vol. 172, no. 2

the american naturalist

august 2008

Male Mating Constraints Affect Mutual Mate Choice: Prudent Male Courting and Sperm-Limited Females

Roger Ha¨rdling,1,* Thomas Gosden,1,† and Robin Aguile´e2,‡

1. Department of Animal Ecology, Ecology Building, Lund University, S-223 62 Lund, Sweden; 2. E´cole Normale Supe´rieure, Unit of Mathematical Evolutionary Biology, 46 rue d’Ulm, F-75230 Paris, France Submitted June 28, 2007; Accepted January 2, 2008; Electronically published June 27, 2008

abstract: Costs of sperm production may lead to prudence in male sperm allocation and also to male mate choice. Here, we develop a life history–based mutual mate choice model that takes into account the lost-opportunity costs for males from time out in sperm recovery and lets mate competition be determined by the prevailing mate choice strategies. We assume that high mating rate may potentially lead to sperm depletion in males, and that as a result, female reproduction may be limited by the availability of sperm. Increasing variation in male quality leads, in general, to increased selective mate choice by females, and vice versa. Lower-quality males may, however, gain access to more fecund higher-quality females by lowering their courting rate, thus increasing their sperm reserves. When faced with strong male competition for mates, low-quality males become less choosy, which leads to assortative mating for quality and an increased mating rate across all males. With assortative mating, the frequency of antagonistic interactions (sexual conflict) is reduced, allowing males to lower the time spent replenishing sperm reserves in order to increase mating rate. This in turn leads to lower sperm levels at mating and therefore could lead to negative effects on female fitness via sperm limitation. Keywords: mutual mate choice, sperm depletion, mating costs, malemale competition, sexual conflict.

Bateman’s (1948) pioneering experiment with fruit flies found a fundamental difference between male and female reproductive success; male reproductive success is seem* Corresponding author; e-mail: [email protected]. †

E-mail: [email protected].



E-mail: [email protected].

Am. Nat. 2008. Vol. 172, pp. 259–271. 䉷 2008 by The University of Chicago. 0003-0147/2008/17202-42698$15.00. All rights reserved. DOI: 10.1086/589452

ingly limited only by the number of mates they can obtain, whereas female reproductive success is constrained by the costs of offspring production (Parker 1979; Thornhill and Alcock 1983). This view, that is, that males carry a small unsubstantial cost to gamete production allowing them to mate freely, whereas females carry the high cost of mating almost single-handedly (Thornhill and Alcock 1983), has long been held within the field of sexual selection (Andersson 1994). However, this has changed over the past few decades as the idea that male costs can shape sexual selection has become more prominent (Wedell et al. 2002). Although sperm competition is a source of selection for increased sperm amount to maintain numerical superiority in the fertilization lottery (Parker 1990; Birkhead and Møller 1998), males cannot produce limitless amounts of sperm (Nakatsuru and Kramer 1982; Wedell et al. 2002). Dewsbury (1982) highlighted the nontrivial costs generated by ejaculate production both energetically and in maintaining mature sperm for mating opportunities. Sperm production costs can be high enough to cause a reduction in life span (Van Voorhies 1992), and they may also constrain the male fitness gain from multiple mating via sperm depletion (Nakatsuru and Kramer 1982; Royer and McNeil 1993), an effect that may be exaggerated by a low-quality diet (Gage and Cook 1994). This will also be negative for females, which may as a result suffer from sperm limitation (Wedell et al. 2002). In vertebrates, for example, males cannot produce an ejaculate for a period of time after mating, so males often need to recover after each mating; in addition, multiple ejaculates are frequently characterized by a reduced amount of sperm (Preston et al. 2001). However, males can still gain from mating in a sperm-depleted state because of a reduced female propensity to remate with other males (Løvlie et al. 2005; Damiens and Boivin 2006), which can cause females to actively avoid multiply mated and therefore possibly sperm-depleted males (Nakatsuru and Kramer 1982; Warner et al. 1995; Harris and Moore 2005). Costs connected with each breeding event determine the strength of competition for mates (Kokko and Monaghan 2001), with obvious implications for the evolution

260 The American Naturalist of mate choice behavior. Mate choice is also known to be strongly influenced by the variance in mate quality (Johnstone et al. 1996). Ha¨rdling and Kokko (2005) showed in a model with passive female choice that male competition for mates may result in the evolution of a prudent-choice strategy, where low-quality males reject high-quality females and high-quality males reject low-quality females. This process is driven by male costs connected with partner takeovers by males of higher quality, which lead to assortative mating (Crespi 1989; Kirkpatrick et al. 1990; Fawcett and Johnstone 2003). To the extent that genes influence quality, assortative mating will have evolutionary consequences by increasing phenotypic and genetic quality variation. Male partner choice is usually considered to be part of a mutual mate choice population strategy, and theoretical models of mutual mate choice have focused on the importance of variation in partner quality (Parker 1983; Owens and Thompson 1994) and various costs of mating, such as energetic costs and parental investment (Dewsbury 1982; Johnstone et al. 1996; Kokko and Monaghan 2001; Kokko and Johnstone 2002). However, these models do not consider that when male sperm production is limited and males need time to recover between matings, the sperm amount in each ejaculate (an important component of male quality) is a direct function of the male’s mate choice strategy. This is a further complication of the problem of mutual mate choice. To analyze this case, we examine the evolution of mutual mate choice strategies, assuming that females differ in fecundity and males differ in the amount of time needed to replenish sperm reserves after mating. Sperm recovery causes an opportunity fitness cost in males, and females incur a fitness cost if they mate with males that are unable to fertilize all their eggs. The trade-off between average mate quality and the number of reproductive events determines the optimal mating rate for both sexes (Owens and Thompson 1994). Mating is dependent on agreement by both sexes, and male and female mating strategies coevolve in an evolutionary game that explicitly considers the dynamics of the population structure, that is, the number of mating and single individuals at any one time. The model is thus self-consistent (Houston and McNamara 1999). One objective of this study is to investigate whether time costs of replenishment of sperm reserves may cause males to be selective in their mate choice. We also want to examine the possible effects of male-male competition on sperm investment and female choice. Although we do not directly test for effects on sperm competition, males are assumed to be able to alter their sperm number at mating by altering their time intervals between mating. We also investigate the repercussions that such behavior has on

