch accent systems, 225-251. Dordrecht: Foris. Odden, David. 2009. Tachoni verbal tonology. Language Sciences 31: 305-324. Onyango, Jacktone Okello. 2004. Tone Mapping and Tense in Olunyala. Malilime: Malawian Journal of Linguistics 4: 65-81. Onyango, Jacktone Okello. 2005. Universal Properties of Tone: The Interaction Between Verbal Mood and Tone Patterns in Olunyala. In R. Bett, C. Etzhhold, & E. Müller, eds., Proceedings of the Conference “Across Borders: Benefiting from Cultural Differences”. Nairobi: DAAD, Regional Office for Africa. Onyango, Jacktone Okello. 2006. Verb tonology in Olunyala. Ph.D. dissertation, Kenyatta University. Poletto, Robert. 1998. Constraints on Tonal Association in Olusamia: An Optimality Theoretic Account. In Larry Hyman & Charles Kisseberth, eds., Theoretical Aspects of Bantu Tone, 331-364. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. Purvis, J. B. 1907. A manual of Lumasaba grammar. London: Society for promoting Christian knowledge. Sample, Ward Arthur. 1974. The Applied Extension with Dative and Benefactive Implications in Logooli. MILA 4. Sample, Ward Arthur. 1976. The Application of Rules in the Phonology of Olukisa, Ph.D. dissertation, Indiana University. Schadeberg, Thilo. 1989. The Velar Nasal in Nyole. Annales Aequatoria 10: 169-179. Siertsema, Berthe. 1968. Pros and Cons of Macro-Phonemes in New Orthographies (Masaba spelling problems). Lingua 21: 429-442. Siertsema, Berthe. 1981. Masaba word list: Masaba-English, English-Masaba. Tervuren: Musée Royal de l’Afrique Central. Sikuku, Justine. In prep. Pro-drop in Lubukusu: A Government and Binding approach. University of Nairobi, Department of Linguistics and Languages, Ph.D. dissertation. SIL 2004. Lunyole Orthography Guide. Entebbe, Uganda: Lunyole Language Association and SIL International. Stevick, Earl. 1969. Tone in Bantu. International Journal of American Linguistics 35: 330-341. Tucker, A. N. & M. A. Bryan. 1957. Linguistic survey of the northern Bantu borderland. Vol 4: languages of the eastern section, Great Lakes to Indian Ocean. London: Oxford University Press for the International African Institute (IAI). Wasike, Aggrey. 2004. Vowel Hiatus in Bukusu. Ms. Cornell University. Wasike, Aggrey. 2007. The Left Periphery, WH- In-Situ and A-Bar Movement in Lubukusu and Other Bantu Languages. Ph.D. dissertation, Cornell University. Williams, Ralph. 1972. An Introduction to Oluluyia. Ph.D. dissertation, Trinity College. Williams, Ralph. 1973. A lexico-statistical look at Oluluyia. Paper presented at Annual Conference on African Linguistics 4.

15

Luyia Tonal Dialectology

Dec 16, 2009 - California) on Bukusu; with Kris Ebarb (Indiana University), Jennifer ... Peter Otiato (University of Illinois) on Nyala-West; and with Robert Botne ...

316KB Sizes 1 Downloads 123 Views

Recommend Documents

Neo-Aramaic Dialectology: Jews, Christians, and Mandaeans ...
Jun 26, 2013 - International Conference June 26 – 27, 2013 at The Israel Institute for Advanced ... The Vowel System of Midyat Ṭuroyo – A Phonemic Analysis.

Neo-Aramaic Dialectology: Jews, Christians, and Mandaeans ...
Jun 26, 2013 - Neo-Aramaic Dialectology: Jews, Christians, and Mandaeans. International Conference June 26 – 27, 2013 at The Israel Institute for Advanced ...

tonal elements and their alignment
to the rate at which overall pitch range is manipulated leads to pervasive ambiguity in analysis .... Liberman's metrical treatment of English intonation differed from autosegmental accounts of tone ... Compare, for example, the treatment of syllable

Adaptive, Selective, Automatic Tonal ... - Research at Google
Oct 24, 2009 - Most commer- cial photo editing software tools provide the ability to automati- ... We select the best reference face automatically given a collection ... prototypical face selection procedure on a YouTube video blog, with the ...