Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

Personal variables, motivation and avoidance learning strategies in undergraduate students Fernando Doménech-Betoret a,⁎, Amparo Gómez-Artiga b,1, Susana Lloret-Segura c,2 a b c

Dept. of Educational and Developmental Psychology, Universitat Jaume I, Castellón, Spain Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universitat de València (Estudi General), València, Spain Methodology of the Behavioral Sciences, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history: Received 25 June 2013 Received in revised form 27 May 2014 Accepted 28 June 2014 Keywords: Instructional model Theoretical framework Research in the classroom Higher education Educational setting

a b s t r a c t This study examines the relationships among students' personal variables, their initial motivation and the avoidance learning strategies they used during the teaching/learning process followed in the Educational Psychology subject matter. The sample comprised 195 Spanish undergraduate students who studied Educational Psychology. A questionnaire was administered at the beginning of the academic year to measure students' personal variables and their initial motivation, while another was administered at the end of the academic year to measure students' involvement in their learning process through the avoidance strategies they used. The data analysis was done by using structural equation modeling. The results reveal important associations among students' personal variables, their motivation at the beginning of the academic year and the avoidance strategies used during the learning process followed in the Educational Psychology subject matter. The implications of these findings for teaching and learning a specific subject matter in formal classroom contexts are discussed. © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction The main objective of this research is to better understand why certain students, even those in the same educational setting, get involved and make an effort to learn using all the resources and strategies they have available, while others make the minimum effort and work to learn. This phenomenon, known by the terms avoidance strategies or work avoidance, reflects passivity or inaction and, when applied to education, refers to those strategies that students should use to learn, but they do not use them. Previous research has suggested that “work avoidance may be an academic goal in which students seek to minimize the amount of work they do at school” (Seifert & O'Keefe, 2001, p. 81). In order to study this phenomenon, we firstly revised the literature to identify, select, and later evaluate, the most characteristic behaviors that describe students who use avoidance strategies. Secondly, we designed an explicative causal model of such behavior by taking the Educational Situation Quality Model (MCSE ‘Modelo de Calidad de Situación Educativa’), developed by Doménech (2006, 2011a,b, 2012,

⁎ Corresponding author at: Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva, Educativa, Social i Metodología, Universitat Jaume I, Campus Riu Sec, 12071 Castellón, Spain. Tel.: +34 964 72 95 50; fax: +34 964 72 92 62. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (F. Doménech-Betoret), [email protected] (A. Gómez-Artiga), [email protected] (S. Lloret-Segura). 1 Tel.: +34 96 386 44 20x4709; fax: +34 96 386 46 71. 2 Tel.: +34 96 3864585; fax: +34 963864697.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.06.007 1041-6080/© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

2013), as a reference, whose characteristics are briefly commented on below. The main contribution of this study is to help explain why students use avoidance strategies, which undermine their performance and limit their capacity to learn. As we become capable of understanding why students use avoidance strategies that make their learning difficult by identifying the variables responsible for this behavior, we will be able to design preventive actions in the classroom to improve students' implication and efficacy in their learning. Despite the importance of this issue due to the practical implications derived, studies centered on avoidance strategies are relatively scarce, especially in higher education. 1.1. Educational Situation Quality Model (MCSE ‘Modelo de Calidad de Situación Educativa’) The MCSE could be defined as an instructional model which simultaneously considers the teaching and learning process where motivation plays a central role. It also provides a methodological way to research in the educational setting. As seen in Fig. 1, the model is made up of five blocks of variables arranged into three major sequential phases: input, process and product. The components and variables of the model considered in this study are explained below. 1.1.1. The input phase: personal variables (Block 1) Below, we briefly comment on students' personal variables from Block 1.

F. Doménech-Betoret et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129

123

Fig. 1. Educational Setting Quality Model (MCSE): Organization and functional relationship of input, process and product (a revised version of Doménech, 2006).

1.1.1.1. Prior knowledge about the subject. According to the constructivist learning approach, prior knowledge facilitates new learning and students' comprehension. Research has verified that individuals with greater prior knowledge of a topic understand and remember more than those with more limited prior knowledge (Schneider & Pressley, 1997). Furthermore, prior knowledge in specific domains has a positive influence on students' learning and achievement (Alexander & Judy, 1988; Dochy, Segers, & Buehl, 1999; Thompson & Zamboanga, 2004). 1.1.1.2. Interest in the subject. The beneficial effects of interest in learning are well-documented (Renninger & Hidi, 2002). The Person–Object Conception of Interest (POI) was developed in the Educational Psychology field. In accordance with this conception, researchers usually adopt the distinction between situational and individual. In the past, many studies have focused on examining the influence of individual interest on learning process and achievement, and the obtained results have verified that level of interest accounts for about 10% of the variance observed in achievement (Schiefele, Krapp, & Winteler, 1992). 1.1.1.3. Beliefs about the formative scope of the subject. The value that a particular task or subject has for students is an important stimulus to generate motivation (Pintrich, 1989; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990). Yet the value that students attribute to a particular subject depends, to a certain extent, on the utility that they perceive it has for them in terms of developing their skills both professionally and personally. 1.1.1.4. General academic self-efficacy. Self-efficacy is a component of Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) (1986) and is defined as “an individual's belief in his or her own ability to organize and implement action to produce the desired achievements and results” (Bandura, 1997, p. 3). Perceived self-efficacy that a person has of one's own capabilities to perform or undertake a task increases the likelihood of the task being successfully performed (Bandura, 1986). Prior research has verified that students' self-efficacy beliefs are associated with other motivation constructs and with students' academic performances and achievement (Pajares, 1996, 1997). 1.1.1.5. General self-esteem. Self-esteem is the evaluative component of Self-concept, which can be defined as “the positivity of a person's evaluation of self” (Baumeister, 1998, p. 694). Self-esteem has been associated with learning strategies (Núñez et al., 1998), with academic achievement (Mestre, García, Frías, & Llorca, 1992) and with goal

