c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex

Research report

Perirhinal cortex tracks degree of recent as well as cumulative lifetime experience with object concepts Devin Duke a, Chris B. Martin a,c, Ben Bowles a, Ken McRae a and €hler a,b,* Stefan Ko a

Brain and Mind Institute and Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre, Toronto, Canada c Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada b

article info

abstract

Article history:

Evidence from numerous sources indicates that recognition of the prior occurrence of

Received 12 February 2016

objects requires computations of perirhinal cortex (PrC) in the medial temporal lobe (MTL).

Reviewed 6 May 2016

Extant research has primarily probed recognition memory based on item exposure in a

Revised 23 July 2016

recent experimental study episode. Outside the laboratory, however, familiarity for objects

Accepted 19 January 2017

typically accrues gradually with learning across many different episodic contexts, which can

Action editor Emrah Du¨zel Published online 31 January 2017

be distributed over a lifetime of experience. It is currently unknown whether PrC also tracks this cumulative lifetime experience with object concepts. To address this issue, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment in healthy individuals in

Keywords:

which we compared judgments of the perceived lifetime familiarity with object concepts, a

Recognition memory

task that has previously been employed in many normative studies on concept knowledge,

Autobiographical memory

with frequency judgments for recent laboratory exposure in a study phase. Guided by

Semantic memory

neurophysiological data showing that neurons in primate PrC signal prior object exposure at

Hippocampus

multiple time scales, we predicted that PrC responses would track perceived prior experience

fMRI

in both types of judgments. Left PrC and a number of cortical regions that are often coactivated as part of the default-mode network showed an increase in Blood-Oxygen-Level Dependent (BOLD) response in relation to increases in the perceived cumulative lifetime familiarity of object concepts. These regions included the left hippocampus, left mid-lateral temporal cortex, as well as anterior and posterior cortical midline structures. Critically, left PrC was found to be the only region that showed this response in combination with the typically observed decrease in signal for perceived recent exposure in the experimental study phase. These findings provide, to our knowledge, the first evidence that ties signals in human PrC to variations in cumulative lifetime experience with object concepts. They offer a new link between the role of PrC in recognition memory and its broader role in conceptual processing. © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

* Corresponding author. Brain and Mind Institute, Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada. € hler). E-mail address: [email protected] (S. Ko http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.015 0010-9452/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

62

1.

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

Introduction

The ability to recognize prior occurrences of objects in the environment is critical to many aspects of adaptive behavior. There is a consensus in the neuroscience literature across rodents, non-human primates, and humans that it requires the integrity of perirhinal cortex (PrC) in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) (Brown & Aggleton, 2001; Eichenbaum, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, 2007; Suzuki & Naya, 2014; for exceptions, see € hler, 2013). A noticeable but less Martin, McLean, O'Neil, & Ko consistent body of research also suggests that the role of PrC in recognition is specific to item-based familiarity assessment (Brown & Aggleton, 2001; Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Montaldi & Mayes, 2010; Ranganath & Ritchey, 2012; but see Wixted & Squire, 2011). Recognition of prior occurrence is typically probed in humans with recognition-memory tasks that include an initial experimentally controlled study phase for a list of items. Responses are classified as familiarity-based when an item is endorsed as studied, with no evidence of successful recovery of contextual detail about that study encounter. Critically, when meaningful stimuli such as object concepts (i.e., the concrete object that a word or picture refers to; Martin, 2015) are employed, as is the case in the majority of published studies (Kim, 2013), participants make their memory judgment with reference to the recent study episode rather than with respect to their lifetime of experience, which may have involved hundreds or thousands of item encounters in many different episodic contexts. As such, extant research on the role of human PrC in recognition memory for meaningful stimuli has typically examined recent incremental changes rather than absolute levels of familiarity (Mandler, 1980; for further discussion see Bridger, Bader, & Mecklinger, 2014; Mandler, 2008). Behavioral findings suggest that humans can also judge their cumulative past experience with object concepts accrued outside the laboratory. For example, people can easily judge whether they are more familiar with pliers or scalpels. Indeed, in normative databases on conceptual knowledge (Cree & McRae, 2003; Moreno-Martinez, Montoro & Rodriguez€ der, Gemballa, Ruppin, & Wartenburger, Rojo, 2014; Schro 2012), participants are often asked to rate their cumulative familiarity with object concepts over their lifetime (sometimes simply referred to as ‘concept familiarity’). Such judgments display considerable consistency across participants, and are also known to have some external validity, as reflected in significant correlations with objective word frequency (Cree & McRae, 2003; Moreno-Martinez, Montoro & € der et al., 2012). The neural and Rodriguez-Rojo, 2014; Schro cognitive mechanisms that support this ability, however, are poorly understood at present. In the present study, we took a first step in addressing this issue with a functional neuroimaging approach. Specifically, we asked whether the medialtemporal lobe structure that has most consistently been implicated in recognition memory in prior research, i.e., PrC, tracks not only recent but also cumulative lifetime experience with object concepts. Two sources of evidence hint at a potential role of PrC in signaling cumulative levels of prior object exposure over the long-term. First, neurophysiological recordings in non-human