female optimal mating decisions, assuming that females seek to maximize the number of eggs fertilized. The Model For both sexes, fitness is determined by a trade-off between the number of mates and average mate quality. Males may increase the rate of mating by mating more indiscriminately, but this is at a cost of mating more frequently with lowquality females that do not produce many eggs and mating with lower sperm reserves. Female costs arise from sperm limitation (i.e., males may not be able to fertilize all of their eggs). We assume that both males and females can be classified as low- or high-quality individuals (represented below by L and H, respectively) and define high-quality males as males that recover their sperm reserves faster than lowquality males. The sperm recovery function Si(t) describes the increase in sperm reserves as a function of time (t) since the last copulation, for a male of quality i (i 苸 {L, H}; eq. [1]). A positive relationship between time since last mating and sperm transferred has been shown in many species (Dewsbury 1982; Møller 1991; Eberhard 1996). We assume that the sperm reserves gradually approach a maximum level S ∗, which is identical for all males: Si(t) p S ∗(1 ⫺ e⫺k it).

(1)

How fast S ∗ is reached is determined by the “quality parameter” ki ; thus, k L ! k H. In all examples below, we standardize S ∗ p 1. A female’s quality is defined by her fecundity, such that low- and high-quality females lay eL and eH eggs after mating, respectively. We assume 0 ! e L ! e H, and standardize e H p 1 such that females may become sperm limited (Wedell et al. 2002). While sperm reserves or egg amount cannot be directly observed, we assume that some phenotypic cues allow males and females to accurately assess the general quality (L or H) of potential mates before mating. Both males and females can mate repeatedly (fig. 1). At any one time, individuals are (i) single and resting after having mated (recovering); (ii) searching for a sexual partner (single); or (iii) mating (fig. 1). The rate Mij at which single males of quality i form couples with receptive females of quality j can be written as M ij p Gpij q jimiSfj S.

(2)

G is a constant meeting rate determined by the probability that a given male will find a given female within one time unit. This depends on the mobility of animals and how easily males detect females. Terms miS and fj S denote the number of single i-males and j-females. A male of quality i courts a j-female with probability pij , and the j-female

Prudent Male Courting and Sperm-Limited Females 261 accepts the courting male with probability qji (fig. 1; table 1 for a summary of our notation). If T is the average mating duration, the average rate of couple disassociation (fig. 1) can be calculated as 1/T (Kokko and Monaghan 2001). After mating, males recover sperm supplies during a time ri and enter the searching state at the rate 1/ri (fig. 1). Males are assumed to continue to recover sperm reserves while searching for females. Females replenish their (quality-specific) supply of eggs during a time r, which is equal for all females (fig. 1). The recruitment rate (I) is assumed to be identical for both sexes; that is, there is an even sex ratio. Mortality (m) is constant (see fig. 1). A population strategy p0 consists of the eight male and female mate choice probabilities pij and qji and two male recovery times ri , where (i, j) 苸 {L, H}2. For any given population strategy, the population stabilizes at stable numbers of single males and females and the other possible states. The numerical technique for finding this population structure follows Ha¨rdling and Kokko (2005). Let miR and mijM denote the number of recovering and mating males, respectively. At equilibrium, the following system of four dynamic equations is solved for the number of males in different states: ⫺mmiS ⫺ M iH ⫺ M iL ⫹

miR p ⫺I, ri

⫺mmiLM ⫺

miLM ⫹ M iL p 0, T

M ⫺mmiH ⫺

M miH ⫹ M iH p 0, T

(3)

miR miM ⫹ p 0. ri T

⫺mmiR ⫺

For females, an equivalent system of dynamic equations yields the number of females in each state: ⫺mfj S ⫺ MHj ⫺ MLj ⫹ f LjM

⫺mf ⫺ M Lj

⫺mf HjM ⫺

T f HjM

⫺mfj R ⫺

T fj R r

fj R r

p ⫺I,

⫹ MLj p 0,

(4)

⫹ MHj p 0, ⫹

fj M T

p 0.

As the dynamics of males and females are interdependent, the simultaneous solution to both systems is found nu-

merically by an iterative procedure where we repeatedly use the solution to the male (alternatively, the female) system (eq. [3]) to find the solution to the female (male) system (eq. [4]). This is done for high- and low-quality individuals so that the final solution solves a system of 16 linear equations with 16 unknown variables (2 sexes # 2 qualities # 4 states). The population structure stabilizes at a point where, not surprisingly, the numbers of mating males and females are the same ( 冘i, j mijM p 冘i, j fijM ). Next, we let all strategy parameters evolve in a game between males and females in order to find an evolutionarily stable strategy for all evolving parameters simultaneously (Houston and McNamara 1999). Mutant Fitness To find an evolutionarily stable strategy (ESS), we must derive an expression for the fitness of a mutant with a strategy p that deviates from the prevailing strategy p0 in the population (Maynard Smith 1982; Houston and McNamara 1999). We do this for both males and females in order to find a population strategy that is an ESS for both sexes simultaneously. A male’s fitness is proportional to his mating rate times the average offspring production of females he mates with. To calculate mating rate, consider a male of quality i using the mutant strategy p ( p0. His total life span can be divided up into time spent copulating with low- and highquality females (tiLM and tiHM), time spent recovering sperm (tiR), and time spent single (tiS). Observing that 1/m p (tiLM ⫹ tiHM ⫹ tiR ⫹ tiS), we can take advantage of the fact that for any subclass of males, such as reoccurring mutants with strategy p, the recruitment must equal the number that die at equilibrium. Thus, solving the dynamic system (3) for the strategy p and a recruitment rate I p 1 yields a solution that can be interpreted as the amount of time spent in each state (Ha¨rdling et al. 2004; Ha¨rdling and Kokko 2005). When deriving this solution, the prevailing strategy p0 defines the equilibrium number of single females. The number ni(p, p0) of mating events during the life of the mutant male is ni(p, p0 ) p

tiLM(p, p0 ) ⫹ tiHM(p, p0 ) ⫹ tiR(p, p0 ) . T ⫹ ri

(5)