progress and intrinsic motivation at school (Vasalampi, Salmela-Aro, & Nurmi, 2010). 1.1.2. The initial phase of the process: motivational positioning variables (Block 3) The motivational positioning variables (MPV), generated by students at the beginning of the educational process, refer to their initial expectations or ideas about how the teaching/learning process, with a specific subject matter and a specific teacher, will be implemented. This idea may have been generated before classes began, caused by previous experiences with a similar content, or be based on the information students already have about the teacher, etc. It may also arise on the first days of class when they meet the teacher and find out about the study syllabus, evaluation requirements, how the teacher is going to conduct the class, etc. Students now have sufficient information to enable them to answer four important implicit questions deriving from the motivational theory proposed by Pintrich (1989), Pintrich and De Groot (1990), and from the Expectancy-Value Theory proposed by Feather (1982) and Vroom (1964): a) Will I be successful in this subject? b) What value has this subject for me? c) How will I feel studying this subject, and d) How much time and effort will I devote to studying this subject according to the value it has for me? This same process similarly occurs with the teacher, but is not considered in this study. 1.1.3. The interactive involvement phase: avoidance learning strategies (Block 4) The interactive involvement phase (Block 4) refers to the way that the teacher and students interact though a specific curricular content; that is to say, everything the teacher does to teach and everything students do to learn that particular content. The degree of teacher and student involvement in this phase and the degree of their interrelationship will depend on the expectations generated by them in the previous phase (the initial positioning phase). The present study centered on what students do to avoid learning, specifically on the avoidance learning strategies they used during the teaching/learning process undertaken in Educational Psychology. Previous motivational research has suggested that work avoidance in the school context is an academic goal in which students seem to make little effort to understand or complete academic tasks (Jarvis & Seifert, 2002; Seifert & O'Keefe, 2001). Seifert and O'Keefe (2001) indicated that students tend to avoid effort or minimize the amount of work they do at school for three major reasons: feeling of competence, boredom and lack of control. Students

124

F. Doménech-Betoret et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129

who perceive little control or competence seek to avoid effort because they believe they cannot do the work, or because they want to feel protected from the humiliation and shame associated with failure (Covington, 1984). Moreover, the students who perceive themselves capable of doing a task, but do not see any reason for doing it (for instance, because they find that the task is meaningless), also try to avoid making efforts in their learning process (Seifert & O'Keefe, 2001). Sloan (2007) found that motivational traits are one of the major reasons that teachers used to explain why students avoid work. Finally, attendance in classroom sessions of the Educational Psychology subject matter was also taken into account since it can be understood as a kind of avoidance strategy. 1.2. Objectives and hypothesis The main aim of this study was to examine the process which explains why undergraduate students employ avoidance learning strategies. Based on the aforementioned rationale, the hypothesized connections have been addressed and tested simultaneously using the Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) procedure. The causal model presented (see Fig. 2) has been designed in accordance with the configuration of the MCSE displayed in Fig. 1. As seen in the hypothesized model, the following predictions have been addressed. First, personal variables (from Block 1, the input phase) such as Prior knowledge, Interest in the subject, General Academic Self-Efficacy, Formative scope of the subject, General Self-Esteem, were expected to be good predictors of the students' initial motivation or intention to learn at the beginning of the T/L process (from Block 3, the preprocess phase) in Educational Psychology. Second, the students' initial motivation (from Block 3, the preprocess phase) was expected to be a good predictor of avoidance strategies and classroom attendance (from Block 4, the process phase).

2. Method 2.1. Participants and procedure The sample consisted of 195 psychology students, of whom 159 were female (81.5%) and 36 male (18.5%), who were aged between 20 and 47 years old (M: 22.78, SD: 3.83). The participants studied Educational Psychology during the 2010–11 and 2011–12 academic years at the Universitat Jaume I, Castellón (East Spain). Educational Psychology is an annual core subject and is taught in the third year of the Psychology degree. The study was carried out during two consecutive academic years in the Educational Psychology subject matter (an annual core subject taught in the third year of the Psychology degree). Questionnaires were administered twice, at the start (time 1) and at the end (time 2) of the teaching/learning process conducted in this subject matter. The teacher, the methodology and the evaluation system were the same in the studied educational settings. As the effect of these variables was neutralized, the classes studied can be treated as a unique educational setting. 2.2. Measures I would like to underline that all the scales used have been reviewed and refined in previous studies (Doménech, 2006, 2011a,b, 2012, 2013) based on the MCSE model. Nevertheless, we tested the adequacy of these measures in a two-stage process. First, an explorative factor analysis, the principal component method with varimax rotation, was conducted in all the scales to check their construct validity. An observed measure was obtained by averaging the items included in each factor or subscale. Second, we included these subscales as the observed variables in the measurement model part of the structural model tested.

Fig. 2. Hypothesized Model (HM). Relationship among students' personal variables (block 1), students' positioning variables (block 3) and avoidance strategies (block 4). All the effects among the five personal variables and the four students' positioning variables are hypothesized as positive; all the effects among the four students' positioning variables on classroom attendance are hypothesized as positive, while the effect of these variables on avoidance strategies are hypothesized as negative.