primates have shown that some PrC neurons track the degree of exposure to objects that accumulates through hundreds of € lscher, Rolls, & presentations over the course of weeks (Ho Xiang, 2003; Rolls, Franco, & Stringer, 2005). This cumulative increase in responding contrasts with the decrease that is typically observed in relation to repetition within experimental sessions, a phenomenon that has been coined repetition suppression (Fahy, Riches, & Brown, 1993; Li, Miller, & Desimone, 1993; Miller, Li, & Desimone, 1991; Xiang & Brown, 1998). A second source of evidence comes from a neuropsychological case study from our laboratory, which recently showed that an individual (NB) with a rare left anterior temporal lobe lesion that includes PrC but spares the hippocampus exhibits noticeable abnormalities in judging cumulative lifetime familiarity for object concepts (Bowles, € hler, 2016). This individual Duke, Rosenbaum, McRae, & Ko was also impaired in making frequency judgments for graded exposure to concept names in a recent experimental study phase (Bowles et al., 2016), and in making familiarity-based memory judgments in other experimental paradigms that required reference to such a study phase (Bowles et al., 2007; € hler, 2011). Our findings in Martin, Bowles, Mirsattari, & Ko NB suggest that left anterior temporal structures play a necessary role in assessing the cumulative lifetime familiarity of object concepts. However, NB's lesion, which resulted from surgical resection, was not restricted to PrC but included anterior-lateral and temporo-polar cortex as well. Thus, these neuropsychological findings do not specifically point to PrC. Given that neighboring anterior temporal structures in the left hemisphere have also been implicated in conceptual processing (e.g., Jackson, Hoffman, Pobric & Lambon-Ralph, 2016; Skipper, Ross, & Olson, 2011; Wright, Randall, Clarke, & Tyler, 2015), it is important to seek evidence that directly links variations in lifetime experience with objects concepts to PrC. In the present study, we employed functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in healthy individuals to examine whether PrC tracks participants' responses when they judge their cumulative lifetime familiarity with object concepts. For the purpose of comparison, we also included frequency judgments that required assessment of the degree of recent item exposure in an experimental study phase. We provide evidence that left PrC and several other cortical structures respond to perceived higher lifetime familiarity for object concepts with an increase in Blood-Oxygen-Level Dependent (BOLD) signal. We also report that PrC is the only structure that shows this pattern in combination with a signal change in opposite direction for perceived recent item exposure.

2.

Materials and methods

2.1.

Participants

Twenty healthy young adults (mean age ¼ 25.6, SD ¼ 3.9, 10 females) participated in the experiment. Participants were pre-screened to rule out the presence of neurological disorders. This research was approved by the Health Sciences Research Ethics Board at Western University.

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

2.2.

Materials

Two hundred verbal labels for basic-level object concepts were drawn from the normative database on object concepts published by Cree and McRae (2003) (see also McRae, Cree, Seidenberg, & McNorgan, 2005). Two sets of 100 words were used, and were matched on word frequency, word length, number of syllables, and the range and mean of normative concept familiarity ratings (i.e., normative ratings of lifetime concept experience; Cree & McRae, 2003). Assignment of sets to tasks was counterbalanced across participants. Creation of subsets of items with different familiarity values was based on normative ratings; we selected five subsets of 20 items with progressively increasing levels of normative lifetime familiarity, with items in each set matched on word length and number of syllables. For frequency judgments of recent exposure, five subsets of 20 object concepts, matched for mean normative lifetime familiarity, were assigned to one of the five repetition conditions during the initial study phase. For the lexicaldecision task at study, forty pronounceable non-words were also employed as fillers. Presentation order at study was constrained such that any stimulus repetition was separated by at least three trials.

2.3.

Procedure

During an initial study phase, participants were exposed to the set of 100 object concepts later to be probed in the frequency-judgment task, in combination with the non-word filler items (see Fig. 1). Each item was presented for 1000 msec with a 500 msec inter-stimulus interval (ISI). Participants made lexical-decision judgments and were asked to respond as quickly as possible. They were also informed that some stimuli would be repeated, but no reference was made to any subsequent memory test. The five subsets of object concepts were presented 1, 2, 4, 7, or 12 times, respectively. The subsequent test phase provided the fMRI data of interest and included five runs with 40 experimental trials per run. Here, participants made frequency judgments for the object concepts presented in the recent study phase or judgments of cumulative lifetime experience for the other set of 100 object concepts (not presented at study). For the latter task, participants were asked to rate their lifetime familiarity with the type of ‘thing’ (i.e., object concept) the word denotes (see Supplementary Materials for detailed instructions). For the frequency task, participants judged relative repetition frequency from the initial study phase for each concept. Both types of judgments required use of a five-point scale, with one corresponding to the lowest value (Fig. 1). Items were presented in five-trial blocks of each task for 2500 msec (per item), with a jittered ISI that ranged from 2500 msec to 10,000 msec, and the order of trials and jitter optimized within each fMRI run, using the OptSeq2 algorithm (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/ optseq/). During all ISIs, participants were instructed to rest but stay ready for the next trial.

2.4.

Functional data acquisition and analysis

fMRI data were obtained on a 3T Siemens TIM Trio scanner using a T2*-weighted single-shot gradient echo-planar

63

acquisition sequence (repetition time (TR) 2500 msec; echo time (TE) 25 msec; slice thickness 3 mm; in-plane resolution 3  3 mm; field of view (FOV) 192  192 mm; flip angle, 70 . To optimize Magnetic Resonance (MR) signal in the most inferior and anterior aspects of the temporal lobe, we used an oblique transverse slice orientation. Each of the 171 functional volumes included 42 contiguous slices collected in an interleaved manner that covered the entire brain volume except for the most superior aspects (<10 mm) of frontal and parietal cortex. T1-weighted anatomical images were obtained using an Magnetization-Prepared Rapid Acquisition with Gradient Echo (MPRAGE) sequence (192 slices; TR 2300 msec; TE 4.25 msec; flip angle 9 ; FOV 256  256 mm; 1 mm isotropic voxels). Data were preprocessed using BrainVoyager QX version 2.6 (Brain Innovation). Functional images were slice-scan time corrected, motion corrected, and high-pass filtered. Functional images were subsequently co-registered with the anatomical images in Talairach space, and smoothed using a three-dimensional Gaussian kernel with a full-width at half-maximum of 8 mm. A canonical hemodynamic response function was used for analyzing event-related responses (Friston et al., 1998). The BOLD response to each event type was modeled by convolving a series of delta functions corresponding to the onset of each event with canonical hemodynamic response function. For each participant, all response outcomes (lifetime 1e5 and frequency 1e5) were modeled as separate conditions. A wholevolume voxel-wise approach was used to identify linear trends in BOLD signal that tracked participants' ratings in each task. These trends were probed in a conjunction analysis (Nichols, Brett, Andersson, Wager, & Poline, 2005). Statistical significance was established using control for false discovery rate (FDR) (p < .05).