The average mating rate yi(p, p0) (matings per time unit) is simply ni(p, p0) divided by the male life span (1/m): yi(p, p0 ) p

[tiLM(p, p0 ) ⫹ tiHM(p, p0 ) ⫹ tiR(p, p0 )]m . T ⫹ ri

(6)

The mean fitness gained by a male at each reproductive event can be calculated as the weighted average produc-

262 The American Naturalist

Figure 1: Flow diagram of the model. Individuals change state by moving in the direction shown by the arrows. For explanation of parameters, see table 1 and text.

tivity of high- and low-quality females. Because the number of offspring produced is equal to female fecundity unless the male does not deliver enough sperm to fertilize all of a female’s eggs, the offspring produced by a mating i-quality male and a j-quality female equals vij (p, p0 ) p min [e j , Si(ri(p, p0 ) ⫹ ¯tis(p, p0 ))].

(7)

Here ri is the mutant recovery time, and ¯tiS is the average time the male spends searching for a female. Assuming that the waiting times are exponentially distributed, this is the inverse of the per capita rate of mating: S

¯tiS(p, p0 ) p ti (p, p0 ) . 冘j M ij

(8)

Male mutant fitness Wm, i(p, p0) is thus Wm, i(p, p0 ) p yi(p, p0 )

viL(p, p0 )tiLM(p, p0 ) ⫹ viH(p, p0 )tiHM(p, p0 ) . (9) tiLM(p, p0 ) ⫹ tiHM(p, p0 )

Because females also mate repeatedly, female fitness depends on the rate at which she completes a mating cycle. Therefore, the fitness of a female mutant with a strategy differing from the prevailing one is computed in a way similar to that for males. Female variables and functions are denoted with a prime to separate them from male equivalents. Female remating rate yj(p, p0 ) is

yj(p, p0 ) p (t Lj (p, p0 ) ⫹ t Hj (p, p0 ) ⫹ t j (p, p0 ))m . T⫹r M

M

S

(10)

Fitness of a female mutant Wfj(p, p0) is Wfj (p, p0 ) p yj(p, p0 ) v Lj (p0 , p0 )t Lj (p, p0 ) ⫹ v Hj (p0 , p0 )t Hj (p, p0 ) # . M t M Lj (p, p0 ) ⫹ t Hj (p, p0 ) M

M

(11) Observe that the fitness of the female mutant is calculated assuming that the male partner uses the average population strategy. This way of formulating female fitness implies that a female needs to copulate once before each reproductive event; that is, females do not store sperm. We calculate the ESS numerically. First, after finding the stable population structure connected with the given population strategy p0 as described above, we use equations (9) and (11) to search for the best reply values (Houston and McNamara 1999) of the parameters pij , qji , and ri. To find the best reply for a single parameter, we pick the parameter value that maximizes mutant fitness when all other parameter values are kept at the population levels defined by p0. In each iteration in the numerical procedure that calculates the ESS, a new population strategy is cal-

Prudent Male Courting and Sperm-Limited Females 263 Table 1: Summary of the notation Symbol

Description

i, j Si(t) S∗ kL, kH eL, eH m Mij G mSi , fj S mRi , fj R M mM ij , fij pij qji T ri r tijM, t M ij tiR, t Rj tiS, t Sj ¯tiS ni yi, yj vij

Male and female quality 苸{low (L), high (H)} Sperm amount as a function of recovery time Maximum sperm reserve Sperm increase rate of low- and high-quality male Fecundity of low- and high-quality female Mortality rate Rate at which i-quality male and j-quality females form couples Meeting rate constant Number of single and sexually active males and females Number of individuals recovering after mating Number of mating individuals (for couple i, j) Probability that i-quality male courts j-quality female Probability that j-quality female accepts i-quality male Average mating duration Male recovering time Female recovering time Total time spent mating during life for males and females Total time spent recovering for males and females Total time spent as single for males and females Average time spent searching for mates Number of mating events Mating rate of males and females Offspring production by (i, j)-quality male and female

culated as a linear combination of the old parameter values ˆ , according to the formula (p0) and the best-reply values (m) ˆ p0(n ⫹ 1) p (1 ⫺ l)p0(n) ⫹ lp(n),

(12)

where l is a proportion. The new population strategy is then used to recalculate the stable population structure for the next iteration step. The algorithm at first did not stabilize at a single evolutionarily stable strategy, but we overcame this problem by defining l in equation (12) as an exponentially decreasing function (13) of the number of iterations (see Houston and McNamara 1999). l(n) p 0.05e [⫺ ln (0.05/0.002)/(400⫺1)](n⫺1).

(13)

The numerical procedure ensures that the ESS cannot be invaded by alternative strategies, because it checks that the fitness of all alternative strategies p is lower than the ESS strategy p ∗, that is, that W(p, p ∗) ! (p ∗, p ∗) for all p ( p ∗ (Maynard Smith 1982). However, although this procedure converges to an ESS, it finds only one ESS at a time if multiple solutions exist. Results To explore the dynamics of the model, we examined a large parameter range with varying male quality (k L ! k H ! 16 # k L); population density (or recruitment; 2.5 !