F. Doménech-Betoret et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129

This procedure avoids including every single item in the measurement model, thus reducing the number of estimated parameters. Scales used are briefly described on below, for more information see Table 1. 2.2.1. Input phase (time 1): students' personal variables scales (SPVS) 2.2.1.1. Prior knowledge scale (3 items). This scale was used to assess the prior knowledge about the subject matter taught that students had at the beginning of the academic year; that is, Educational psychology. Students indicated their level of agreement on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (I am not convinced) to 4 (I am absolutely convinced). A single component was extracted from the exploratory factor analysis. 2.2.1.2. Student interest scale (5 items). The interest scale was used to assess the extent of student interest in the content of the subject matter. Students indicated their level of agreement, ranging from 1 (completely false) to 6 (completely true). A single component was extracted from the exploratory factor analysis. 2.2.1.3. Scale of beliefs about the formative scope of the subject matter (8 items). The Subject formative scope scale was used to assess the extent that students believed at the beginning of the academic year that this subject would provide them with effective formative scope to succeed in their future job. Students indicated their level of agreement within a range from 1 (minimum formative scope) to 4 (much formative scope). The results obtained from the exploratory factor analysis conducted indicated the bidimensional structure of the scale. 2.2.1.4. Students' general academic self-efficacy scale (20 items). This scale was adapted by Doménech (2011a) for Spanish university students based

125

on the original scales created by Bandura (1990) and Pastorelli et al. (2001). This subscale was used to assess students' self-perception of how competent they were in the academic field. Students indicated their level of agreement within a range from 1 (Very bad) to 4 (Very good). Five factors corresponding to the five academic skills were extracted from a second exploratory factor analysis conducted on the remaining 19 items. 2.2.1.5. General self-esteem scale (10 items). One of the most extensively used instruments to assess self-esteem is the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965, 1989). This instrument was translated into Spanish and validated in the university context by Martín-Albo, Núñez, Navarro, and Grijalvo (2007). The Spanish version was used in this study. Students indicated their level of agreement within a range from 1 (completely disagree) to 6 (completely agree). The exploratory factor analysis conducted supported the two-factor structure of the scale (positively orientated and negatively orientated) in line with previous research in Spanish university students (see Martín-Albo et al., 2007). 2.2.2. The initial phase of the process (time 1): the motivational positioning variables scale (MPVS) We designed this scale composed of 17 items to measure students' initial motivation (through MPV), with the subject being taught, which was generated at the beginning of the teaching/learning process. It was structured and designed based on the Motivational Theory proposed by Pintrich (1989) and Pintrich and De Groot (1990). Students indicated their level of agreement on a 5-point Likert scale within a range from 1 (I am absolutely not convinced) to 5 (I am absolutely convinced). The exploratory factor analysis conducted supported the 4-factor structure of the scale.

Table 1 Summary of the factor analysis, internal consistency and item example of the scales. Scales

Factors

Input phase: Students' personal variables Prior Knowledge

1

M

S.D.

Variance

Cronbach's α

Item exemple

3

2.89

0.52

65.79

.72

5 8 5 3 19 5 4 4 3 3 10 5 5

3.64

1.25

.88

2.16 2.67

.54 .65

.87 .83

“It will help me to live and live alongside others” “It will help me become a good professional”

2.93 3.17 3.24 2.83 2.82

.61 .53 .43 .55 .56

.85 .81 .76 .83 .71

“How good are you at making summaries to help you study?” “How well do you memorize what you study for an exam?” “How well do you cope with teamwork with colleagues?” “How well do you express what you want to say in writing?” “How do you cope in exam situations?”

3.26 2.85

.44 .28

67.95 65.34 38.98 26.36 67.13 17.27 13.94 12.91 12.51 10.48 58.43 29.34 29.09

“The knowledge acquired in previous academic years will help you learn this subject matter?” “I would not like to work as a psychologist in a school”

1 2

.83 .77

“I think that I have a good number of qualities” “I sometimes feel really useless”

17 5 4 4 4

2.95 2.60 3.02 2.49

.64 .56 .46 .59

76.30 22.83 21.52 17.53 14.89

.93 .91 .88 .86

“The time and effort you must devote to pass this subject will be too much ?” “How useful is this subject for you?” “Do you think you will be able to obtain good marks for this subject?” “Do you think you will find this subject enjoyable?”

26 6 6

3.04 1.56

.68 .46

61.77 13.98 13.78

.85 .82

F3: Effort and challenges

6

2.10

.58

12.95

.85

F4: Novelty

4

2.18

.59

11.75

.81

“I played a passive role during classes” “I have actively collaborated with my colleagues in teamwork tasks” (reverse-coded) “I have decided to do those tasks which are easier for me so I can avoid difficult challenges” “I preferred those activities which I found familiar than those which introduced new

procedures and concepts” F5: Help seeking

4

2.08

.66

9.29

.76

Interest Beliefs about the Formative Scope of the subject F1: Personal development F2: Professional development General Academic Self-Efficacy F1: Verbal skills F2: Study techniques F3: Team work skills F4: Memorization capacity F5: Coping with exam situations General Self-Esteem F1: Positively orientated items F2: Negatively orientated items Initial process phase: Initial motivation Motivational variables of positioning F1: Time and effort expected to be done F2: Subject value F3: Achievement expectations F4: Enjoyable learning expectations Process phase: Students' learning strategies Students' avoidance strategies scale F1: Participation during the classroom F2: Team work collaboration

5

2

4

5

Items (n)

“When I did not know how to do a task, I tried to do it my way instead of asking for help”

−.053 −.319⁎⁎ −.215⁎⁎ −.203⁎ .174⁎ .082 −.241⁎⁎ −.077 −.105 .169⁎ −.070 −.089 −.111 −.031 −.074

−.089 .008 −.056 .014 −.004

.032 .042 .105 .000 .134 .059

.097 −.100 .017 −.128 −.104 −.076 .069 −.046 −.134 −.039 −.060 .101 −.065 −.092 −.025 −.067 .094

Gender: 1 Male; 2 Female. ⁎ p b .05. ⁎⁎ p b .01.