3.

Results

3.1.

Behavioral results

The lifetime familiarity ratings of different participants correlated highly with each other (mean r ¼ .53; SD ¼ .06 across 100 items). We also observed a significant positive relationship between participants' ratings and the five levels (i.e., bins) of normative concept familiarity (Cree & McRae, 2003) that were used to generate our lists (mean r ¼ .66, SD ¼ .07, p < .001; see Fig. 2a). For the frequency task probing recent exposure, ratings of relative frequency of experimental exposure showed a significant positive relationship with the five levels of repetition frequency used in the study phase (mean r ¼ .40, SD ¼ .13, p < .001; see Fig. 2a), revealing sensitivity to our frequency manipulation at study, and by extension, some validity of the judgments provided.

3.2.

Neuroimaging results

In the fMRI literature on recognition-memory, a decrease in PrC signal is the most commonly reported response that is associated with judged recent exposure to meaningful verbal stimuli (e.g., Daselaar, Fleck, & Cabeza, 2006; Henson, Cansino, Herron, Robb, & Rugg, 2003; Kim, 2013; but see Martin, Cowell, € hler, 2016; for findings with other Gribble, Wright, & Ko

64

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

Fig. 1 e Procedure and trial structure. Data from the test phase provided fMRI data of interest.

Fig. 2 e Behavioral and fMRI results (a) Cumulative lifetime familiarity ratings and frequency ratings of recent exposure as a function of normative estimates of lifetime experience and of recent exposure during the study phase, respectively. See Methods for number of repetitions that correspond to the five frequency bins. Behavioral ratings on both tasks were correlated with experimental manipulations. (b) Left PrC region identified in the conjunction analysis. (c) Beta plot for this left PrC region (extracted at FDR p < .05). Error bars reflect Standard Error of the Mean (SEM). Left PrC tracks behavioral ratings with a BOLD response that follows opposite directions in both tasks.

stimulus types). Accordingly, we predicted that the judged frequency of recent laboratory exposures would be negatively correlated with BOLD signals in PrC. For cumulative lifetime experience, directional predictions are more difficult to make based on published data. However, neurophysiological recordings in non-human primates have revealed PrC neurons that exhibit a gradual increase in responding over the course of weeks of testing, with hundreds of stimulus repetitions, € lscher when animals perform recognition memory tasks (Ho et al., 2003; Rolls et al., 2005). While the mapping of repetition effects in single-cell recordings onto fMRI BOLD signals remains incompletely understood (see Grill-Spector, 2006), we note that evidence from local field potentials, i.e., physiological signals that have been closely linked to fMRI BOLD responses (Logothetis, Pauls, Augath, Trinath, & Oeltermann, 2001), also suggests that long-term familiarity of objects, accumulated over many sessions, is reflected in a response increase in monkey inferotemporal cortex (Anderson, Mruczek, Kawasaki, & Sheinberg, 2008). In line with this notion, prior fMRI research has revealed increases in PrC

activity during processing of familiar meaningful as compared to unfamiliar abstract objects (Barense, Henson, & Graham, 2011). Accordingly, we predicted that the perceived cumulative lifetime experience with object concepts would be positively correlated with BOLD signals in PrC, effectively leading to opposite directions of change in signal for both types of memory judgments. A whole-volume voxel-wise analysis that tested for a significant conjunction of these two linear trends (i.e., with logical operator ‘and’; Nichols et al., 2005) offered a powerful way to examine our predictions in a single test across all experimental conditions (i.e., all response options across both tasks). The only brain region that showed the typical decrease in signal for perceived recent exposure in combination with an increase in response for increases in lifetime experience was a portion of left PrC in the anterior collateral sulcus. Activity in this region survived statistical control for FDR (p < .05) and remained the only region showing a significant effect even when a lenient criterion of p < .05 (uncorrected) was applied for the conjunction (see Fig. 2b, c). Fig. 3 illustrates the close

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

65

Fig. 3 e 3D anatomical reconstruction of (a) patient NB's medial temporal lobe structures after surgical resection, and (b) those of a representative participant in the current study. The left PrC region that tracked behavioral ratings in both tasks of the present fMRI experiment (also shown in Fig. 2b) is overlaid on the 3D reconstruction, revealing a close correspondence between this activation focus and the anterior part of left PrC that was resected in patient NB. For details of 3D reconstruction see Bowles et al. (2007).

correspondence between this activation focus and the anterior portion of left PrC that was resected in patient NB. As previously noted, this individual showed abnormalities in making judgments in both tasks in our prior neuropsychological research (Bowles et al., 2016). Notably, when the linear contrasts for each task were examined in isolation, there were no additional clusters in PrC, nor within neighboring antero-lateral temporal or temporopolar cortex (see Binney, Embleton, Jefferies, Parker, & Ralph, 2010; Skipper et al., 2011; Wright et al., 2015, for potential role of these structures in conceptual processing) that showed a linear trend in either direction. The linear task contrast for judgments of lifetime familiarity did, however, reveal regions outside the anterior temporal lobe that showed an increase in signal with perceived increases in lifetime experience with object concepts. These regions included several structures that have previously been found to be co-activated as part of the default mode network and in autobiographical memory tasks (Andrews-Hanna, Smallwood, & Spreng, 2014), including the hippocampus, mid-lateral temporal cortex, as well as medial prefrontal and medial parietal cortex (see Fig. 4).

4.

