Current Directions in Psychological Science http://cdp.sagepub.com/

Attention, Distraction, and Cognitive Control Under Load Nilli Lavie Current Directions in Psychological Science 2010 19: 143 DOI: 10.1177/0963721410370295 The online version of this article can be found at: http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/19/3/143

Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Association for Psychological Science

Additional services and information for Current Directions in Psychological Science can be found at: Email Alerts: http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://cdp.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at UCL Library Services on February 24, 2011

Attention, Distraction, and Cognitive Control Under Load

Current Directions in Psychological Science 19(3) 143-148 ª The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721410370295 http://cdps.sagepub.com

Nilli Lavie Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London

Abstract The extent to which people can focus attention in the face of irrelevant distractions has been shown to critically depend on the level and type of information load involved in their current task. The ability to focus attention improves under task conditions of high perceptual load but deteriorates under conditions of high load on cognitive control processes such as working memory. I review recent research on the effects of load on visual awareness and brain activity, including changing effects over the life span, and I outline the consequences for distraction and inattention in daily life and in clinical populations. Keywords attention, distraction, executive control, perception, load, working memory, capacity limits

Inability to focus attention in the face of irrelevant distractions is a common, and often rather frustrating, experience. The consequences range from merely reducing the quality of life (e.g., not being able to focus while reading a good book, or even this article) to affecting the ability to study or to concentrate at work and causing one to be more prone to accidents (e.g., while driving). A main goal of attention theory is to delineate the determinants of focused attention that allow people to ignore irrelevant distractions. This goal, however, has proved rather hard to reach, and the very question of whether attention can ever affect the perception of distractors has been controversial ever since attention research began in the late fifties. Many striking demonstrations of people failing even to notice various distractors when focusing attention on their task (e.g., people attending to a ball game have failed to notice a woman walking across the pitch and holding up an umbrella) have led to an ‘‘early selection’’ view, according to which people have limited perceptual processing capacity and will perceive just what they attend to. Unattended distractors in this view are fully ignored: They are simply never perceived. But many observations of interference effects from irrelevant distractors have also accumulated. These seemed to accord with a converse ‘‘late selection’’ view, in which perception is seen as an automatic process both in the sense that it has unlimited capacity (so that everything is perceived, whether relevant or irrelevant to the current task) and in the sense that it is mandatory (so that people cannot shut down perception of irrelevant information simply because they wish to).

Load Theory of Attention and Cognitive Control Recent research on the load theory of attention and cognitive control (Lavie, Hirst, De Fockert, & Viding, 2004) offers a resolution to this debate while also clarifying the major determinants of successful focused attention and cognitive control. The load theory resolves the early- and late-selection debate by combining within one hybrid model the early-selection assumption that perception has limited capacity and the late-selection assumption that perception is an automatic process (in the sense that it is involuntary and so cannot be shut down at will). It follows, then, that tasks involving high perceptual load that engage full capacity will simply leave no capacity for irrelevant distractor perception (leading to an early-selection result). In contrast, in tasks of low perceptual load, spare capacity remaining beyond the task-relevant processing spills over involuntarily to irrelevant distractor processing (leading to late-selection results). The efficiency of late selection (that is, the extent to which distractors that have been perceived can be prevented from gaining control over behavior) depends on the level of load on cognitive-control functions such as working memory. High working memory

Corresponding Author: Nilli Lavie, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR UK E-mail: [email protected]

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at UCL Library Services on February 24, 2011

144

Lavie A

C

B

K

K Z X

M H

N

M H W

W

Low Load

E Awareness Reports (Percentages)

D 100 Mean Distractor Cost (in ms)