I ! 100); female recovery time (0.1 p T ! r ! 5 # T ); and female quality (0.1 ! e L ! e H p 1). We cannot present all these simulations, but since we observed very few qualitatively different mating patterns within this parameter range, we performed a final set of simulations using a full factorial combination of the following parameter values— I p {2.5, 100}; r p {0.15, 0.45}; e L p {0.1, 0.9}—and over the male quality range (k L ! k H ! 8 # k L). The results for these combinations capture the behavior of the model we have observed, and below we present the results for three factors that cause significant and qualitative changes in the stable solution to these simulations. These factors are (i) male differences in the sperm recovery rate, (ii) female quality differences as defined by their egg production, and (iii) female recovery time r. All results are presented as functions of male quality differences (figs. 2–4). For example, figure 2a shows the ESS male mate choice parameter pLL (i.e., the probability that a low-quality male courts a low-quality female) with the quality of a high-quality male (kH) and a low-quality male (kL) on the horizontal and vertical axes, respectively. High- and low-quality males are identical along the diagonal where k H p k L. Case 1: Male Quality Variation and Its Effect on Assortative Mating and Male Mating Rate The results presented in figure 2 are derived under the assumptions that females differ widely in quality (tenfold

Figure 2: Evolutionarily stable mating strategies as functions of male quality differences. In each panel, the horizontal and vertical axes denote the quality of a high- and a low-quality male, respectively. The degrees of shading in the panels depict pLL, the probability that a low-quality male courts a low-quality female (a), and qHL, the probability that a high-quality female accepts a low-quality male (b). c–f, Net mating rate between a lowquality male and a low-quality female (i.e., MLL; c); a low-quality male and a high-quality female (MLH; d); a high-quality male and a low-quality female (MHL; e); a high-quality male and a high-quality female (MHH; f ). The average sperm amount at mating is similar for high-quality (h; SH) and low-quality (g; SL) males when both court only high-quality females. With assortative mating for quality (i.e., areas where both c and f are white), the sperm amount at mating is lowered and unequal for low- and high-quality males. The parameter settings were eH p 1; eL p 0.1; r p 0.45; I p 2.5; m p 2.7; T p 0.1.

Prudent Male Courting and Sperm-Limited Females 265

Figure 3: Figure is similar to figure 2 but shows a lower difference between low- and high-quality females. Parameter settings: eH p 1; eL p 0.9; r p 0.45; I p 2.5; m p 2.7; T p 0.1.

difference in egg production) and that female recovery time (r) is 4.5 times as long as the mating duration (T). High-quality males court only high-quality females and never low-quality females (not shown). Low-quality males also court all available high-quality females, but whether they court low-quality females depends on the quality dif-

ference between males (fig. 2a). This is because female acquiescence is necessary for mating, and with increasing quality differences among males, high-quality females are less interested in accepting low-quality males as mates (fig. 2b). When quality differences among males become so large that high-quality females completely refuse to mate

266 The American Naturalist

Figure 4: Figure is similar to figure 2 but shows a shorter recovery time for females. Parameter settings: eH p 1; eL p 0.1; r p 0.15; I p 2.5; m p 2.7; T p 0.1.

with low-quality males (fig. 2b, lower right corner), lowquality males are forced to court and mate with low-quality females (fig. 2a). High-quality females always accept highquality males (not shown) and low-quality females are indiscriminate and accept mating attempts by both highand low-quality males (not shown). The net mating rates resulting from ESS male (M) and female (F) mate choices are shown in figure 2c–2f as fol-

lows: 2c, {M: low, F: low}; 2d, {M: low, F: high}; 2e, {M: high, F: low}; 2f, {M: high, F: high}. Large quality difference between males results in assortative mating between highand low-quality individuals (cf. fig. 2c and 2f ). When the quality difference between males is low or moderate, lowquality females are never courted, even if they would accept both high- and low-quality males as mates. Instead, males court only high-quality females, even if these are

Prudent Male Courting and Sperm-Limited Females 267 choosy and accept mainly high-quality males as mates (cf. fig. 2d and 2f ). The larger the difference between male qualities, the lower the probability that a low-quality male will be accepted as a mate. The ultimate factor determining whether a female chooses to mate is the expected amount of sperm that will be delivered by the male, since this is directly related to female fitness. The expected ejaculate size depends on the average time the male has to search before finding another mate (eq. [8]), which in turn depends on female and male mate choice. This interdependency is automatically taken into account in the model because of our inclusion of the population dynamics. The average sperm reserve a male has when mating is shown in figure 2g for low-quality males and in figure 2h for high-quality males. When males compete for access to high-quality females (low male quality difference), males have very similar sperm reserves when mating, even if high-quality males wait less time on average until finding a new mate (fig. 2g, 2h). When males differ widely in quality, assortative mating for quality relaxes competition between males for females. This results in an overall decrease in male waiting duration and, as a consequence, lowered sperm reserves when mating, in both high- and low-quality males (fig. 2g, 2h). This is costly for high-quality females, since it will cause sperm limitation and a decrease in fitness.

Case 3: Short Female Recovery Time Reducing Male Competition and the Extent of Assortative Mating Shortening the time necessary for females to build up a new batch of eggs (r) lowers the operational sex ratio (Emlen and Oring 1977), since more females are receptive at any one time. The important consequence of this is lowered male competition for mates, and high-quality females will then more readily accept low-quality males. In figure 4, r is reduced from 0.45 to 0.15, that is, only 50% longer than the mating duration, while there is a tenfold difference in quality between females, similar to “Case 1” above. The parameter region with assortative mating for quality (cf. fig. 2) is, as a result, almost completely lost. Low-quality males do not court low-quality females except when the quality difference between males is at its most extreme (fig. 4a). At the same time, high-quality females almost completely refuse to accept a low-quality male (fig. 4b). The resulting mating rates are seen in figure 4c–4f, which shows that matings occur almost exclusively with high-quality females. This is entirely due to male mate choice since low-quality females accept all courting males whether of high or low quality (not shown). The average sperm amount delivered at each mating (fig. 4g, 4h) is similar for males of low and high quality except with assortative mating as in (fig. 2). Discussion

Case 2: Male Competition Leading to Reduced Courting Activity in Low-Quality Males In figure 3, female quality classes differ by only 10% (e H p 1 and e L p 0.9), while the female recovery time is 4.5 times the mating duration, as in case 1 above. Therefore, male mate choice depends very little on female quality; males are equally willing to court low- and high-quality females and compete with each other for access to females. This male competition has surprising and differential effects on the mate choice tactics of low- and high-quality males. High-quality males court all encountered females (not shown). Females under almost all circumstances accept high-quality males (not shown). Low-quality males, in contrast, sometimes refrain from courting females they encounter (i.e., the ESS p Lj ! 1). This is in a parameter range where the rate of sperm recovery is relatively low for both high- and low-quality males (fig. 3a). The lower male mating rate results in increased sperm amount at copulation for low-quality males, which approaches that of high-quality males (cf. fig. 3g and 3h). Within the same parameter range, females always accept courting low-quality males (fig. 3b) such that the net rate of mating (fig. 3c, 3d) is dictated by male mating decisions.