Discussion

The present fMRI experiment revealed that PrC tracks both the perceived frequency of recent laboratory exposure as well as the perceived cumulative lifetime familiarity with object concepts. We found that activity in several cortical regions increased with perceived cumulative lifetime experience. However, left PrC was the only structure that showed this signal change in combination with a decrease in responding for perceived recent laboratory exposure. While a critical role of human PrC in signaling recent occurrence is well established in the neuroscience literature, the current study

provides, to our knowledge, the first evidence that also links signals in this structure to variations in cumulative lifetime familiarity with object concepts. Past fMRI research on the role of PrC in recognition memory has typically built on paradigms that require memory decisions with respect to a recent item encounter in an experimentally controlled study phase, which took place minutes, hours, or days prior to memory testing. Findings from this research have linked item-based recognition to differential PrC responses for previously studied as compared to novel items (see Diana, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, 2007; Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Kim, 2013 for reviews). Several studies have reported a negative relationship between the PrC BOLD response and degree of confidence in the perceived ‘oldness’ of test items, a behavioral marker that is often assumed to reflect recent changes in item-based familiarity (see Daselaar et al., 2006; Gonsalves, Kahn, Curran, Norman, & Wagner, 2005; Montaldi, Spencer, Roberts, & Mayes, 2006; Wang, Ranganath, & Yonelinas, 2014; Yonelinas, 2002). In the present study, we observed a strikingly similar negative relationship between the BOLD response in left PrC and the perceived frequency of recent laboratory exposure. Like recognition-memory decisions, frequency judgments have been proposed to depend on both assessments of item-based familiarity as well as recollection (Hintzman & Curran, 1994; Hintzman, 2004). Evidence from computational modeling and behavioral research on retrieval dynamics suggests, however, that under many experimental conditions, familiarity is the primary basis for accurate frequency judgments (Hintzman & Curran, 1994; see also Dobbins, Simons, & Schacter, 2004). In the present experiment, the exposure to items at study was manipulated in small increments over a substantial range of presentations and, at test, we included no items without any prior study exposure

66

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

Fig. 4 e Additional regions co-activated with left PrC (circled in yellow) that show linear increases in BOLD response related to increases in perceived lifetime familiarity with object concepts (linear contrast thresholded at FDR p < .05).

(i.e., no novel lures). With this arrangement, we minimized the likelihood that any reliable source of contextual information would allow for differentiation of the frequency of recent exposures, maximizing the need to rely on graded signals that code for recent incremental changes in familiarity. Evidence in support of the success of our manipulation comes from patient-based research with this paradigm. We have previously reported that a focal anterior temporal-lobe lesion that includes left PrC, but spares the hippocampus, produced impairments in making frequency judgments on this task in an individual with documented preserved recollection abilities (Bowles et al., 2016). Thus, although the present results do not establish that the graded PrC response we observed specifically reflects an item-based memory signal, rather than recollection, such a conclusion is supported by prior behavioral work on frequency judgments, as well as by prior lesion-based research. Moreover, it is consistent with prominent accounts of graded PrC activity that has been observed in relations to confidence-based recognition-memory judgments in prior fMRI research. Critically, our study identified a region in left PrC that did not only track perceived frequency of recent laboratory exposure but also perceived cumulative lifetime experience with object concepts. This finding dovetails with evidence from neurophysiological recordings in non-human primates, which has shown that some PrC neurons track prior exposure of objects over extended time periods. In one study, a gradual increase in firing rates was observed as visual stimuli became increasingly more familiar over the course of weeks of testing € lscher et al., 2003). with hundreds of stimulus repetitions (Ho Xiang and Brown (1998) noted that PrC neurons differ in their sensitivity to effects of recent versus long term exposure when monkeys perform recognition memory tasks. They reported that some PrC neurons (termed ‘recency neurons’) respond to repetition of a recent (i.e., within-session) stimulus encounter; this response was observed regardless of whether the stimulus in question had previously been seen in many

other sessions distributed over multiple days (see also Fahy et al., 1993; Li et al., 1993; Miller et al., 1991; but see; Thome, Erickson, Lipa, & Barnes, 2012). Other PrC neurons identified by Xiang and Brown (1998) responded to whether stimuli had been encountered frequently on prior days, regardless of whether they were seen for the first or the second time in the current session (‘familiarity neurons’). It is worth noting that in prior neurophysiological work examining responses to frequent exposure over extended time periods, response changes in PrC were observed in the absence of any behavioral requirement to judge the degree of € lscher et al., 2003; prior exposure across multiple sessions (Ho Xiang & Brown, 1998). In the current fMRI study, by contrast, participants made explicit judgments of cumulative lifetime experience with object concepts during scanning, allowing us to link the graded PrC signals to observed behavior. This behavioral relevance was revealed by performing a parametric examination of the relationship between the BOLD response and judgment outcome. An important question to consider is whether the response pattern in PrC we observed could also reflect differential engagement of episodic encoding processes during lifetime familiarity judgments. There is substantial evidence in the fMRI literature showing that PrC is not only involved in retrieval but also in the encoding (i.e., memory formation) of information about specific object encounters (see Kim, 2013; Diana et al., 2007 for review). Past fMRI research has revealed, for example, that PrC tracks the amount of item-related details that is successfully encoded when visually presented objects are encountered in a study phase, as reflected in performance on a subsequent memory test for these encounters (Staresina & Davachi, 2010). Also, neuroimaging research has shown that trial-specific activity in MTL structures during recognitionmemory decisions predict behavioral performance on a subsequent second memory test (Stark & Okado, 2003). Finally, behavioral findings obtained with the current task suggest that increases in lifetime familiarity of object concepts are