Z X

High Load 80 60 40 20 0 Response Competition

Low Load 100

High Load

80 60 40 20 0

Attentional Capture

Inattentional Blindness

Fig. 1. Perceptual load effects on distraction: example stimuli and results. Panel (a) depicts an example of a high-load display from Forster and Lavie (2007, 2008). The response-competition paradigm shown in this panel is used to measure distraction. Subjects make speeded responses indicating which of two pre-specified letters (X or N) is present in the letter circle, attempting to ignore a peripheral distractor letter. Slower responses in the presence of a response-incongruent distractor (shown) compared with a congruent distractor (e.g., distractor X for target X) indicate that the distractor produced response-competition effects. (In the low-load condition, the target letter X or N is presented among small os so that it appears to ‘‘pop out’’ of the display.) Irrelevant distractor images of famous cartoon characters are presented on a few of the low- and high-load trials in Forster and Lavie (2008; Panel b). Panel (c) shows an example display from Cartwright-Finch and Lavie (2007), used in both the low- and high-load conditions. In the low-load condition, subjects were instructed to report which of the cross arms was green (horizontal or vertical); in the high-load condition, they were to report which was longer (the small length difference makes this task highly demanding). An extra stimulus (e.g., the small square shown on the top left) is presented unexpectedly on the very last trial, and as soon as the subjects respond to the task on that last trial, they are asked whether they noticed the presence of any extra stimulus. The results from Forster and Lavie’s (2008) experiments are shown in (d). In the low-load conditions, the distractor interferes with task performance (mean distractor cost). Incongruent (compared to congruent) distractor letters produce response competition effects and the presence (compared to absence) of a task-irrelevant distractor cartoon produces attentional capture effects. These distractor interference effects are eliminated with high perceptual load. Panel (e) shows the results from Cartwright-Finch and Lavie’s (2007) experiments: The high load significantly reduces the percentage of awareness reports; in this condition, 90% of the subjects fail to notice the extra task-irrelevant stimulus.

load during task performance results in greater distractor interference. The load theory has received much empirical support (see Lavie, 2005, 2006 for reviews). In this article, I review some of the recent research on the effects of load on visual awareness and brain activity, including findings of changes over the course of development and aging, and relate these findings to distraction and inattention in daily life (e.g., in educational settings, driving, and at work) and in clinical populations.

Perceptual Load and Daily Life Distractions Some of the early perceptual load studies used the response competition paradigm, since this is the most conventional laboratory index of distraction (Fig. 1a). Response competition effects were found under tasks with low but not high levels of perceptual load (Lavie, 2005, Fig. 1d). More recently, Forster and Lavie (2007) asked how these laboratory findings relate to individual differences in daily life distractibility. We

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at UCL Library Services on February 24, 2011

Attention, Distraction, and Cognitive Control Under Load

145

A

B Low Working Memory Load High Working Memory Load

****** Z X

4

Mean Distractor RT Cost (in ms)

821462

100 80 60 40 20 0

Fig. 2. Working memory load effects on distraction: example stimuli and results. Panel (a) shows an example trial with a high working memory load (low perceptual load) from Lavie et al. (2004). Subjects are asked to memorize the set of digits presented at the start of each trial and rehearse this set during performance of a selective attention task (a response competition task was used in this study). A probe digit is presented at the end of each trial and subjects are asked to indicate whether it was present or absent in the memory set. In the low-load condition (not shown), only one memory set digit is presented. Panel (b) shows the results: Working-memory load has the opposite effect on distraction that perceptual load does: Distractor response competition effects (mean distractor response time, RT, costs) are greater in high than in low working memory load.

assessed the individual magnitude of distractor response competition effects under task conditions of either low or high perceptual load (Fig. 1a). Participants also completed the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ; Broadbent, Cooper, FitzGerald, & Parkes, 1982), an established measure of inattention and distractibility in daily life. High CFQ scorers report being more prone to inattention incidences such as ‘‘starting doing one thing at home and getting distracted into doing something else’’ (a questionnaire item). We found that high CFQ scores were also associated with greater distractor interference effects in our task as long as it was conducted under a low load. High perceptual load in the task significantly reduced distractor interference for all people, including those highly distracted both in our low-load task and in daily life. High CFQ scores are known to be associated with increased risk of various types of careless errors (e.g., losing unsaved work when computing) and accidents during driving or on the job (e.g., Wallace & Vodanovich, 2003). The Forster and Lavie (2007) findings therefore may be important in two senses. First, the positive correlation between the behavioral distraction measure and propensity to distraction in daily life suggests that this measure can be used as a distraction test. The level of test performance can then be used to predict which individuals are more distractible and accident prone in daily life. Second, modifications of some daily tasks to involve a higher level of perceptual load may prove useful for focusing attention for all people, even those who are otherwise highly distractible.

