© 2007 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience

NEWS AND VIEWS

A real red-letter day Edward M Hubbard Synesthesia, in which letters or numbers elicit color perception, could be due to increased brain connectivity between relevant regions, or due to failure to inhibit feedback in cortical circuits. Diffusion tensor imaging now provides evidence for increased connectivity in word processing and binding regions of the brain.

If looking at this page of text causes you to see a cascade of colors, you have graphemecolor synesthesia, in which viewing letters and numbers in black and white elicits the experience of seeing colors1,2. For a graphemecolor synesthete, the letter ‘A’ might always be tinged red, a ‘5’ might have a blue overlay, or the word ‘synesthesia’ might be associated with yellow and green because of the colors of the individual letters. Grapheme-color synesthesia occurs in as many as 2 out of every 100 people3 and is the most intensively studied form of synesthesia. Although behavioral4,5 and neuroimaging studies6,7 have shown consistent differences between synesthetes and nonsynesthetes, the underlying neural basis for these differences has been a matter of substantial debate. Some researchers propose that the additional experiences of synesthetes are due to increased connectivity between relevant brain regions, such as those involved in word and color perception, perhaps because of incomplete pruning1,6. Others argue that synesthesia does not depend on anatomical differences, but is instead due to a failure of inhibition in cortical feedback circuits8. To date, these models have been supported by indirect evidence, as neither behavioral nor neuroimaging data can distinguish between these two neural mechanisms. In this issue, Rouw and Scholte9 provide direct evidence of increased structural connectivity in synesthetes, supporting the first hypothesis that synesthesia is a result

The author is at Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale Unité 562, Neuroimagerie Cognitive, CEA/SAC/DSV/DRM/ NEUROSPIN, Bât. 145, Point courrier 156, 91191 Gif-Sur-Yvette, France. e-mail: [email protected]

Figure 1 The outer cortical surface with relevant brain regions indicated. The color-selective hV4 is indicated in red, and the visual word form area is indicated in green. Cross-activation between these regions, mediated by increased anatomical connectivity, correlates with the generation of the additional experiences of grapheme-color synesthesia, and the degree of connectivity determines their strength. The posterior IPS, thought to be involved in binding, is in blue. Additional anatomical connectivity in this region may be critical for synesthetic binding, which must operate on the colors generated by the cross-activation between grapheme regions and hV4. These regions have been projected to the left hemisphere for simplicity.

of increased connectivity between relevant brain regions. The authors combined two neuroimaging techniques to provide insights into the neural basis of this condition. First, the authors used diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), a neuroimaging technique that measures the diffusion of water molecules in the living human brain. Water molecules diffuse more easily parallel than perpendicular to the direction of whitematter fibers, because of the myelin sheaths

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 10 | NUMBER 6 | JUNE 2007

and axonal membranes. By measuring relative differences in how easily water diffuses along different axes (termed fractional anisotropy), it is possible to infer the size, orientation and degree of myelination of white matter tracts in vivo. Rouw and Scholte9 used this technique to demonstrate increased structural connectivity in synesthetes compared with controls in three brain regions: the right fusiform gyrus, which is near regions involved in word and color processing,

671

© 2007 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience

NEWS AND VIEWS and the left intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and frontal cortex, both of which are part of a network of regions involved in binding and consciousness (Fig. 1). Although differences in structural connectivity may coexist with differences in inhibitory processes, and may even be the consequence of such differences10, the present study clearly shows increased connectivity in regions thought to be critical to the genesis of grapheme-color synesthesia. This is consistent with previous proposals supported by indirect evidence1,6. In addition to the group differences between synesthetes and nonsynesthetes, behavioral6,11 and neuroimaging studies6 have shown stable individual differences among synesthetes. Some synesthetes (‘projectors’) report strong experiences projected into the external world, whereas others (‘associators’) report weaker experiences that appear in their mind’s eye11. Rouw and Scholte9 assessed the intensity of synesthetic experiences using a structured questionnaire, in which subjects rated their experiences on a five-point scale. They found that the degree of fractional anisotropy in the right temporal cortex was positively correlated with the reported intensity of synesthetic experience, suggesting that such differences in intensity are due to differences in connectivity in the temporal cortex. In addition, by showing that phenomenological differences correlate with anatomical differences among different synesthetes, these findings constitute an essential replication of reports of stable individual differences among synesthetes6,11 using a different group of subjects and a different imaging modality. These individual differences may be important for understanding some of the contradictory findings in the literature1 and must be taken into consideration in future investigations of synesthesia. Rouw and Scholte9 also identified regions of increased fractional anisotropy in the IPS and frontal cortex, which is consistent with neuroimaging12 and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)13 data showing that the parietal cortex is essential for synesthetic binding of color and form. Notably, the degree of fractional anisotropy in parietal and frontal regions did not correlate with the subjective reports of their synesthetes, suggesting that differences in the parietal cortex may be important for determining whether or not someone is a synesthete, but not for determining the strength of their experiences. Taken together, these results suggest a two-stage model of graphemecolor synesthesia (Fig. 1). In the first stage, anomalous color experiences are generated via cross-activation in ventral