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

associated with increases in perceived pertinent knowledge.1 In light of this evidence, it is possible that the graded PrC response we observed also reflects encoding of varying amounts of semantic feature knowledge that is brought online as the outcome of varying degrees of lifetime experience. To explore a potential link between assessment of lifetime familiarity and episodic encoding, we conducted a supplementary behavioral experiment in which we examined whether degree of perceived lifetime familiarity modulates successful encoding during lifetime-familiarity judgements, such that performance on a subsequent recognition-memory test would be better for items with higher lifetime familiarity ratings (see Supplementary materials). The results of our behavioral experiment did indeed reveal such a pattern of performance. We found that object concepts with high as compared to low lifetime familiarity ratings received a subsequent increase in endorsement as previously studied items. Specifically, we observed an increase in subsequent ‘remember’ responses and a decrease in ‘know’ responses in a test paradigm that emphasized successful access to episodic contextual information as the critical difference between both response types (see Martin et al., 2011; McCabe & Geraci, 2009; for further rationale). To the extent that PrC has most consistently been implicated in encoding of information that supports item-based familiarity, rather than recollection of episodic context (e.g., Ranganath et al., 2004; Staresina & Davachi, 2008; for review see; Diana et al., 2007), the pattern of behavior observed can be seen as providing only limited support for an interpretation of the present fMRI findings in terms of encoding processes. However, the evidence currently available clearly does not rule out such an interpretation. First, Remember-Know paradigms rely on a subjective assessment as to whether a recognition judgment is based on item- or episodic-contextual information, and these subjective assessments do not always converge with the outcome of objective probing (see Diana & Ranganath, 2011; Phelps & Sharot, 2008; Wixted, 2009; for discussion). Moreover, they do not offer any direct measure of the amount of feature information available in item-based memory decisions. Second, firm conclusions require research that goes beyond behavioral experiments, including studies based on functional neuroimaging. Future fMRI research could examine, for example, whether activity differences in PrC for object concepts with different degrees of lifetime familiarity predict subsequent performance on memory tests for item information encountered during scanning. Regardless of the outcome of this future research, we note that a role for PrC in assessment of lifetime experience and in the encoding of specific object encounters are not mutually exclusive. Indeed, there is considerable evidence in the cognitive neuroscience literature that ties episodic encoding to retrieval of conceptual information from semantic 1

We conducted a behavioral experiment in a separate group of healthy young participants (n ¼ 31), in which we asked participants to judge not only lifetime familiarity but also the amount of knowledge they have for specific object concepts (again using items taken from McRae et al., 2005). The two types of ratings were obtained in separate blocks of the experiment. Analyses of the resulting data revealed a strong positive correlation (across items r ¼ .54, p < .05, averaged across participants) between perceived lifetime familiarity and estimated knowledge for object concepts.

67

memory (see Greenberg & Verfaellie, 2010; Prince, Tsukiura, & Cabeza, 2007; Tulving, 2002). As such, a demonstrated role of PrC in episodic encoding during lifetime-familiarity judgements would not be incompatible with the conclusions drawn in our prior neuropsychological research concerning a critical role of this structure in the assessment of lifetime familiarity. The current study primarily focused on PrC due to the prominent role this structure has played in prior neuroscience research on recognition memory in human and non-human animals (see Ranganath & Ritchey, 2012; Suzuki & Naya, 2014 for recent reviews). Critically, we found a response profile that was unique to left PrC in a conjunction contrast that probed for the typically observed decrease in BOLD signal based on recent exposure in combination with an increase in BOLD signal with increasing lifetime familiarity. However, when we examined the latter contrast in isolation, we also identified several other cortical regions whose BOLD response showed a positive relationship to the perceived cumulative lifetime familiarity of object concepts. These regions included the left hippocampus, left mid-lateral temporal cortex, and medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortices. Notably, these regions are part of the default-mode network, which has been proposed to support self-projection and internally directed thought that is often characterized by an autobiographical attentional focus (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014; Buckner & Carroll, 2007). Arguably, such a functional characterization also captures judgments of the lifetime familiarity of object concepts. A number of fMRI studies have shown that a subset of structures in the default-mode network, centered on the hippocampus, are frequently conjointly activated during episodic recollection in laboratory-based and in autobiographical memory tasks (see Kim et al., 2016; Spreng, Mar, & Kim, 2009; for reviews). Against this background it is interesting to consider whether episodic recollection may have played a role in the task we employed to probe cumulative lifetime familiarity in the current study. This could be the case even though our task had no explicit requirement to make reference to specific past object encounters (see Sheldon & Moscovitch, 2012; for related discussion). For example, while making their judgments, participants may have relied on monitoring the ease with which a pertinent episodic encounter came to mind. Although the available evidence that speaks to this issue is sparse, we note that recent neuropsychological findings argue against a critical (i.e., necessary) role for recollection in assessing cumulative lifetime familiarity. Specifically, we have found that an amnesic individual with well documented impairments in autobiographical recollection (patient HC), due to a lesion in the extended hippocampal system that spared PrC, displays no abnormalities in making judgments about lifetime familiarity (Bowles et al., 2016; Kwan, Carson, Addis and Rosenbaum, 2010). Considered in combination with the reported abnormal performance in patient NB, this neuropsychological evidence suggests a double dissociation between the recollection of particular autobiographical instances of object encounters, and the assessment of cumulative experience with such objects that has accrued over hundreds or thousands of different episodic contexts. Moreover, it suggests that the integrity of the hippocampus may not be necessary for making cumulative lifetime familiarity judgments. Future fMRI research can build on these neuropsychological findings and