In daily life, many sources of distraction are entirely unrelated to the current task. For example, while searching the Internet for information related to the topic of study (e.g., some of the references mentioned in this article), you may get distracted by a popup ad with unrelated content (e.g., advertising a new Internet dating service) or the sight of your spouse hovering around, or even simply the sun breaking through a gloomy sky. Some distractions may feel more welcome than others; nonetheless they may all interfere with successful concentration on the task, with the irrelevant stimuli effectively capturing attention. Would tasks of higher perceptual load be less prone to such irrelevant attentional capture? Forster and Lavie (2008) introduced a laboratory measure of attentional capture by task-irrelevant stimuli, presenting large, meaningful, salient distractors (Fig. 1b) that are likely to capture attention even when entirely irrelevant and bearing no relation to the lettersearch task performed. We found that such task-unrelated distractors did indeed capture attention and disrupt task performance, but only in conditions of low load. High load in the letter task eliminated any task-irrelevant attentional capture (Fig. 1d). In another study (Lavie, Lin, Zokaei, & Thoma, 2009), subjects performed a letter-search task while attempting to ignore a wide range of meaningful but task-unrelated distractors (e.g., a pictured spider or car). Following the attention task, subjects received a surprise memory-recognition task. The results showed that even when the distractor objects were

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at UCL Library Services on February 24, 2011

146

Lavie

presented directly where people were gazing, they could only recognize having previously seen these distractors when the task at exposure involved a low load. Recognition memory fell to chance levels in task conditions of high perceptual load. This was also the case when human faces were presented as irrelevant distractors (Jenkins, Lavie, & Driver, 2005). The fact that recognition memory levels are no better than chance in tasks of high perceptual load has important implications for the reliability of eyewitness testimony: Clearly one has to take into account the level of perceptual load involved at exposure when considering such memory-based testimonies.

Awareness and Mind Wandering Under Load The aforementioned distractor recognition studies have also tested distractor recognition upon immediate promoting. The results indicated that people fail to recognize distractor objects just viewed under conditions of high perceptual load, exhibiting a form of inattentional blindness. Inattentional blindness can also be manifest in people failing to notice the mere presence of simple shapes in tasks of higher perceptual load (Cartwright-Finch & Lavie, 2007; Fig. 1e). Although blindness to irrelevant distractors can be a beneficial effect of perceptual load (as it helps to focus attention on the task), there are of course situations in which this blindness is undesirable. For example, while in daily life tasks such as driving, it is often vital that people are not blind to the presence of other vehicles on the road and are able to detect and recognize the meaning of the simple shapes used as road signs. This has begun to be noted by some driving authorities (e.g., Traffic for London), which have made recent attempts to improve drivers’ awareness with advertisements informing about the potential consequences of inattentional blindness and requesting drivers to watch out for pedestrians, motorcyclists, and so on. Enhancing drivers’ intentions to avoid inattentional blindness is an important first step, but the implications of the load research are that the effects of intention alone are rather limited. In tasks of high perceptual load, people may continue to ‘‘look but not see’’ (Fig. 1e). Indeed, a recent study demonstrates that people remain inattentionally blind to peripheral stimuli under high perceptual load even when fully intending to detect their appearance as instructed (Macdonald & Lavie, 2008). Signal-detection analyses of these data (used to isolate effects on perceptual sensitivity from those on response criterion) confirm that high perceptual load reduces perceptual sensitivity to other stimuli, rather than affecting response criterion or bias. This suggests that it is important to consider ways of actually lowering the level of perceptual load (e.g., restricting the number and location of advertising billboards) in parts of the road that are likely to demand more attention (e.g., at junctions). So far I have considered external sources of distraction, but perhaps most frustrating is the experience of distraction produced by one’s own mind. Picture the following scenario: You have escaped any source of external distraction, being tucked away in a very quiet place. Now is the time to devote attention