We explore evolutionarily stable mutual mate choice strategies when optimal mating rate for both sexes is determined by a trade-off between average mate quality and number of reproductive events. We assume that male and female quality varies and that time requirements for regeneration of sperm give rise to a fitness opportunity costs in males. Female sperm limitation is assumed to occur if an ejaculate contains fewer sperm than necessary for fertilizing all eggs. The incentive to choose a mate is expected to increase with costs of breeding and differences in mate quality (Dewsbury 1982; Parker 1983; Johnstone et al. 1996; Bonduriansky 2001; Kokko and Johnstone 2002), while intense competition for mates in general should make individuals less choosy (Bonduriansky 2001; Servedio and Lande 2006). Our analysis adds to these insights by highlighting the important feedback between competition intensity, mate choice strategies, and male siring ability by explicitly incorporating the population dynamics and the life-history context of evolution. First, the number of receptive and mating females is decisive for the competitive situation among males (Houston and McNamara 1999; Williams et al. 2005). Mate choice strategies presented here are also linked to the effective male quality, namely, the average

268 The American Naturalist amount of sperm a male delivers when mating, because this is a function of the average time interval between matings for males (eq. [8]). Moreover, power relations of the sexes in a mating encounter (i.e., who decides whether to mate) is a result of this complex game that may have counterintuitive solutions (Williams et al. 2005). With large variation in female quality in our model and little difference in quality among males, all males will compete over the high-quality females (fig. 2). The average sperm reserve when mating is then more or less equal for high- and low-quality males (fig. 2g, 2h), because highquality females have control in the conflict over the occurrence of mating. This is because mating depends on female acquiescence, and the ESS for high-quality females is then to tune acceptance rates of males such that on average they receive the same benefit (i.e., the same number of fertilized eggs) when mating with a high- or lowquality male. Low-quality males are therefore accepted with a lower probability (fig. 2b). When males and females both vary greatly in quality, mating becomes assortative for quality so that high- and low-quality males target their “own” corresponding female quality class (fig. 2c, 2f ). Therefore, the intrasexual competition among males is lower than in the earlier case, and the average time between matings for males is shortened. The average amount of sperm in ejaculates is then, as a consequence, lower than before, and it also differs for the two types of males (fig. 2g, 2h). This will have a negative fitness effect on high-quality females, since it leads to sperm limitation, that is, a lower proportion of their eggs that are fertilized at each mating (Nakatsuru and Kramer 1982; Warner et al. 1995; Wedell et al. 2002). However, the lowered competition is positive for high-quality males because they experience a higher mating rate and receive higher net fitness. The results presented in figure 2 stress the importance of considering feedback influences: important features of male quality (fertilization rate) may be influenced by (mutual) mate choice strategies, and vice versa. Behavioral sexual conflict (i.e., antagonistic interactions) can be expected whenever males want to mate but females do not (Arnqvist and Rowe 2005). In the context of our model, this form of conflict over the occurrence of mating most commonly exists when all males compete over high-quality females. There will then often be conflicts of interest between females and low-quality males as low males will be forced to mate below their own optimal mating rate. This might lead to selection favoring males with a means of forcing a female to copulate, so-called male offense adaptations (Clutton-Brock and Parker 1995; Rice 1998). Strong male-male competition can lead to more indiscriminate male mate choice (Bonduriansky 2001; Servedio and Lande 2006) by increasing the oppor-

tunity costs of choice, and it will lead to assortative mating if both sexes vary in quality (Johnstone et al. 1996). With assortative mating, disagreement between males and females occurs in fewer encounters, and the frequency of antagonistic interactions between the sexes may therefore be reduced by strong male competition. Female mate choice has a surprising effect on competition among males if quality differences among females are small. In that case, both high- and low-quality males court both types of females (fig. 3). While high-quality males are always willing to mate, low-quality males under certain parameter settings have a reduced mating motivation (pm ! 1; fig. 3a). This is when male qualities are relatively low, or in other words, when the rate of sperm recovery is slow for both high- and low-quality males. We may interpret the lowered mating probability of low-quality males in this case as a tactic for increasing attractiveness and competitiveness (i.e., sperm amount in ejaculates) for two reasons. First, note that the lowered mating probability of low-quality males is counteracted by an increased female acceptance probability (fig. 3b). The prediction is that males have “control”; that is, mating is a male decision in this parameter region. The increase in male waiting time also increases the sperm amount at copulation for lowquality males to almost match that of high-quality males (cf. fig. 3g and 3h). Females therefore “gain” almost equally from mating with the two classes of males. It is known empirically that males can adjust sperm investment in order to maximize fitness, for example, by investing heavily in matings with virgin females while forgoing mating with unprofitable mated females (Wedell et al. 2002; Engqvist and Reinhold 2006; Ball and Parker 2007). In many insect species, waiting duration affects ejaculate size; for example, in water striders, the number of sperm transferred at mating increases with the male recovery period (Arnqvist and Danielsson 1999). This contributes to constraining the reproductive advantage of large males that have the highest copulation frequency (Rowe and Arnqvist 1996). Moreover, in three different species of water striders where large males copulated more frequently, small males copulated for a longer time and thus transferred a larger amount of sperm than large males, maybe because of longer average recovery period (Rowe and Arnqvist 1996; Arnqvist and Danielsson 1999). The pattern of small males copulating for a longer period than large males has also been seen in Scatophaga stercoraria (Ward and Simmons 1991) and Drosophila melanogaster (Pitnick 1991). We assume that a female needs a fixed time to regenerate a batch of eggs and must mate once before each time she oviposits. Her fitness increases with the number of times she completes a mating cycle. This corresponds to the situation in Nauphoeta cinerea (Harris and Moore 2005)