−.129 .010 −.040 −.031 −.049 −.021 .052 −.086 −.027 .158⁎ .018 −.042 .110 .119 .121 .012 .093 .096 .138 −.288⁎⁎

−.078 −.070 −.199⁎ −.215⁎⁎ .128 −.144

.029 −.168⁎ .044 −.100 .257⁎⁎

.057 −.014 −.095 −.025 −.026 .092 −.026

−.076 −.160⁎

.066 .360⁎⁎ .224⁎⁎

−.047 .486⁎⁎ −.219⁎⁎

−.037 .228⁎⁎

−.025 −.285⁎⁎ −.071 −.145 .287⁎⁎

−.105 .021 −.020 −.012 .046 .032

−.082 −.051 −.065 −.028 −.097 −.024 .032

−.033 .443⁎⁎ −.330⁎⁎ −.006 .506⁎⁎ −.240⁎⁎

.116 .506⁎⁎

.088 −.160⁎ .304⁎⁎ −.189⁎

.091 −.030 −.108 .075 −.138 −.262⁎⁎ .150 .102 .063 .324⁎⁎

−.069 −.143 .159⁎ .725⁎⁎

.160⁎ −.006 .083 .256⁎⁎ .035 .326⁎⁎ .268⁎⁎ −.149⁎ .046 −.116 −.154⁎

.054 .076 −.043 .107 .066 .166⁎

−.027 .014 .118 .117 .134 .146 .094 .234⁎⁎

−.065 −.033 .053 −.155⁎ −.160⁎ .461⁎⁎ .484⁎⁎

3 2 Gend.

1 1 −.145⁎

A bivariate correlational analysis was carried out as an approach to explore the relationships between input (students' personal variables), students' initial motivation (MPV) and the learning process (avoidance strategies) followed by students. In general, positive and significant correlations were obtained between personal variables and students' initial motivation, the most remarkable was the one found between interest and value of the subject (r = .725, p b 0.01). Negative and significant correlations were also obtained between students' initial motivation (MPV) and the avoidance strategies used by students, the most

Age

3.2. Correlation between variables

Age Gender 1. Prior Know. 2. Interest 3. Study tech. 4. Memorizat. 5. Team work 6. Verbal skills 7. Cop. exams 8. Prof. Devel. 9. Pers. Devel. 10. S-E posit. 11. S-E negat. 12. Ex. effort 13. Value 14. Achiev. Ex. 15. Enjoy L. Ex. 16. Av. Partic. 17. Av. Collab 18. Av. Effort 19 Av. Novel. 20. Av. Help 21. Attendance

The mean, standard deviation, reliability, structure of the scales and item examples are shown in Table 1. In general, the factorial analyses performed confirmed the scales' original structure and configuration. Cronbach's alpha coefficients indicated good internal consistency for all the scales with a 0.71 to 0.94 range. See Table 1 for more details.

Table 2 Pearson's bivariate correlations between the constructs considered.

3.1. Descriptive statistics and internal consistency of scales

−.021 .000 .218⁎⁎ .193⁎ .202⁎⁎

4

3. Results

.043 .043 .170⁎

.128 .205⁎⁎ .219⁎⁎

5

.235⁎⁎ .160⁎ −.016 −.034 .135 .114 −.040 .065 .093 .022 −.197⁎

6

.368⁎⁎ −.263⁎⁎ −.099 .214⁎⁎

7

−.230⁎⁎ −.264⁎⁎ .451⁎⁎ .280⁎⁎

8

.596⁎⁎ −.218⁎⁎ −.166⁎

9

−.116 −.128 .089 .606⁎⁎

10

The Maximum Likelihood (ML) Robust method of estimation, developed by Satorra and Bentler (1988, 1994), appears to be a good approach when the multivariate normality assumption is violated and the sample size is small (Curran, West, & Finch, 1996; Hu, Bentler, & Kano, 1992). This study suffers from both problems, as is frequently the case in social sciences. Sample size is slightly under the widely recommended value of 200 subjects (Hoe, 2008). Regarding the normality assumption, all except one of the variables in the study did not conform to it. “Interest” was the only variable that did it (for this variable, the K-S test was nonsignificant, p = .398). Therefore, the ML robust method of estimation was used to test the hypothesized connections by means of the EQS program (Bentler, 1995, 2006). Since the chi-square test is sensitive to sample size, the use of relative fit indices, such as the CFI, the NNFI and the RMSEA, is strongly recommended (Bentler, 1990a,b). Values smaller than .05 for RMSEA indicate a good fit, whereas values of up to .08 indicate a poor fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993). For NNFI and CFI, values greater than .90 (Hoyle, 1995) or even .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999) have been fixed as the cutting-off point.