to that important assignment that demands your full concentration. But soon after starting, you find your mind drifting away into unrelated thoughts. You have generated your own internal form of distraction and are engaged in mind wandering instead of attending to your task. Can this form of distraction be overcome? Our recent research (Forster & Lavie, 2009) suggests that it can: Mind-wandering rates are significantly reduced in tasks of higher perceptual load. Note that as the manipulation of perceptual load in this study mainly increased the amount of visual stimulation in the task without adding to the task complexity, the results suggest it may also be possible to reduce mind wandering during the performance of cognitively demanding tasks (e.g., learning in educational settings) without altering their semantic content or complexity (e.g., merely by enhancing the course material with visual presentations that provide higher perceptual load).

Implications for Development, Aging, and Clinical Populations The effects of perceptual load are due to a limited processing capacity. Once capacity is reached (in high-load tasks), none is left for any additional processing. Interestingly, it may be possible to enhance perceptual capacity with certain types of training. Moreover, some clinical conditions (e.g., autism spectrum condition, congenital deafness) may involve some atypical enhancements of perceptual capacity. The higher capacity in these cases allows people to perceive both task-relevant and task-irrelevant information under higher levels of perceptual load (e.g., Bavelier, Dye, & Hauser, 2006; Remington, Swettenham, Campbell, & Coleman, 2009) than in matched control groups. What about conditions that involve reduced processing capacity? These have been identified for patients with brain damage in areas that are thought to be critically involved in attentional capacity (e.g., the parietal cortex). It is also known that processing capacity develops throughout childhood and deteriorates in old age. Thus, younger children and elderly people have reduced perceptual processing capacity compared to middle adulthood. Fortunately when it comes to distraction, reduced processing capacity may actually have some positive implications. According to load theory, reduced capacity should lead to the benefit of reduced perception of irrelevant distractors at a lower level of perceptual load. The benefits for selective attention at smaller increases in perceptual load have now been found for all of these populations (Lavie, 2005, for review).

Cognitive Control and the Frontal Lobe Under Load The studies reviewed so far clearly indicate that all people fail to ignore distractors in tasks of low perceptual load. Moreover, although small increases of load already improve focused attention for children and elderly people, both populations were found to be more vulnerable to distractor interference at very

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at UCL Library Services on February 24, 2011