Prudent Male Courting and Sperm-Limited Females 269 and favors female choice for males that are able to fertilize all or most of her eggs. We also expect this assumption to apply to many other species where males have evolved efficient defense adaptations, such as mating plugs, to defend their sperm against female remating (Rice 1998; Wolfner 2002; Gillott 2003; Colonello and Hartfelder 2005). Even if females are under selection to avoid spermdepleted males (Wedell et al. 2002), we still expect to see few female rejections of low-quality males when these males tactically reduce their courting rate, thereby increasing their sperm number at mating. To interpret female choice pattern, it is therefore important to consider the fitness consequence of different female options and the mating history of males (Harris and Moore 2005). The number of females that are sexually receptive at any one time increases with decreasing female recovery time r. When females spend a short time recovering after mating, the operational sex ratio decreases, as does the competition among males for females and the male cost of choosing, namely, the fitness opportunity cost when males court only high-quality females. For this reason, the parameter region with assortative mating between highand low-quality individuals is greatly reduced when the female recovery time is short (fig. 4c, 4f ). High-quality females do not benefit by completely refusing to accept low-quality males as mates but instead accept them (albeit with a lower probability; fig. 4b). As a consequence, lowquality males are not forced to accept low-quality females, as when females have a long recovery time (cf. fig. 2). In accordance with the results presented above, the ESS sperm amount at mating is similar for both male quality classes as long as they compete over high-quality females, but when low-quality males choose to court low-quality females, average ejaculate size is greatly reduced (fig. 4g). Female sperm storage is common in various animal taxa (see references in Birkhead and Møller 1998) and may decrease the risk of sperm depletion and a female’s need to remate (see Birkhead and Møller 1998). We assume in the model that females mate with one male and do not store sperm between clutches, valid assumptions for some groups of insects (see references in Arnqvist et al. 2000; Harris and Moore 2005), organisms with efficient male sperm competition defense adaptations (Rice 1998), such as mating plugs (Ridley 1989; Wigby and Chapman 2004; Contreras-Garduno et al. 2006), certain mammals, and species such as fish with external fertilization (Levitan and Petersen 1995). However, a general effect of relaxing this assumption can be predicted, because including sperm storing ability will have an effect in our model similar to increasing the recovery time (r) for females, namely, to decrease the number of females that are receptive at any one time and increase the competition among males. This analogy means that we expect more pronounced female

choice for highly fertile males, which as a consequence could have led to assortative mating for quality (as in “Case 1” above). Another caveat is that males in the model are able to adjust only the amount of sperm transferred on mating (Wedell et al. 2002) by increasing the time spent recovering sperm supplies. Males of some species may be more versatile (e.g., Pizzari et al. 2003), which will, in general, reduce their opportunity cost of mating. Several models of mutual mate choice have considered monogamy and/or resource-demanding parental care, both of which are factors that increase the cost of making a bad mate choice (Ihara and Aoki 1999; Bergstrom and Real 2000; Kokko and Johnstone 2002). We have modeled a system with remating males and females and without parental care, showing that these systems can also allow mutual choice under some circumstances. Maria Servedio and Russell Lande (2006) recently published a population genetic model of males and mutual mate choice in a polygynous mating system. Their analysis focuses on evolution of preference for arbitrary traits in both sexes, and they concluded that this is unlikely. This is mainly because male-male competition causes direct fitness costs for males that do not court females at the maximum rate. For mutual choice to be stable, these costs must be outweighed, for example (as in our model), by courting more fecund females (Servedio and Lande 2006). According to our model, both high- and low-quality males transfer more sperm at each mating event when male competition for high-quality females is strong. This is also predicted by some sperm competition models (Parker et al. 1997; Parker 1998) as a male strategy for optimizing sperm allocation (Pizzari et al. 2003; delBarcoTrillo and Ferkin 2006). In our model, female choice is decisive. With strong male competition over high-quality females, sperm amount in ejaculates increases both because of automatic increases in male average time interval between matings and because some males evolve a reduced mating rate to increase their attractiveness to high-quality females. A possible test of our model could thus be to experimentally remove male-male competition and let each male have access to a large number of females to mate with. This is predicted to lead to a general decrease in the sperm investment per mating, but the decrease should be more pronounced for low-quality males or those males with the slowest rate of sperm recovery. As a possible secondary consequence of this, female reproduction may become sperm limited (Wedell et al. 2002). Acknowledgments S. Kuchta, E. Svensson, and M. Wellenreuther gave helpful comments. R.H. wishes to thank the Swedish Research Council for funding.

270 The American Naturalist Literature Cited Andersson, M. 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Arnqvist, G., and I. Danielsson. 1999. Postmating sexual selection: the effects of male body size and recovery period on paternity and egg production rate in a water strider. Behavioral Ecology 10:358– 365. Arnqvist, G., and L. Rowe. 2005. Sexual conflict. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Arnqvist, G., M. Edvardsson, U. Friberg, and T. Nilsson. 2000. Sexual conflict promotes speciation in insects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 97:10460–10464. Ball, M. A., and G. A. Parker. 2007. Sperm competition games: the risk model can generate higher sperm allocation to virgin females. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20:767–779. Bateman, A. J. 1948. Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. Heredity 2:349–368. Bergstrom, C. T., and L. A. Real. 2000. Towards a theory of mutual mate choice: lessons from two-sided matching. Evolutionary Ecology Research 2:493–508. Birkhead, T. R., and A. P. Møller, eds. 1998. Sperm competition and sexual selection. Academic Press, London. Bonduriansky, R. 2001. The evolution of male mate choice in insects: a synthesis of ideas and evidence. Biological Review 76:305–339. Clutton-Brock, T. H., and G. A. Parker. 1995 Sexual coercion in animal societies Animal Behaviour 49:1345–1365. Colonello, N. A., and K. Hartfelder. 2005. She’s my girl: male accessory gland products and their function in the reproductive biology of social bees. Apidologie 36:231–244. Contreras-Garduno, J., A. V. Peretti, and A. Cordoba-Aguilar. 2006. Evidence that mating plug is related to null female mating activity in the scorpion Vaejovis punctatus. Ethology 112:152–163. Crespi, B. J. 1989. Causes of assortative mating in arthropods. Animal Behavior 38:980–1000. Damiens, D., and G. Boivin. 2006. Why do sperm-depleted parasitoid males continue to mate? Behavioral Ecology 17:138–143. DelBarco-Trillo, J., and M. H. Ferkin. 2006. Male meadow voles respond differently to risk and intensity of sperm competition. Behavioral Ecology 17:581–585. Dewsbury, D. A. 1982. Ejaculate cost and male choice. American Naturalist 119:601–610. Eberhard, W. G. 1996. Female control: sexual selection by cryptic female choice. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Emlen, S. T., and L. W. Oring. 1977. Ecology, sexual selection, and evolution of mating systems. Science 197:215–223. Engqvist, L., and K. Reinhold. 2006. Theoretical influence of female mating status and remating propensity on male sperm allocation patterns. Jounal of Evolutionary Biology 19:1448–1458. Fawcett, T. W., and R. A. Johnstone. 2003. Mate choice in the face of costly competition. Behavioral Ecology 14:771–779. Gage, M. J. G., and P. A. Cook. 1994. Sperm size or numbers: effects of nutritional stress upon eupyrene and apyrene sperm production strategies in the moth Plodia interpunctella (Lepidoptera, Pyralidae). Functional Ecology 8:594–599. Gillott, C. 2003. Male accessory gland secretions: modulators of female reproductive physiology and behavior. Annual Review of Entomology 48:163–184. Ha¨rdling, R., and H. Kokko. 2005. The evolution of prudent choice. Evolutionary Ecology Research 7:697–715.