.540⁎⁎ .162⁎

11

.093 −.175⁎ .309⁎⁎

2.3. Statistics analyses

−.093 −.060 .063 .152 −.040 .053 −.049

12

.153 .190⁎ .291⁎⁎

13

−.009 .582⁎⁎ −.321⁎⁎

14

.162⁎ −.003 −.028 −.204⁎ −.141 .088 −.074

15

−.256⁎⁎ .085 −.201⁎ −.057 −.145 .255⁎⁎

16

.069 .186⁎ .155 .289⁎⁎ −.335⁎⁎

17

.132 .029 .043 .006

18

.612⁎⁎ .282⁎⁎ −.051

19

.206⁎ −.013

20

2.2.3. The interactive involvement phase (time 2): the student avoidance strategies scale (SASS) The second questionnaire was made up of five scales: avoiding novelty (6 items); avoiding help seeking (6 items); avoiding effort and challenges (6 items); avoiding participation in the classroom (6 items); avoiding peer collaboration (6 items). The first two avoidance strategies scales (avoiding novelty and avoiding help seeking) are a version (Doménech, 2011a) for the Spanish university context that was adapted from those used by Turner et al. (2002) for sixth-grade elementary school students. The remaining scales (avoiding effort and challenges, avoiding participation, and avoiding peer collaboration) were created for the present study. Students indicated their level of agreement on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Totally disagree) to 5 (Totally agree). Five factors were extracted from a second exploratory factor analysis conducted on the remaining 26 items, corresponding to the five avoidances strategies considered. Finally, attendance in the classroom sessions of the Educational Psychology course was also taken into account since it can be understood as a kind of avoidance strategy. Therefore, students were required to inform about how frequently they attended these classroom sessions during the course on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never/hardly ever) to 4 (always/almost always).

-.088

F. Doménech-Betoret et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129

−.064 .362⁎⁎

126

F. Doménech-Betoret et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129

127

Fig. 3. Optimized Model (OM). Relationship among students' personal variables (block 1), students' positioning variables (block 3) and avoidance strategies (block 4). Structural configuration and standardized coefficients of the optimized model are displayed.

remarkable was the one found between value of the subject and avoiding participation (r = .321, p b 0.01). See Table 2 for more details. 3.3. Structural equation modeling Structural equation analysis was carried out to explore the predicted connections in the hypothesized model (HM) among students' personal variables from Block 1, the motivational positioning variables (students' intention to learn) from Block 2 and avoidance strategies from Block 3. The fit indices values obtained using the ML Robust method (Satorra– Bentler scaled χ2 = 250.013; p = .000, d.f. = 166; χ2/d.f. = 1.506; NNFI = .77; CFI = .82; IFI = .84; RMSEA = .061) indicated that the model did not completely fit the data. Following the recommendations of Wald and Lagrange's tests for dropping and adding parameters (provided by the EQS program), some adjustments were then introduced and an optimized model (OM) was proposed and tested. We dropped the General Self-esteem variable (Block 1) and some structural effects that were not significant across variables (see the paths that have been deleted in the optimized model presented in Fig. 3 in comparison to the hypothesized model displayed in Fig. 2). The most remarkable modification relates to deleting General Self-esteem. This adjustment has not only been based on statistical information, but has also been supported by the fact that General Academic Self-efficacy and General Self-esteem are general personal variables and are both highly related. Self-esteem is the affective side of Self-concept (the cognitive side). Self-concept and Self-efficacy are overlapping and analogous constructs since they share many similarities (Bong & Skaalvik, 2003). In operationalized terms, the Self-esteem items refer to a subject's overall evaluation of his/her own worth, whereas the General Academic Selfefficacy items refer to a subject's belief in his/her ability to accomplish academic tasks. It seems that students may have found it difficult to discriminate between the level of academic competence that they report (General Academic Self-efficacy) and the perception of his/her own worth when completing the scales. Fig. 3 shows the

structural configuration and standardized coefficients for the optimized model (OM) obtained. The fit indices values obtained using the ML Robust method of estimation (Satorra–Bentler scaled χ 2 = 185.228; p =.0155, d.f. = 146; χ2/d.f. = 1.268; NNFI = .89; CFI = .91; IFI = .91; RMSEA = .044) indicate that the model fitted the data. Regarding the relationship between constructs, the results of the optimized model (OM) can be summarized as follows. As can be seen in Fig. 3, the regression equations revealed that a relevant amount of variability in students' initial motivation (intention to learn) is explained by the personal variables included in the model: 40% of variance of Achievement expectation is explained by General Academic Selfefficacy; 72% of variance of Value of the subject perceived by students at the beginning of the educational process is explained by Interest in the subject, together with Beliefs in the Formative Scope of the Subject; 53% of variance of Enjoyable Learning is explained by Beliefs in the Formative Scope of the Subject and General Academic Self-efficacy; and finally, 11% of the variance of Dedication expectation (in terms of time and effort), students reported to work the subject is explained by General Academic Self-efficacy. The regression equations obtained for avoidance student strategies revealed that, as expected, the motivational positioning variables (intention to learn) have an effect on them, although this effect is not as strong as in the previous case. The avoidance strategies that students used have been partially explained (14% of variance) by Achievement expectations and by Value of the subject, and students' attendance has been partially explained (16% of variance) by Value of the subject, Enjoyable learning expectations, and Prior Knowledge about the subject. 4. Discussion and conclusions The hypothesized connections shown in the Model presented (Fig. 2) have been examined simultaneously to explore the system of relationships. It was expected that, first, personal variables would be good predictors of students' initial motivation or intention to learn