Attention, Distraction, and Cognitive Control Under Load low perceptual load than were young adults, and some individuals remain more distracted than others in low-load tasks throughout adulthood (Forster & Lavie, 2007). What determines the level of distractor interference in conditions of low perceptual load, in which early perceptual selection fails? In other words, what determines the efficiency with which people achieve ‘‘late selection,’’ ensuring that distractors which have been perceived do not gain full control over behavior? Here it is important to consider higher-level, or ‘‘executive,’’ ‘‘cognitive’’ control functions, such as working memory, that actively maintain current priorities and behavioral goals during task performance. Neuropsychological studies have established the importance of an intact frontal lobe for goal-directed control of behavior. Following damage to the frontal lobe, patients are often characterized as suffering from a ‘‘disexecutive syndrome,’’ being unable to plan or maintain behavior in line with current goals and to suppress responses to goal-irrelevant distractors. The frontal lobe is known to be the last to develop and the first to deteriorate at older age. One may then explain the increase in distraction in the low-load conditions found for the children and elderly as the result of their reduced frontal cognitive control capacity. Indeed, individual differences in distractibility are also associated with individual differences in cognitive control capacities (Engle, 2002). Of course one cannot make causal inferences on the basis of correlative individual differences results or the co-occurrence of symptoms following a large brain lesion. We therefore set out to directly manipulate the availability of cognitive control to an attention task by requesting healthy people to perform the task under either low or high cognitive control load (Fig. 2). Note that the high-load conditions in these studies are expected to mimic the effect of frontal lobe damage and result in greater irrelevant distraction, the opposite effect to that of perceptual load. Cognitive control functions are loaded when people have to switch back and forth between different tasks or when people have to actively maintain in working memory some taskunrelated material (e.g., a random sequence of digits) during task performance. Studies using such load manipulations have shown that irrelevant distraction (measured, for example, with response competition and attentional capture effects) is increased with higher cognitive control load (Lavie et al., 2004; Fig. 2; Lavie & De Fockert, 2005). Recent research has also shown that tactile distraction (interference by an irrelevant touch) is greater under conditions of higher working memory load (Dalton, Lavie, & Spence, 2009) and that multiple-task coordination loads more on cognitive control, hence resulting in greater distraction, when the tasks involve different modalities (e.g., vision and hearing) than when both recruit the same modality (Brand-D’Abrescia & Lavie, 2008). The opposite effects of perceptual load and cognitive control load show that it is important to consider the precise nature of mental processes that are loaded in a given task. The opposite pattern (more distraction with high cognitive control load but less with high perceptual load) also rules out general task difficulty as an account for the effects of either type of load.

147

Brain Processing of Distractors Under Load The load theory makes rather strong predictions regarding brain activity. In this theory, the brain is expected to respond to task-irrelevant stimuli even if people wish to ignore these when the relevant task only involves low perceptual load. In contrast, high perceptual load should reduce or even eliminate the brain response to task-irrelevant stimuli. The first study to test these predictions used fMRI to image brain activity during task performance and visual distraction, under conditions of either low or high perceptual load. Rees, Frith, & Lavie (1997) presented a distracting moving star-field array in the background while people focused attention on a word task presented at the center of the screen. Response to the taskirrelevant motion was seen in a network of motionresponsive sensory brain areas under low load in the word task, but not under high task load. Since then, many neuroimaging studies have demonstrated similar modulations of distractor-related brain responses with manipulations of perceptual load. High perceptual load has been reported to reduce or even eliminate the neural signature of meaningful distractor images (e.g., depicting a place or a familiar object) in the brain regions that specialize in coding them. The responses to visually salient, flickering highcontrast stimuli in early visual cortex (including primary visual cortex) are also significantly reduced in tasks of high (compared to low) perceptual load (Lavie, 2005). These findings demonstrate inattentional blindness in the brain and suggest that the experience of inattentional blindness under high perceptual load may be the result of the weak sensory brain response to the distractor stimuli under such conditions. Interestingly, even the differential brain response (e.g., in the amygdala, a brain area known to mediate emotional processing) to distractors of different emotional content (e.g., angry versus happy distractor faces) is eliminated with tasks of high perceptual load. This effect is found for all people, including highly anxious individuals who show a larger brain response to emotional stimuli in tasks of low load (Bishop, Jenkins, & Lawrence, 2007). These findings have the positive implication that one may be able to reduce emotional distractions (a desirable goal especially when one is highly anxious) by engaging in a task with high perceptual load (e.g., a video game). In an important contrast with the effects of perceptual load, high working memory load has been shown to increase distractor-related responses in the brain, in line with the loadtheory predictions and the behavioral research (De Fockert, Rees, Frith, & Lavie, 2001).