68

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

directly compare MTL signals for judgments of cumulative lifetime familiarity with judgments that require recollection of specific autobiographical experiences in response to the same cues. Returning to PrC, the present findings add to a growing body of evidence that links this structure to the processing of object concepts in task contexts other than memory judgments for recent experimental study encounters. Several prior fMRI studies have employed priming paradigms in which repeated exposure to object concepts led to changes in PrC signals in semantic tasks that made no reference to these repetitions (Dew & Cabeza, 2011; Heusser et al., 2012; O'Kane, Insler, & Wagner, 2005; Voss, Hauner, & Paller, 2009; Wang, Lazzara, Ranganath, Knight, & Yonelinas, 2010, Wang et al., 2014). Past fMRI research has also implicated human PrC in processing of object concepts in tasks that did not involve any manipulation of item repetition, including object naming and judging the presence of specific semantic features (Bruffaerts et al., 2013; Clarke & Tyler, 2014; Liuzzi et al., 2015). In the latter set of studies, it has been shown that PrC carries information that is of particular relevance for making fine-grained distinctions among similar object concepts with high degrees of feature overlap. This evidence has been taken as support for models of PrC functioning that emphasize a broader role in feature integration that is not only critical for memory judgments but also for the representation of objects in perceptual and semantic tasks (Clark & Tyler, 2014; Cowell, Bussey, & Saksida, 2010; Graham, Barense, & Lee, 2010; Murray & Bussey, 1999; O'Neil, Protzner, McCormick, McLean, Poppenk € hler, 2012). An important topic for future and Cate and Ko research is to examine how the sensitivity of PrC to recent incremental changes and the perceived lifetime familiarity of object concepts we report here is related to computations that are critical for feature integration.

Acknowledgments We thank Asaf Gilboa and Shayna Rosenbaum for helpful discussion. This study was supported by a Discovery Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to SK (238661-2012).

Supplementary data Supplementary data related to this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.015.

references

Anderson, B., Mruczek, R. E., Kawasaki, K., & Sheinberg, D. (2008). Effects of familiarity on neural activity in monkey inferior temporal lobe. Cerebral Cortex, 18(11), 2540e2552. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn015. Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of

the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 29e52. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12360. Barense, M. D., Henson, R. N., & Graham, K. S. (2011). Perception and conception: Temporal lobe activity during complex discriminations of familiar and novel faces and objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 3052e3067. Binney, R. J., Embleton, K. V., Jefferies, E., Parker, G. J., & Ralph, M. A. L. (2010). The ventral and inferolateral aspects of the anterior temporal lobe are crucial in semantic memory: Evidence from a novel direct comparison of distortioncorrected fMRI, rTMS, and semantic dementia. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2728e2738. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq019. Bowles, B., Crupi, C., Mirsattari, S. M., Pigott, S. E., Parrent, A. G., Pruessner, J. C., et al. (2007). Impaired familiarity with preserved recollection after anterior temporal-lobe resection that spares the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(41), 16382e16387. € hler, S. Bowles, B., Duke, D., Rosenbaum, R. S., McRae, K., & Ko (2016). Impaired assessment of cumulative lifetime familiarity for object concepts after left anterior temporal-lobe resection that includes perirhinal cortex but spares the hippocampus. Neuropsychologia, 90, 170e179. Bridger, E. K., Bader, R., & Mecklinger, A. (2014). More ways than one: ERPs reveal multiple familiarity signals in the word frequency mirror effect. Neuropsychologia, 57, 179e190. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.03.007. Brown, M. W., & Aggleton, J. P. (2001). Recognition memory: What are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(1), 51e61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ 35049064. Bruffaerts, R., Dupont, P., Peeters, R., De Deyne, S., Storms, G., & Vandenberghe, R. (2013). Similarity of fMRI activity patterns in left perirhinal cortex reflects semantic similarity between words. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(47), 18597e18607. Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 49e57. Clarke, A., & Tyler, L. K. (2014). Object-specific semantic coding in human perirhinal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(14), 4766e4775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.282813.2014. Cowell, R. A., Bussey, T. J., & Saksida, L. M. (2010). Components of recognition memory: Dissociable cognitive processes or just differences in representational complexity? Hippocampus, 20(11), 1245e1262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20865. Cree, G. S., & McRae, K. (2003). Analyzing the factors underlying the structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunk, cherry, chisel, cheese, and cello (and many other such concrete nouns). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.163. Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Triple dissociation in the medial temporal lobes: Recollection, familiarity, and novelty. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(4), 1902e1911. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01029.2005. Dew, I. T., & Cabeza, R. (2011). The porous boundaries between explicit and implicit memory: Behavioral and neural evidence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224(1), 174e190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.17496632.2010.05946.x. Diana, R. A., & Ranganath, C. (2011). Recollection, familiarity and memory strength: Confusion about confounds. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 337e338. Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: A three-component model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 379e386. Dobbins, I. G., Simons, J. S., & Schacter, D. L. (2004). fMRI evidence for separable and lateralized prefrontal memory monitoring processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(6), 908e920.