128

F. Doménech-Betoret et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129

(measured through MPV at the beginning of the T/L process); and second, students' initial motivation would be good predictors of Avoidance strategies and Classroom attendance in Educational Psychology. The results are discussed below. An optimized model, presented in Fig. 3, was obtained after introducing some adjusting parameters. The results are discussed below. General Academic Self-efficacy (from Block 1) has a significant and positive influence on Achievement Expectations (from Block 3) which, in turn, has a significant and negative effect on Avoidance Strategies. This means that Avoidance Strategies are influenced directly and negatively by Achievement Expectations and that Achievement Expectations is explained well by General Academic Self-efficacy. Self-efficacy is a component of Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory (1986). Bandura's theory postulated that the feeling of competence determines the quality of task engagement. Bandura (1986) differentiated between “efficacy expectations”, defined as an individual's belief in his/her own capability in accomplishing a given task, and “outcome expectations”, defined as one's belief that the effort that one exerts would lead to a desired outcome. The literature usually encompasses both constructs of selfefficacy under the label “expectation for success” (Liem, Lau, & Nie, 2008). General academic self-efficacy is similar to “efficacy expectation” and, according to Boekaerts (1999), it is considered a general construct and categorized as a superordinate/middle level, whereas Achievement expectations is similar to “outcome expectations” and, according to Boekaerts (1999), it is categorized as a domain-specific construct. The results indicate, firstly, that students' beliefs in a behavior resulting in successful outcomes determine the degree of avoidance strategies used and, secondly, that students' beliefs of competence in accomplishing academic tasks determine their beliefs in outcomes. As a whole, this result suggests that “expectation for success”, formed at the beginning of the teaching/learning process conducted with Educational Psychology, will affect the amount of work students are willing to do to learn this subject. The students who feel they are not capable of doing the work avoid making effort because they are trying to protect self-worth or because they believe they cannot succeed despite such effort (Covington, 1984). Beliefs in the Formative Scope of the subject and Interest in the subject (from Block 1) have a significant, positive influence on Value of the subject matter (from Block 3) which, in turn, has a significant and negative effect on Avoidance Strategies and a significant and positive effect on Classroom attendance. It means that Avoidance Strategies and Classroom attendance are influenced directly by Value of the subject matter and that Value of the subject matter is well explained by Beliefs in the Formative Scope of the subject and, to a lesser extent, by Interest in the subject. The Expectancy-value theory posits that the goals students hold in learning (mastery, performance and work avoidance) are influenced by their self-efficacy and task value (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). Students make their best efforts and spend a significant amount of their time to engage in and master the academic task if they perceive it as important and useful for them (Miller & Brickman, 2004). In line with this, teachers must inculcate among undergraduate students the belief that the subject is important and instrumental since the contents selected in this subject are useful for practicing their profession or for professional performance. In this way, the Value of the subject matter will increase since students' interest and their belief in the formative value of the subject will improve. Beliefs in the Formative Scope of the subject (from Block 1) has a significant, positive influence on students' enjoyable learning expectations (from Block 3) which, in turn, has a significant and positive effect on Classroom attendance. It means that Classroom attendance is influenced directly and positively by Enjoyable learning expectations and that enjoyable learning expectations are well explained by Beliefs in the Formative Scope of the subject. The results indicate that, firstly, students' expectations of enjoyable learning formed on the first days of the T/L process to be conducted with Educational Psychology predict the degree of avoidance strategies used by students to learn; secondly,

students' perception of the formative value of Educational Psychology has a positive effect on their Enjoyable learning expectations. Other than learning, students must also enjoy learning. This occurs when students feel positive emotions during their learning process. For instance, work avoidance has been related to lack of perceived meaning and boredom (Dowson & McInerney, 2001; Seifert & O'Keefe, 2001). In the same vein, Pekrun, Goetz, Daniels, Stupnisky, and Perry (2010) have claimed that “boredom” is positively related to attention problems, and negatively to intrinsic motivation, effort, use of elaboration strategies, selfregulation and subsequent academic performance. According to the Control–Value Theory of Achievement emotions (Pekrun, Frenzel, Goetz & Perry, 2007), control appraisals and value appraisals of achievement activities and outcomes are the proximal determinants of these emotions. Therefore, one way of improving enjoyable expectations will, on the one hand, consist in explaining and emphasizing the formative value that the subject matter has and, on the other hand, make students feel they control their learning. Finally, and contrary to our expectations, the results revealed that Prior knowledge has a direct, positive and significant effect on Classroom attendance, indicating that Classroom attendance also depends, although moderately, on the Prior knowledge students have of the subject. It suggests that the students who do not have the appropriate knowledge or prerequisites to follow the progress made in class tend to avoid Classroom attendance. It also suggests that the evaluation of undergraduate students' Prior knowledge at the beginning of the educative process is recommended to detect any deficiencies that may prevent them from following the progress of the course, which may lead to not attending classes. In summary, and in line with previous studies with the MCSE (Doménech, 2006, 2011b), the Motivational Positioning Variables perceived by students on the first days of the teaching/learning process play an important role in explaining the degree of students' engagement and the amount of work they are willing to do in Educational Psychology. Specifically, according to the MCSE, the answers that students give to some important implicit questions raised on the first days of the educative process should be taken into account to help improve student learning and success: a) Will I be successful in this subject?; b) What value has this subject for me?; c) How will I feel studying this subject matter? This suggests that the messages (implicit or explicit, verbal or non verbal) that the teacher transmits to students on the first days of the educative process, for instance, through their attitudes or through the way they present the course syllabus (assessment, methodology, assignments, etc.) and the subject matter, are very important as they influence students' engagement and the quality of learning right from the beginning.” 4.1. Practical implications Previous research done in the field of MCSE (Doménech, 2006, 2011b) and the results obtained herein seem to indicate that motivational positioning variables (MPV) are capable of predicting students' involvement in their learning process. In this sense, we wish to underline the importance of taking into account such variables on the first days of the educative process. Therefore, it is recommended that teachers evaluate these variables at the beginning of the teaching/learning process since it will provide them with valuable information about the extent to which students will be engaged in studying and working on a specific subject. Furthermore, evaluating certain personal variables (Prior knowledge, Interest in the subject, General Academic Self-Efficacy, Formative scope of the subject) is also recommendable. Thus, if necessary, preventive and corrective instructional measures may be applied in time when motivation deficiencies are detected. Such early intervention can help reduce the avoidance strategies that students adopt throughout the educative process, improving their learning and academic performance. This work provides teachers of the Psychology subject matter with useful and easy tools to apply to help diagnose and evaluate the motivational level of