672

visual areas. Every time a synesthete looks at a letter or number, additional excitatory activity passes from the regions involved in grapheme processing to those involved in color processing, with the degree of connectivity determining the strength of those experiences. After synesthetic colors are generated via this cross-activation, they are then bound by stronger than normal parieto-frontal binding mechanisms, which may elicit a kind of ‘hyperbinding’12,13. In the same session, Rouw and Scholte9 tested the same subjects using standard whole-brain functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Consistent with previous reports6,7, the authors found increased activation in the ventral-occipital cortex, in the human V4 complex (hV4). Unlike previous investigators6, they did not find a correlation between fMRI activation and their subjective report measure, despite their larger sample size. However, as the authors note, this lack of a correlation may be due to anatomical variability in the location of hV4 and the exact location of activated cortex, which cannot be detected without using retinotopic mapping in individual subjects. Additional studies combining retinotopic mapping and diffusion tensor tractography may clarify these issues. Because they collected DTI and fMRI data in the same subjects, Rouw and Scholte9 were able to compare the locations of the anatomical and functional differences. Although both were in the right temporal cortex, the anatomical differences were anterior to the location of increased blood oxygenation level–dependent signal, suggesting that the interplay between anatomical and functional differences is more complex than is suggested by the simple direct cross-activation hypothesis1. Some of this unexpected complexity might be due to the presence of multiple stages involved in reading. In the past five years, models of the neural basis of reading have become more sophisticated, moving from the notion of a single visual word form area14 to suggesting a hierarchy of stages beginning in early visual areas and increasing in complexity, invariance and receptive field size across the entire ventral visual pathway15. A better understanding of the mechanisms of reading will be critical for interpreting these neuroanatomical and functional differences. The new anatomical data also have relevance to the question of the laterality differences, if any, in synesthesia. Previous fMRI studies (for example, refs. 6,7) showed either left-lateralized or bilateral activation in hV4. Contrary to this, Rouw and Scholte9 find increased fractional anisotropy and increased fMRI blood oxygenation level–

dependent signals in the right temporal cortex. Similarly discrepant lateralization is found in fMRI and TMS studies examining the role of parietal cortices in synesthetic binding. Increased activity in the left, but not right, IPS is seen by fMRI during synesthetic binding12, whereas synesthetic binding is disrupted only after TMS stimulation of the right IPS13. Consistent with the fMRI results, but not the TMS results, Rouw and Scholte9 found significant anatomical differences between synesthetes and nonsynesthetes only in the left hemisphere. Given the small number of subjects commonly tested and the differing lateralizations obtained using different techniques, assertions of the laterality in synesthesia should be taken with caution until larger studies are conducted to examine these questions. In sum, this study demonstrates anatomical differences between synesthetes and nonsynesthetes. Not only do these results provide clear support for the hypothesis that anatomical differences underlie at least some aspects of synesthetic experience, they also suggest that pre-existing neuroanatomical differences may underlie differences in conscious experience more generally. Future investigations into the neural correlates of unusual sensory experiences, including other forms of synesthesia, Charles Bonnet syndrome and even schizophrenic hallucinations, should use methods such as these to investigate whether they depend on similar functional and anatomical differences in the relevant brain regions. COMPETING INTERESTS STATEMENT The author declares no competing financial interests. 1. Hubbard, E.M. & Ramachandran, V.S. Neuron 48, 509–520 (2005). 2. Rich, A.N. & Mattingley, J.B. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 43–52 (2002). 3. Simner, J. et al. Perception 35, 1024–1033 (2006). 4. Mattingley, J.B., Rich, A.N., Yelland, G. & Bradshaw, J.L. Nature 410, 580–582 (2001). 5. Dixon, M.J., Smilek, D., Cudahy, C. & Merikle, P.M. Nature 406, 365 (2000). 6. Hubbard, E.M., Arman, A.C., Ramachandran, V.S. & Boynton, G.M. Neuron 45, 975–985 (2005). 7. Nunn, J.A. et al. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 371–375 (2002). 8. Grossenbacher, P.G. & Lovelace, C.T. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 36–41 (2001). 9. Rouw, R. & Scholte, H.S. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 792–797 (2007). 10. Hensch, T.K. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 877–888 (2005). 11. Dixon, M.J., Smilek, D. & Merikle, P.M. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 335–343 (2004). 12. Weiss, P.H., Zilles, K. & Fink, G.R. Neuroimage 28, 859–868 (2005). 13. Esterman, M., Verstynen, T., Ivry, R.B. & Robertson, L.C. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1570–1576 (2006). 14. Cohen, L. et al. Brain 123, 291–307 (2000). 15. Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M. & Vinckier, F. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 335–341 (2005).

VOLUME 10 | NUMBER 6 | JUNE 2007 NATURE NEUROSCIENCE

NN0607 NV.indd - Nature

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 10 | NUMBER 6 | JUNE 2007 ... letters or numbers elicit color perception, could be due to increased brain connectivity.

146KB Sizes 1 Downloads 163 Views

Recommend Documents

Eck - Nature
Massachusetts 02215, USA. 3Biosciences Division, Structural Biology Center,. Argonne National Laboratory, Argonne, Illinois 60439, USA. 4Department of.