Conclusions Load theory has generated much research that has furthered our understanding of attention, awareness and cognitive control, and their neural correlates. An important goal for future research would be to unravel the exact neural mechanisms mediating the effects of load both within and across the different sensory modalities. Further explorations of the potential beneficial effects

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at UCL Library Services on February 24, 2011

148

Lavie

of load for different clinical populations could help these groups to capitalize on their points of strength and gain greater control over their attention. Research into the applied daily life implications of load theory has just begun. Future research advancing this direction should prove useful for improving performance and productivity in a wide variety of real-world settings that require focused attention and goal-directed behavioral control yet include many potential distractions. Recommended Reading Bishop, S.J., Jenkins, R., & Lawrence, A. (2007). (See References). An interesting application of the load theory to research of the brain mechanisms involved in emotional control. Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J.W., & Viding, E. (2004). (See References). Discusses the load theory and its relation to research of executive control in more detail than the current article. Lavie, N., & Robertson, I. (2001). The role of perceptual load in visual neglect: Rejection of ipsilesional distractors is facilitated with higher central load. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 867– 876. Provides an empirical example of the beneficial effects of perceptual load for patients with visual neglect. Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 183–197. Discusses the early versus late selection debate and reviews the related literature in more detail than the current paper.

Acknowledgments I am grateful to Jon Driver, Sophie Forster, and Nick Berggren for their valuable feedback. I also thank my husband and boys for letting me write this article with minimal distractions while being on holiday in a beautiful, remote, and quiet island, leaving me only prone to mind wandering as the main source of irrelevant distraction.

Funding Preparation of this article was supported by Wellcome Trust Grant WT080568MA.

References Bavelier, D., Dye, M.W.G., & Hauser, P. (2006). Do deaf individuals see better? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 512–518. Bishop, S.J., Jenkins, R., & Lawrence, A. (2007). The neural processing of task-irrelevant fearful faces: Effects of perceptual load and individual differences in trait and state anxiety. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1595–603. Brand-D’Abrescia, M., & Lavie, N. (2008). Task coordination between and within sensory modalities: Effects on distraction. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 508–515.

Broadbent, D.E., Cooper, P.F., FitzGerald, P., & Parkes, K.R. (1982). The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21, 1–16. Cartwright-Finch, U., & Lavie, N. (2007). The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102, 321–340. Dalton, P., Lavie, N., & Spence, C. (2009). The role of working memory in tactile selective attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 635–644. De Fockert, J.W., Rees, G., Frith, C.D., & Lavie, N. (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 291, 1803–1806. Engle, R.W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23. Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2007). High perceptual load makes everybody equal: Eliminating individual differences in distractibility with load. Psychological Science, 18, 377–382. Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: The role of load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 73–83. Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the wandering mind: The role of perceptual load. Cognition, 111, 345–55. Jenkins, R., Lavie, N., & Driver, J.S. (2005). Recognition memory for distractor faces depends on attentional load at exposure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 314–320. Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 75–82. Lavie, N. (2006). The role of perceptual load in visual awareness. Brain Research, 1080, 91–100. Lavie, N., & De Fockert, J.W. (2005). The role of working memory in attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 669–674. Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J.W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354. Lavie, N., Lin, Z., Zokaei, N., & Thoma, V. (2009). The role of perceptual load in object recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1346–1358. Macdonald, J., & Lavie, N. (2008). Load induced blindness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1078–1091. Rees, G., Frith, C., & Lavie, N. (1997). Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science, 278, 1616–1619. Remington, A., Swettenham, J., Campbell, R., & Coleman, M. (2009). Selective attention and perceptual load in autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Science, 20, 1388–1393. Wallace, J.C., & Vodanovich, S.J. (2003). Can accidents and industrial mishaps be predicted? Further investigation into the relationship between cognitive failure and reports of accidents. Journal of Business and Psychology, 17, 503–514.

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at UCL Library Services on February 24, 2011

Science Current Directions in Psychological

doing one thing at home and getting distracted into doing some- thing else'' (a ... a new Internet dating service) or the sight of your spouse hover- ing around, or .... ''disexecutive syndrome,'' being unable to plan or maintain behavior in line ...