F. Doménech-Betoret et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129

their students in class at the very beginning of the educative process. It is only with such knowledge that efficient corrective actions can be introduced and addressed, a) to the whole class in order to reinforce and improve the intention to learn or b) with certain students who were identified as being at risk of using Avoidance strategies. 4.2. Limitations and suggestions for future research Although the results obtained in this longitudinal study are satisfactory, some limitations and suggestions for future research should be pointed out. First, the results were obtained from a specific subject matter, so this limits our generalizations to this field. Further research is needed by extending the study to include other subject matters and degrees. Second, future research should also include the contextual variables from Block 2, which were not considered in this study. Third, other initial motivational variables, such as “goal orientation” (Pintrich, 2000) or “learning approaches”, should also be taken into account in the pre-process phase since they could increase the amount of variance explained on Avoidance strategies. References Alexander, P. A., & Judy, J. E. (1988). The interaction of domain-specific and strategic knowledge in academic performance. Review of Educational Research, 58, 375–404. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. (1990). Multidimensional scales of perceived self-efficacy. Stamford, C.A.: Stanford University. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman. Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 680–740). Boston: The McGraw Hill, Companies. Bentler, P.M. (1990a). Comparative fix indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246. Bentler, P.M. (1990b). EQS. Structural equations program manual (2nd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: BMDP Statistical Software. Bentler, P.M. (1995). EQS structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software, Inc. Bentler, P.M. (2006). EQS 6 structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software. Boekaerts, M. (1999). Motivated learning: Studying student situation transactional units. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14(1), 41–55. Bong, M., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2003). Academic Self-Concept and Self-Efficacy: How Different Are They Really? Educational Psychology Review, 15, 1–40. Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 132–162). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Covington, M. (1984). The self-worth theory of achievement motivation: Findings and implications. Elementary School Journal, 85, 5–20. Curran, P. J., West, S. G., & Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 1, 16–29. Dochy, F., Segers, M., & Buehl, M. M. (1999). The relation between assessment practices and outcomes of studies: The case of research on prior knowledge. Review of Educational Research, 69, 145–186. Doménech, F. (2006). Testing an instructional model in a university educational setting from the student's perspective. Learning and Instruction, 16(5), 450–466. Doménech, F. (2011a). Evaluating and investigating in the university educational setting. A new approach from the European Higher Education Area [Evaluar e investigar en la situación educativa universitaria. Un nuevo enfoque desde el Espacio Europeo de Educación Superior]. Publicaciones de la Universitat Jaume I, Universitas, 32. Doménech, F. (2011b). Examining the viability of an instructional model: A preliminary study from the student's perspective [Examinando la viabilidad de un modelo instruccional: Un estudio preliminar desde la perspectiva del estudiante]. Comunicación presentada en el VI Congreso Internacional de Psicología y Educación celebrado en Valladolid los días 29-30-31 de marzo y 1 de abril de 2011. Doménech, F. (2012). Educational psychology: Its application in the classroom context [Psicología de la educativa: Su aplicación al contexto de la clase]. Spain: Publicaciones de la Universitat Jaume I. Col·lecció Psique, 13, Castellón. Doménech, F. (2013). An instructional model for guiding reflection and research in the classroom: The educational situation quality model. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 11(1), 239–260. Dowson, M., & McInerney, D. (2001). Psychological parameters of students' social and work avoidance goals: A qualitative investigation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 35–42. Feather, N. (1982). Expectations and actions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hoe, S. L. (2008). Issues and procedures in adopting structural equation modeling thechnique. Journal of Applied Quantitative Methods, 3, 76–83.