PersPeCTives - Nature
whole organism6–10. When alternative chromatin ..... A full analysis of this example is given in FIG. 4. The biological model shows hypothesized connections between phenotypic variation and its heritable basis. Blue circles indicate ... locus). see

History of Practice Nature of Western Influence Nature ...
engraining it within Indian culture, and thus support the existence of the transsexual community. ... Chicago: The. University of Chicago Press. Williams, Walter L.

The fractal nature of nature: power laws, ecological complexity and ...
May 1, 2002 - 1999a,b) have developed models that explain these .... ency to be satisfi ed with the 'model' or equation that gives ..... M. Diamond), pp. 81–120 ...

Nature Club Bringing Nature Closer July 2014.pdf
Bird feeder. Bird bath/shower. Bird home. Ladybird sanctuary. Creepy crawly homes. Creepy crawlies you might find. Hedgehog home. Bee home. Moth catcher.

Nature Biotech.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. Nature Biotech.

Radishes gone wild - Nature
Sep 27, 2006 - radish R. sativus) were introduced into. California from Europe and became naturalized. By the early 20th century, a few apparent hybrids were ...

newS anD viewS - Nature
Jul 7, 2008 - possibly breast cancers4,5. However, in most advanced tumors, the response to antian- giogenic therapy, even in combination with conventional chemotherapy6, is not long lasting, and tumor cells bypass targeted sig- naling pathways and r

Nature News
Oct 20, 2008 - Co-author Santo Fortunato of the Institute for Scientific Interchange in Turin, Italy, says that they ... Filing system: cross-discipline research.

Nature Boy.pdf
fools and kings. 34. ̇. ̇. ̇ œ. œ ̇n. œ. œ œ. j. œ. This he said to. œ. œ. œ. ̇. ̇. ̇. œ. œ. ̇. ̇. ̇. me . . ̇. ̇œ œ. œ. œ œ. œ œ ̇. ̇. ̇. ‰ j. œ. The. œ. œ. œ. ̇. ̇ ̇. ̇. ̇. ̇. œ œ. ̇. great est thing.

Radishes gone wild - Nature
Sep 27, 2006 - Arecent study of wild and cultivated radishes in California provides some of the best evidence to date that natural hybridization is a legitimate.

Duffy final.indd - Nature
Source at Oak Ridge, Tennessee, and laser facilities such as the National. Ignition Facility in Livermore, California. It is worth emphasizing that static techniques are only one method of achieving ultra-high pres- sure–temperature conditions. His

Gruber final.indd - Nature
affect the capacity of Earth's biosphere to continue absorbing carbon from the atmosphere (see page 289), ..... effort that includes the entire set of tools and approaches available to researchers who work in the fields of ... Can management of the g

A social call - Nature
Jul 22, 2004 - activation during singing in zebra finches found that the areas of the brain responsible for song production and for perception are differentially ...

The fractal nature of nature: power laws, ecological ...
May 1, 2002 - Underlying the diversity of life and the complexity of ecology is order that refl ects the operation of funda- mental physical and biological ... laws of biological inheritance, evolution by natural selection, and many others. ... cesse

Read Adventures in Nature: Honduras (Adventures in Nature (John ...
Download Adventures in Nature: Honduras (Adventures in Nature (John Muir)), ... At the same time, it also explains both sides of the country s environmental ...

28.3 nNv mx - Nature
ences) to link the warning signal to unpalata- bility. This is the explanation for the ... Mike Speed is in the Department of Biology,. Liverpool Hope, Hope Park, ...

Remember that photon - Nature
Dec 8, 2005 - To put the problem in more quantum-mechanical terms, a classical repeater breaks down quantum entanglement. This delicate phenomenon is ...

Radishes gone wild - Nature
Sep 27, 2006 - Arecent study of wild and cultivated radishes in California provides some of the best evidence to date that natural hybridization is a legitimate conservation concern. Whereas plant and animal species typically harbor suites of traits

Receptor-targeted siRNAs - Nature
Jun 6, 2005 - directed particle aggregation6,7. In addition, changes in the LSPR of gold and silver nano- structures have recently been used to observe the binding of biomolecules to their surfaces8,9. The sensitivity of the LSPR is such that small f

Remember that photon - Nature
Dec 8, 2005 - Over the past twenty years I have ... dinner at University College,. November 17. FromNature 7 ... behaviour. The degree of photon splitting can.

NpgRJ_Nmeth_1236 735..740 - Nature
Jul 20, 2008 - A central challenge in neuroscience is to understand the formation and function of three-dimensional (3D) neuronal networks. In vitro studies ...

mixed up - Nature
NATURE STRUCTURAL & MOLECULAR BIOLOGY VOLUME 14 NUMBER 1 JANUARY 2007. 13. Earnshaw ... Acad. Sci. USA, published online 13 December 2006, doi:10.1073/ ... observed, the amplitude of which corresponds to the degree.

Nature Conservation
database since 1981, based on volunteer and expert ob- servations from all around ... the exact superimposition of the map grid lines. (Nathan and Shmida ...