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. R., & Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 123. Fahy, F. L., Riches, I. P., & Brown, M. W. (1993). Neuronal activity related to visual recognition memory: Long-term memory and the encoding of recency and familiarity information in the primate anterior and medial inferior temporal and rhinal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 96(3), 457e472. Friston, K. J., Fletcher, P., Josephs, O., Holmes, A., Rugg, M. D., & Turner, R. (1998). Event-related fMRI: Characterizing differential responses. NeuroImage, 7(1), 30e40. Gonsalves, B. D., Kahn, I., Curran, T., Norman, K. A., & Wagner, A. D. (2005). Memory strength and repetition suppression: Multimodal imaging of medial temporal cortical contributions to recognition. Neuron, 47(5), 751e761. Graham, K. S., Barense, M. D., & Lee, A. C. (2010). Going beyond LTM in the MTL: A synthesis of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings on the role of the medial temporal lobe in memory and perception. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 831e853. Greenberg, D. L., & Verfaellie, M. (2010). Interdependence of episodic and semantic memory: Evidence from neuropsychology. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(05), 748e753. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ S1355617710000676. Grill-Spector, K. (2006). Selectivity of adaptation in single units: Implications for FMRI experiments. Neuron, 49(2), 170e171. Henson, R. N. A., Cansino, S., Herron, J. E., Robb, W. G. K., & Rugg, M. D. (2003). A familiarity signal in human anterior medial temporal cortex? Hippocampus, 13(2), 301e304. Heusser, A. C., Awipi, T., & Davachi, L. (2013). The ups and downs of repetition: Modulation of the perirhinal cortex by conceptual repetition predicts priming and long-term memory. Neuropsychologia, 51(12), 2333e2343. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.018. Hintzman, D. L. (2004). Judgment of frequency versus recognition confidence: Repetition and recursive reminding. Memory & Cognition, 32(2), 336e350. Hintzman, D. L., & Curran, T. (1994). Retrieval dynamics of recognition and frequency judgments: Evidence for separate processes of familiarity and recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(1), 1e18. € lscher, C., Rolls, E. T., & Xiang, J. (2003). Perirhinal cortex Ho neuronal activity related to long-term familiarity memory in the macaque. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18(7), 2037e2046. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02903.x. Jackson, R. L., Hoffman, P., Pobric, G., & Ralph, M. A. L. (2016). The semantic network at work and Rest: Differential connectivity of anterior temporal lobe subregions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(5), 1490e1501. Kim, H. (2013). Differential neural activity in the recognition of old versus new events: An Activation Likelihood Estimation MetaAnalysis. Human Brain Mapping, 34(4), 814e836. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21474. Kim, H. (2016). Default network activation during episodic and semantic memory retrieval: A selective meta-analytic comparison. Neuropsychologia, 80, 35e46. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.006. Kwan, D., Carson, N., Addis, D. R., & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2010). Deficits in past remembering extend to future imagining in a case of developmental amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 48(11), 3179e3186. Li, L., Miller, E. K., & Desimone, R. (1993). The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 69(6), 1918e1929. Liuzzi, A. G., Bruffaerts, R., Dupont, P., Adamczuk, K., Peeters, R., De Deyne, S., et al. (2015). Left perirhinal cortex codes for

69

similarity in meaning between written words: Comparison with auditory word input. Neuropsychologia, 76, 4e16. Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T., & Oeltermann, A. (2001). Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature, 412(6843), 150e157. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1038/35084005. Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87(3), 252. Mandler, G. (2008). Familiarity breeds attempts: A critical review of dual-process theories of recognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 390e399. Martin, A. (2015). GRAPESdgrounding representations in action, perception, and emotion systems: How object properties and categories are represented in the human brain. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1e12. € hler, S. (2011). Martin, C. B., Bowles, B., Mirsattari, S. M., & Ko Selective familiarity deficits after left anterior temporal-lobe removal with hippocampal sparing are material specific. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1870e1878. € hler, S. Martin, C. B., Cowell, R. A., Gribble, P. L., Wright, J., & Ko (2016). Distributed category-specific recognition memory signals in human perirhinal cortex. Hippocampus, 26, 423e436. € hler, S. (2013). Martin, C. B., McLean, D. A., O'Neil, E. B., & Ko Distinct familiarity-based response patterns for faces and buildings in perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(26), 10915e10923. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0126-13.2013. McCabe, D. P., & Geraci, L. D. (2009). The influence of instructions and terminology on the accuracy of remember-know judgments. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(2), 401e413. McRae, K., Cree, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & McNorgan, C. (2005). Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things. Behavior Research Methods, 37(4), 547e559. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03192726. Miller, E. K., Li, L., & Desimone, R. (1991). A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. Science, 254(5036), 1377e1379. Montaldi, D., & Mayes, A. R. (2010). The role of recollection and familiarity in the functional differentiation of the medial temporal lobes. Hippocampus, 20(11), 1291e1314. Montaldi, D., Spencer, T. J., Roberts, N., & Mayes, A. R. (2006). The neural system that mediates familiarity memory. Hippocampus, 16(5), 504e520. Moreno-Martı´nez, F. J., Montoro, P. R., & Rodrı´guez-Rojo, I. C. (2014). Spanish norms for age of acquisition, concept familiarity, lexical frequency, manipulability, typicality, and other variables for 820 words from 14 living/nonliving concepts. Behavior Research Methods, 46(4), 1088e1097. http:// dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0435-x. Murray, E. A., & Bussey, T. J. (1999). Perceptual-mnemonic functions of the perirhinal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(4), 142e151. Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T., & Poline, J. B. (2005). Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. NeuroImage, 25(3), 653e660. O'Kane, G., Insler, R. Z., & Wagner, A. D. (2005). Conceptual and perceptual novelty effects in human medial temporal cortex. Hippocampus, 15(3), 326e332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ hipo.20053. O'Neil, E. B., Protzner, A. B., McCormick, C., McLean, D. A., Poppenk, J., Cate, A. D., et al. (2012). Distinct patterns of functional and effective connectivity between perirhinal cortex and other cortical regions in recognition memory and perceptual discrimination. Cerebral Cortex, 22(1), 74e85. Phelps, E. A., & Sharot, T. (2008). How (and why) emotion enhances the subjective sense of recollection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), 147e152.

70

c o r t e x 8 9 ( 2 0 1 7 ) 6 1 e7 0

Prince, S. E., Tsukiura, T., & Cabeza, R. (2007). Distinguishing the neural correlates of episodic memory encoding and semantic memory retrieval. Psychological Science, 18(2), 144e151. Ranganath, C., & Ritchey, M. (2012). Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(10), 713e726. Ranganath, C., Yonelinas, A. P., Cohen, M. X., Dy, C. J., Tom, S. M., & D'Esposito, M. (2004). Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity within the medial temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 2e13. Rolls, E. T., Franco, L., & Stringer, S. M. (2005). The perirhinal cortex and long-term familiarity memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 58(3e4), 234e245. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1080/02724990444000122. € der, A., Gemballa, T., Ruppin, S., & Wartenburger, I. (2012). Schro German norms for semantic typicality, age of acquisition, and concept familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 380e394. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0164-y. Sheldon, S., & Moscovitch, M. (2012). The nature and time-course of medial temporal lobe contributions to semantic retrieval: An fMRI study on verbal fluency. Hippocampus, 22(6), 1451e1466. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20985. Skipper, L. M., Ross, L. A., & Olson, I. R. (2011). Sensory and semantic category subdivisions within the anterior temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 49(12), 3419e3429. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.033. Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative metaanalysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489e510. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21029. Staresina, B. P., & Davachi, L. (2008). Selective and shared contributions of the hippocampus and perirhinal cortex to episodic item and associative encoding. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(8), 1478e1489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/ jocn.2008.20104. Staresina, B. P., & Davachi, L. (2010). Object unitization and associative memory formation are supported by distinct brain regions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(29), 9890e9897. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0826-10.2010.