129

Hoyle, R. H. (1995). The structural equation modeling approach: Basic concepts and fundamental issues. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling, concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 1–15). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Coventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. Hu, L., Bentler, P.M., & Kano, Y. (1992). Can test statistics in covariance structure analysis be trusted? Psychological Bulletin, 112, 351–362. Jarvis, S., & Seifert, T. L. (2002). Work avoidance as a manifestation of hostility, helplessness, and boredom. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 48(2), 174–187. Liem, A.D., Lau, S., & Nie, Y. (2008). The role of self-efficacy, task value, and achievement goals in predicting learning strategies, task disengagement, peer relationship, and achievement outcome. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 486–512. Martín-Albo, J., Núñez, J. L., Navarro, J. G., & Grijalvo, F. (2007). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Translation and validation in university students. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10(2), 458–467. Mestre, M. V., García, R., Frías, D., & Llorca, V. (1992). Self-esteem, depression and school variables: A longitudinal study of childhood and adolescence [Autoestima, depresión y variables escolares: Un estudio longitudinal infancia adolescencia]. Revista de Psicología de la Educación, 4, 51–65. Miller, R. B., & Brickman, S. A. (2004). A model of future oriented motivation and self-regulation. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 9–33. Núñez, J. C., González-Pienda, J. A., García, M., González-Pumariega, S., Roces, C., Álvarez, L., et al. (1998). Learning strategies, self-concept and academic achievement [Estrategias de aprendizaje, autoconcepto y rendimiento académico]. Psicothema, 10, 97–109. Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research, 66, 543–578. Pajares, F. (1997). Current directions in self-efficacy research. In M. Maehr, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement, vol. 10. (pp. 1749). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Pastorelli, C., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Rola, J., Rozsa, S., & Bandura, A. (2001). The structure of children's perceived self-efficacy: A cross-national study. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 17, 87–97. Pekrun, R., Frenzel, A., Goetz, T., & Perry, R. P. (2007). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: An integrative approach to emotions in education. In P. A. Schutz, & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotions in education (pp. 13–36). San Diego: Academic Press. Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control–value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 531–549, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019243. Pintrich, P. R. (1989). The dynamic interplay of student motivation and cognition in the college classroom. In C. Ames, & M. L. Maher (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement, vol. 6. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 452–502). San Diego: Academic Press. Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40. Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. (2002). Student interest and achievement: Developmental issues raised by a case study. In A. Wigfi Eld, & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 173–195). New York: Academic Press. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the adolescent self-image (Rev. ed.). Middeltown, CT: Wesleyan University Press. Satorra, A., & Bentler, P.M. (1988). Scaling corrections for chi-square statistics in covariance structure analysis. ASA 1988 Proceedings of the Business and Economic Statistics, Section (pp. 308–313). Alexandria, VA: American Statistical Association. Satorra, A., & Bentler, P.M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In A. von Eye, & C. C. Clogg (Eds.), Latent variables analysis: Applications for developmental research (pp. 399–419). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Schiefele, U., Krapp, A., & Winteler, A. (1992). Interest as a predictor of academic achievement: A meta-analysis of research. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 183–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between 2 and 20 (2nd ed.). New York: Springer-Verlag. Seifert, T. L., & O'Keefe, B.A. (2001). The relationship of work avoidance and learning goals to perceived competence, externality and meaning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 81–92. Sloan, M.A. (2007). Work avoidance in middle school: Teacher' perspectives. (Thesis). Digital Repository at the University of Maryland (http://www.hdl.handle.net/1903/6961). Thompson, R. A., & Zamboanga, B.L. (2004). Academic aptitude and prior knowledge as predictors of student achievement in introduction to psychology. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 778–784. Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., Anderman, E. M., Midgley, C., Gheen, M., & Kang, Y. (2002). The classroom environment and students' reports of avoidance strategies in mathematics: A multimethod study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 88–106. Vasalampi, K., Salmela-Aro, K., & Nurmi, J. -E. (2010). Education-related goal appraisals and self-esteem during the transition to secondary education: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34(6), 481–490. Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. Nueva York: Wiley and Sons. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81, http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ceps. 1999.1015.

MCSE Learning & individual differences.pdf

Learning and Individual Differences 35 (2014) 122–129. Page 3 of 8. MCSE Learning & individual differences.pdf. MCSE Learning & individual differences.pdf.

820KB Sizes 2 Downloads 138 Views

Recommend Documents

pdf-1495\learning-at-work-how-to-support-individual-and ...
... the apps below to open or edit this item. pdf-1495\learning-at-work-how-to-support-individual-a ... -bridgett-oconner-chester-delaney-michael-bronner.pdf.

pdf Language Learning Strategies and Individual ...
... individual learner Language learning strategies individual learner differences in sla between learner characteristics and learning contexts between successful ...

Multi-task, Multi-Kernel Learning for Estimating Individual Wellbeing
Because our application involves classifying multiple highly related outputs (i.e. the five related measures of wellbeing), we can also benefit from Multi-task ...

MCSE server 2012.pdf
Sign in. Loading… Whoops! There was a problem loading more pages. Whoops! There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect ...

Individual Results.pdf
5 250 Richard PURKIS Senior Men M Tewkesbury Triathlon 39:42 5 5. 6 290 Paul MURRIN MV40 M Chepstow Harriers 40:03 6 1 .... 71 219 Lisa JEFFREY FV45 F Chepstow Harriers 48:58 8 2. 72 237 David GEORGE MV60 M Westbury Harriers .... Individual Results.p

(Individual Contributor) Tablet.pdf
Page 1 of 2. Dell Taipei, Chrome Program Manager (Individual Contributor) Tablet. Description of Position. Owns product development and manages activities ...

Individual Results.pdf
Sign in. Loading… Page 1. Whoops! There was a problem loading more pages. Retrying... Individual Results.pdf. Individual Results.pdf. Open. Extract.

individual members
Answering_your_Questions_about_NPCAs_New_Membership_Model.pdf. Answering_your_Questions_about_NPCAs_New_Membership_Model.pdf. Open.

Individual Results.pdf
71 221 Tim BUCKLEY MV50 M Chepstow Harriers 50:03 62 8. 72 203 Stuart DUNCAN MV50 M Chepstow Harriers 50:13 63 9. 73 213 Rod WYATT MV40 M ...

Individual Results.pdf
Jun 13, 2017 - Page 1 of 6. Pos Bib Name Cat Gender Club Time Gender. Pos Cat Pos. 1 366 Oliver WILLIAMS Senior Men M Les Croupiers Rc 20:19 1 1.

Individual Form.pdf
Page 1 of 1. Page 1. Individual Form.pdf. Individual Form.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu. Displaying Individual Form.pdf. Page 1 of 1.

individual perception.pdf
... transfer from the Central. Prison, Ahmedabad to the Central Prison, Jalandhar on the. ground that his family is based in Punjab; his old parents. were suffering from number of ailments; and further the. 1. (2001) 3 SCC 221. WWW.LIVELAW.IN. Page 3

MCSA/MCSE Windows XP Professional Study Guide ...
Partitioning of Disk Space. Disk partitioning is the act of taking the physical hard drive and creating logical partitions. A logical drive is how space is allocated to the drive's primary and logical partitions. For example, if you have a 5GB hard d

Individual Retirement Account Balances, Contributions
39.2 percent traditional–originating from contributions (TOFC). • 17.7 percent traditional–originating from rollovers (TOFR) (combined traditional IRA category ...