Ha¨rdling, R., H. Kokko, and R. W. Elwood. 2004. Priority versus brute force: when should males begin guarding resources? American Naturalist 163:240–252. Harris, W. E., and P. J. Moore. 2005. Female mate preference and sexual conflict: females prefer males that have had fewer consorts. American Naturalist 165(suppl.):S64–S71. Houston, A. I., and J. M. McNamara. 1999. Models of adaptative behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Ihara, Y., and K. Aoki. 1999. Sexual selection by male choice in monogamous and polygynous human populations. Theoretical Population Biology 55:77–93. Johnstone, R. A., J. D. Reynolds, and J. C. Deutsch. 1996. Mutual mate choice and sex differences in choosiness. Evolution 50:1382– 1391. Kirkpatrick, M., T. Price, and S. J. Arnold. 1990. The Darwin-Fisher theory of sexual selection in monogamous birds. Evolution 44: 180–193. Kokko, H., and R. A. Johnstone. 2002. Why is mutual mate choice not the norm? operational sex ratios, sex roles and the evolution of sexually dimorphic and monomorphic signalling. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 357:319– 330. Kokko, H., and P. Monaghan. 2001. Predicting the direction of sexual selection. Ecology Letters 4:159–165. Levitan, D. R., and C. Petersen. 1995. Sperm limitation in the sea. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 10:228–231. Løvlie, H., C. K. Cornwallis, and T. Pizzari. 2005. Male mounting alone reduces female promiscuity in the fowl. Current Biology 15: 1222–1227. Maynard Smith, J. 1982. Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Møller, A. P. 1991. Sperm competition, sperm depletion, paternal care, and relative testis size in birds. American Naturalist 137:882– 906. Nakatsuru, K., and D. L. Kramer. 1982. Is sperm cheap? limited male fertility and female choice in the lemon tetra (Pisces, Characidae). Science 216:753–755. Owens, I. P. F., and D. B. A. Thompson. 1994. Sex differences, sex ratios and sex roles. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 258:93–99. Parker, G. A. 1979. Sexual selection and sexual conflict. Pages 123– 166 in M. S. Blum and N. A. Blum, eds. Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects. Academic Press, New York. ———. 1983. Mate quality and mating decisions. Pages 141–166 in P. Bateson, ed. Mate choice. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. ———. 1990. Sperm competition games: raffles and roles. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 242:120–126. ———. 1998. Sperm competition and the evolution of ejaculates: towards a theory base. Pages 3–54 in T. R. Birkhead and A. P. Møller, eds. Sperm competition and sexual selection. Academic Press, London. Parker, G. A., M. A. Ball, P. Stockley, and M. J. G. Gage. 1997. Sperm competition games: a prospective analysis of risk assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 264:1793–1802. Pitnick, S. 1991. Male size influences mate fecundity and remating interval in Drosophila melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 41:735–745. Pizzari, T., C. K. Cornwallis, H. Løvlie, S. Jakobsson, and T. R. Birkhead. 2003. Sophisticated sperm allocation in male fowl. Nature 426:70–74. Preston, B. T., I. R. Stevenson, J. M. Pemberton, and K. Wilson. 2001.

Prudent Male Courting and Sperm-Limited Females 271 Dominant rams lose out by sperm depletion: a waning success in siring counters a ram’s high score in competition for ewes. Nature 409:681–682. Rice, W. R. 1998. Intergenomic conflict, interlocus antagonistic coevolution, and the evolution of reproductive isolation. Pages 261– 270 in D. J. Howard and S. H. Berlocher, eds. Endless forms: species and speciation. Oxford University Press, New York. Ridley, M. 1989. The incidence of sperm displacement in insects: four conjectures, one corroboration. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 38:349–367. Rowe, L., and G. Arnqvist. 1996. Analysis of the causal components of assortative mating in water striders. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 38:279–286. Royer, L., and J. N. McNeil. 1993. Male investment in the European corn-borer, Ostrinia nubilalis (Lepidoptera, Pyralidae): impact on female longevity and reproductive performance. Functional Ecology 7:209–215. Servedio, M. R., and R. Lande. 2006. Population genetic models of male and mutual mate choice. Evolution 60:674–685. Thornhill, R., and J. Alcock. 1983. The evolution of insect mating systems. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Van Voorhies, W. A. 1992. Production of sperm reduces nematode lifespan. Nature 360:456–458.