212KB Sizes 3 Downloads 297 Views

Recommend Documents

Current Directions In Psychological Science - Semantic Scholar
Feb 4, 2014 - chology; the image of an infant peering into a checker- board-patterned abyss is among the most famous icons in developmental science. Furthermore, the basic find- ings are well known to the thousands of students who have sat through in

Current Trends and Future Directions in Data Curation Research ...
Current Trends and Future Directions in Data Curation Research and Education.pdf. Current Trends and Future Directions in Data Curation Research and ...

Psychological Science
May 2, 2011 - typically focused on the processing involved in the retrieval attempt itself. For example, the ... One Brookings Dr., St. Louis, MO 63130. E-mail: ...

Psychological Science
Jan 12, 2011 - E-mail: [email protected] ..... choice between two lotteries for Amazon.co.uk gift certifi- cates: “a good ..... Journal of Marketing.

Psychological Science
2014 25: 2027 originally published online 8 October 2014 .... completed physical training or computer training, attended ...... Advance online publica-.

Psychological Science
Jan 16, 2014 - performance increased with lag (F5,50 = 18.14, p < 0.001), indicating that this color RSVP design produces an attention blink (Supplementary ...

Psychological Science
2014 25: 1286 originally published online 3 April 2014 .... 1Centre d'Économie de la Sorbonne, CNRS UMR 8174; 2Paris School of Economics; 3Laboratoire des Systèmes Perceptifs, CNRS UMR 8248, .... Peirce, C. S., & Jastrow, J. (1884).

Psychological Science
Mar 5, 2012 - tified the top five health-information Web sites produced by a ... judgment, prediction, social cognition, decision making, health, randomness cognition ... vidual Web pages for the 12 forms of cancer that were most .... whereas the rem

Psychological Science
Jan 9, 2013 - Email Alerts: ... Moreover, this finding implicates a relatively low level of visual coding, because ... Australia. E-mail: [email protected].

Psychological Science
Psychological Science http://pss.sagepub.com/content/20/10/1214. The online version of this article can .... the Inclusion of Other in Self (IOS) scale (Aron, Aron, &. Smollan, 1992). We used a 7-point version of this .... After describing the film c

Psychological Science
2010 21: 840 originally published online 30 April 2010. Psychological Science .... study was approved by the local university ethics committee. Apparatus and materials ... small target area on a computer screen (Gepshtein, Seydell, &. Trommershäuser

Science Perspectives on Psychological - ENS
Nov 18, 2014 - We present the concept of a community-augmented meta-analysis (CAMA), a simple yet novel tool that significantly facilitates ... and static after publication but can be used and extended by the research community, as anyone can downloa

Clinical Psychological Science
9 Jan 2013 - processing modes rely on different operating principles: the impulsive system, relying on associative ... tive system and that both systems interact (Deutsch & Strack,. 2006). These models have given rise to ... “wanting” usually go

Reflecting on Current Challenges and Future Directions ...
Controversies related to the strengths and limitations of the controlled clinical trials ... Moreover, because the National Institute of Mental Health has been ... allow therapists to master the therapy in an artful manner (B. Arnow, J. Clarkin). ...

Current developments and future directions of bio ... - Semantic Scholar
The com- plexity of an expression is measured by the length of the ...... multiple classifiers is actually a very common practice in machine .... Information Fusion 6,.

Current developments and future directions of bio ... - Semantic Scholar
require close collaboration between computer scientists and ecologists. The rest of .... long as the maximum number of hits (i.e., 50 in this study) is achievable in ...

Current developments and future directions of bio ...
(1)–(3) could be potentially generated by GP. The fitness function determines how well an individual expression fits an observational galaxy profile. The overall.

Social Psychological and Personality Science
Dec 11, 2013 - The online version of this article can be found at: ... of Record. >> at CAMBRIDGE UNIV LIBRARY on December 19, 2013 ..... Data were collected via the ''myPersonality'' application on .... debate about what the appropriate degrees of f

Approaching Psychological Science With Kuhn's Eyes ... - CiteSeerX
As further illustration of this broader issue of concern, for ... making is proposed to impede progress in ... beginning of the science's advancement to unification.