Stark, C. E., & Okado, Y. (2003). Making memories without trying: Medial temporal lobe activity associated with incidental memory formation during recognition. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(17), 6748e6753. Suzuki, W. A., & Naya, Y. (2014). The perirhinal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37, 39e53. Thome, A., Erickson, C. A., Lipa, P., & Barnes, C. A. (2012). Differential effects of experience on tuning properties of macaque MTL neurons in a passive viewing task. Hippocampus, 22(10), 2000e2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22070. Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 1e25. Voss, J. L., Hauner, K. K., & Paller, K. A. (2009). Establishing a relationship between activity reduction in human perirhinal cortex and priming. Hippocampus, 19(9), 773. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1002/hipo.20608. Wang, W. C., Lazzara, M. M., Ranganath, C., Knight, R. T., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2010). The medial temporal lobe supports conceptual implicit memory. Neuron, 68(5), 835e842. Wang, W. C., Ranganath, C., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2014). Activity reductions in perirhinal cortex predict conceptual priming and familiarity-based recognition. Neuropsychologia, 52, 19e26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.006. Wixted, J. T. (2009). Remember/know judgments in cognitive neuroscience: An illustration of the underrepresented point of view. Learning & Memory, 16(7), 406e412. Wixted, J. T., & Squire, L. R. (2011). The medial temporal lobe and the attributes of memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(5), 210e217. Wright, P., Randall, B., Clarke, A., & Tyler, L. K. (2015). The perirhinal cortex and conceptual processing: Effects of feature-based statistics following damage to the anterior temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 76, 192e207. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.041. Xiang, J. Z., & Brown, M. W. (1998). Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe. Neuropharmacology, 37(4), 657e676. Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441e517.

Perirhinal cortex tracks degree of recent as well as ...

a Brain and Mind Institute and Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada ... E-mail address: stefank@uwo.ca (S. Köhler).

1MB Sizes 1 Downloads 133 Views

Recommend Documents

1499590276607-crowd-episode-health-as-well-as-wellness ...
... Other BusyAdults InMind. Page 2 of 2. 1499590276607-crowd-episode-health-as-well-as-wellness-management-of-excess-photobook-ebook.pdf.

Application for AA or AS Degree
Application for AA or AS Degree. Please review the ... DEGREE lN FORMATION. Major for which you are ... Address City State Zip C ode. I understand that it is ...

Early Visual Cortex as a Multiscale Cognitive ... - Floris de Lange
18 Jul 2016 - Abstract. Neurons in early visual cortical areas not only represent incoming visual in- formation but are also engaged by higher level cognitive processes, including attention, working memory, imagery, and decision-making. Are these cog

Universal Quantification and Degree Modification as ...
between (im)precision, scale structure and mereological structure: Burnett (2012)'s Delineation Tolerant, Classical,. Strict and its mereological extension (Burnett ... Adopting an extension-based definition of scale structure: 1. If P is a relative

Make as Much Money as You Damn Well Want and ...
Book synopsis. Are you TIRED of the RAT RACE? Do you wish you had MORE TIME and MORE MONEY? Would you like to NEVER. WORK AGAIN?

role-advertising-bigger-visibility-on-google-as-well-as-bing-seo ...
Whoops! There was a problem loading this page. role-advertising-bigger-visibility-on-google-as-well-as-bing-seo-network-business-1499591450957.pdf.

the-trough-carb-plump-deficiency-as-well-as-ballast ...
... loading more pages. Retrying... the-trough-carb-plump-deficiency-as-well-as-ballast-l ... iserable-fat-the-accepted-wisdom-is-1499492895672.pdf.

Hello! Hope you are doing well. As far as I am ...
part of robust, science based medical ... online – http://sachinkale1.tripod.com. (1, Cont. ) The Monthly Microscope .... Я High degree of correlation was noted between 650 paired CD4 and TLC counts (r = 0.744). ... Я Selection of appropriate TLC

PDF F.U. Money: Make As Much Money As You Damn Well Want And ...
... detailing an exchange Express Helpline Get answer of your question fast from real ... How can I make enough money to afford to STOP WORKING and START ...

well, transpiration cooling occurs and as the plant
Management practices to improve sugarcane yield and quality under moisture stress condition during formative phase. Ann. Rep., Sugarcane Breeding lnst. Coimbatore. p.12. (Received: April 2000; Revised: December 2001) ... in utilizing solar radiation

role-advertising-bigger-visibility-on-google-as-well ...
... HowIt CanHelp Build Your Business . Page 2 of 2. role-advertising-bigger-visibility-on-google-as-well-as-bing-seo-network-business-1499591450957.pdf.

A Role for Perirhinal Cortex in Memory for Novel Object–Context ...
Mar 28, 2012 - fined, in dual-process views of MTL function, the HC is respon- .... in a practice session, and of the remaining 720 objects, 360 were pre-.

On the placement of an obstacle or a well so as to ...
of the Laplacian or of a Schrödinger operator defined within a fixed, bounded, ... it is only necessary for B to have a certain reflection symmetry, but we have ...

Chapter 5 Health as an Indicator of Well-Being in ...
Elephants are wild animals. They have evolved over millennia to survive in the savannas and forests of Africa and Asia. They have developed physiological systems designed to meet conditions defined by these environments. Their evolution has resulted