Ward, P. I., and L. W. Simmons. 1991. Copula duration and testes size in the yellow dung fly, Scatophaga stercoraria (L.): the effects of diet, body size and mating history. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 29:77–85. Warner, R. R., D. Y. Shapiro, A. Marcanato, and C. W. Petersen. 1995. Sexual conflict: males with highest mating success convey the lowest fertilization benefits to females. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 262:135–139. Wedell, N., M. J. G. Gage, and G. A. Parker. 2002. Sperm competition, male prudence and sperm-limited females. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 17:313–320. Wigby, S., and T. Chapman. 2004. Sperm competition. Current Biology 14:100–102. Williams, P. D., T. Day, and E. Cameron. 2005. The evolution of sperm-allocation strategies and the degree of sperm competition. Evolution 59:492–499. Wolfner, M. F. 2002. The gifts that keep on giving: physiological functions and evolutionary dynamics of male seminal proteins in Drosophila. Heredity 88:85–93.

Associate Editor: Go¨ran Arnqvist Editor: Michael C. Whitlock

Male Mating Constraints Affect Mutual Mate Choice ...

In all examples below, we stan- k ! k. L. H dardize . ∗. S p 1. A female's quality is defined by her fecundity, such that low- and high-quality females lay eL and eH ... A population strategy p0 consists of the eight male and female mate choice probabilities pij and qji and two male recovery times ri, where . For any given pop-. 2.

568KB Sizes 1 Downloads 137 Views

Recommend Documents

an evolutionary limit to male mating success
KEY WORDS: Artificial selection, evolutionary potential, fitness, sexual selection. Determining how much ... may be genetic variation in male fitness (McCleery et al. 2004;. Qvarnstrom et al ...... SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC. Mackay, T. F. C., S.

mate choice and human stature: homogamy as a ... - Semantic Scholar
Assortative mating for human height has long attracted interest in evolutionary biology, and the phenomenon has been demon- strated in numerous human populations. It is often argued that mating preferences generate this pattern, but other processes c

mate choice and human stature: homogamy as a ...
We collected data from a sample of 200 individuals from France. ..... easily estimated by fitting the previous model using general tools ..... visual representations of the same data are given in each row: the three-dimensional plot and the ...

Preferences and Choice Constraints in Marital Sorting ...
Nov 26, 2007 - Male high school graduates prefer female high school graduates, while .... between 2000 and 2002 (Fair Trade Commission, 2004). ... A membership contract, which guarantees service for one year, costs 900,000 won (as.

Mating tactics of male feral goats (Capra hircus): risks ...
Oct 25, 2005 - Statistical analysis. We used SAS and SPSS for data analysis, with generalized ... which data were nested either within males that were present.

Message Mate
Original outline copyright © 1985 and Message Mate copyright © 2016 by Charles .... TEXT: Martin Luther (1483–1546); translated (1852) by Frederick H. Hedge ...

Message Mate
Duplication of copyrighted material for commercial use is strictly prohibited. Committed to Excellence in Communicating Biblical Truth and Its Application. MM07.

Message Mate
Spiritually: Tender — hungry, ready to believe, quick to respond, creative. 2. The Difficult Part: Childishness (Isaiah 30:1 – 2, 8 – 9). We must curb and control ...

Message Mate
After arriving in Jerusalem, Nehemiah's first order of business was to seek silence and ... line the reputation of the promise-keeping God of Israel. Nehemiah had a ... For these and related resources, visit www.insightworld.org/store or call USA ...

Message Mate
leadership or the skills and experiences we bring to the table, we can learn ... of excellent leadership must be placed on a foundation of God's Word and ... service, Nehemiah received devastating news concerning Jerusalem's broken walls ...

Message Mate
Duplication of copyrighted material for commercial use is strictly prohibited. Committed to Excellence in Communicating Biblical Truth and Its Application. MM09.

Message Mate
God's grace filled the church and became a bridge of respect and trust ... of what writer Howard Snyder calls kingdom people rather than church people.

Message Mate
The word channels refers to canals or irrigation ditches that run in various ... We can stretch to its breaking point this tension between divine sovereignty and ...

Message Mate
the family of God . . . yet how rare! ... GROWING UP IN GOD'S FAMILY ... copyright © 1985 and Message Mate copyright © 2016 by Charles R. Swindoll, Inc.

Statistical Constraints
2Insight Centre for Data Analytics, University College Cork, Ireland. 3Institute of Population Studies, Hacettepe University, Turkey. 21st European Conference on ...

Does the contraceptive pill alter mate choice in humans?
wish to undermine the crucial social and medical positive associates of ... 0169-5347/$ – see front matter © 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.08.003 Available online xxxxxx. 1 ..... differ in other ways, such as thei

pdf mate
Page 1 of 1. File: Pdf mate. Download now. Click here if your download doesn't start automatically. Page 1 of 1. pdf mate. pdf mate. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu. Displaying pdf mate. Page 1 of 1.

UTI Mutual Fund - NSE
Jun 28, 2018 - Sub: Revocation of suspension of trading in units - on account of Interval Scheme – UTI. Mutual Fund. It is hereby notified that the suspension of ...

Mutual consent.pdf
The memorandum of settlement has been produced. before this Court. 3. It is submitted that the monetary part of the. settlement has been complied with and what remains is only. order on the pending criminal cases and also the application. for divorce

Network Constraints
gate J which is to realize gj at its output. Gates are thus likewise numbered from n to m. Each gate of a partial solution is assigned a number of inputs which may ...

Mitochondrial DNA phylogeography and mating compatibility ... - MEFGL
Abstract. The marine bryozoan Celleporella hyalina is a species complex composed of many highly divergent and mostly allopatric genetic lineages that are reproductively isolated but share a remarkably similar morphology. One such lineage commonly enc

Mitochondrial DNA phylogeography and mating compatibility ... - MEFGL
of the eastern and western Atlantic fringes (Cunningham. & Collins 1998), additional data from the former provides .... published sequence data from populations of Iceland,. Oban, Achill, Amlwch, Spain and The Dorn (accession nos .... scraped off the

Mitochondrial DNA phylogeography and mating ...
between terrestrial and marine system responses to Pleistocene glacial ..... input file. The statistical distribution of the distance measures ... all native localities.