99

Educational Neuroscience, 99–113 BJEP Monograph Series 11, 8 © 2012 The British Psychological Society

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence: The role of structural and functional development in rostral prefrontal cortex Iroise Dumontheil* and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, UK Abstract Background. The rostral prefrontal cortex (RPFC) has increased in size and undergone changes in terms of its cellular organisation during primate evolution. In parallel emerged the ability to detach oneself from the immediate environment and process abstract thoughts and solve problems, as well as the ability to understand other individuals’ thoughts and intentions. The RPFC is thought to play an important role in supporting these abilities: lateral RPFC is involved in processing and focusing on abstract thoughts and medial RPFC is involved in social cognition. Adolescence is a time characterised by change – hormonally, physically, psychologically and socially. Yet until fairly recently this period of life was neglected by cognitive neuroscience. In the past decade, large scale structural MRI studies have demonstrated development during adolescence of white and grey matter brain structure, with more protracted changes in the RPFC than in most other regions. Several fMRI studies have found that activity in the lateral and medial parts of RPFC shows changes between adolescence and adulthood during tasks of abstract thought and social cognition, respectively. Performance on both types of task also continues to improve until late adolescence. These findings highlight how adolescence, and not only childhood, is thus a time of continued maturation of brain and behaviour, when education and the environment can have an impact on cognitive development.

Rostral prefrontal cortex in humans Rostral prefrontal cortex (RPFC), which corresponds approximately to Brodmann area 10 (BA10, Brodmann 1908, 1909), is a large brain region in humans and is thought to be subdivided into separate subregions distinct in terms of cellular organization and function (Christoff & Gabrieli, 2000; Gilbert et al., 2006). Two quite different types of cognitive ability have been associated with the RPFC. The lateral parts of RPFC (rostrolateral prefrontal cortex, RLPFC) appear to support the ability to detach oneself from the environment and to elaborate, evaluate and maintain abstract rules and information, and thus it is involved in reasoning, problem

*Correspondence should be addressed to Professor Iroise Dumontheil, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College, London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK (e-mail: [email protected]). DOI: 10.1348/97818543371712X13219598392372

100 Iroise Dumontheil and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

solving, and more generally abstract thinking (Amati & Shallice, 2006; Christoff & Gabrieli, 2000; Christoff et al., 2009; Gilbert et al., 2007; Koechlin et al., 2003; Ramnani & Owen, 2004). These types of processes have been included in the general “working memory” category in meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies (Cabeza & Nyberg, 2000; Gilbert et al., 2006) (see Figure 1). The medial aspect of RPFC, or medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), is implicated in social cognition, that is, the understanding of other people’s minds (see Figure 1). In the last decade, large scale magnetic resonance (MRI) studies have shown that the RPFC is one of the last brain regions to reach maturity in humans (see Dumontheil et al., 2008, for review). This region is also particularly interesting in terms of its cellular organisation and connection with other regions. RPFC is the only prefrontal region that is predominantly interconnected with supramodal cortex in the PFC (Andersen et al., 1985; Petrides & Pandya, 1999), anterior temporal cortex (Moran et al., 1987; Amaral & Price, 1984) and cingulate cortex (Andersen et al., 1985; Arikuni et al., 1994; Bachevalier et al., 1997; Morecraft & Van Hoesen, 1993). In addition, its projections to these other regions are broadly reciprocal (Passingham et al., 2002; see Ramnani & Owen, 2004, for review). RPFC has a low cell density, which may indicate that this region in humans has more space available for connections both within this region and with other brain regions (Semendeferi et al., 2001). RPFC also has a particularly high number of dendritic spines per cell, an indicator of the number of synaptic connections, which suggests that the computational properties of RPFC are more likely to involve the integration of inputs than those of comparable areas (Ramnani & Owen, 2004). This paper will focus on: how social cognition and the ability to attend to and manipulate abstract thoughts develop during adolescence; how the structural and functional development of the RPFC may underlie the behavioural changes observed during adolescence; and the potential implications of these findings for education policy and practice.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the functional specialisation of rostral prefrontal cortex, shown on a horizontal slice (adapted from Gilbert et al., 2006). Two axes of specialisation have been observed: on the y-axis, mentalising tasks recruit more posterior RPFC than do multitasking tasks (which include task switching and dual tasks); on the x-axis, mentalising activates the medial part of RPFC while episodic and working memory tasks, which include problem solving, abstract thinking and relational reasoning tasks (see Cabesa & Nyberg, 2000; Gilbert et al., 2006) activate lateral RPFC. Multitasking does not exhibit specificity on the x-axis. No hemispheric specificity has been consistently observed.

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence 101

Social cognition in development Humans are an exquisitely social species. We are constantly reading each others’ actions, gestures and faces in terms of underlying mental states and emotions, in an attempt to figure out what other people are thinking and feeling, and what they are about to do next. This is known as theory of mind or mentalising (Frith & Frith, 2007). Developmental psychology research on theory of mind has demonstrated that the ability to understand others’ mental states develops over the first four or five years of life (e.g., Happe et al., 1995). While certain aspects of theory of mind are present in infancy (Baillargeon et al., 2010), it is not until around the age of four years that children begin to explicitly understand that someone else can hold a belief that differs from one’s own, and which can be false (Barresi & Moore, 1996). An understanding of others’ mental states plays a critical role in social interaction because it enables us to work out what other people want and what they are about to do next, and to modify our own behaviour accordingly (Frith & Frith, 2007).

The importance of social interaction in learning There is evidence that social interaction is special and plays a particular role in learning. Kuhl and colleagues (2003) studied American babies who had grown up hearing only English and thus were not able to distinguish between certain Chinese Mandarin sounds. The authors trained three groups of nine-month-old American babies: one group interacted with a real native Chinese speaker, who played with and read to them; a second group saw movies of the same Chinese speaker; the third group heard the same Chinese speaker through headphones. The content and the time of exposure were identical in all three groups. The group that had been exposed to a real live Chinese person significantly improved their ability to distinguish between the two sounds, performing at around the same level as native Chinese babies. In striking contrast, babies who had been exposed to the same amount of Chinese but in the form of video or sound recordings showed no learning and their post-training performance was the same as the American babies who had received no exposure. These results show that social interaction is a critical and constraining factor for learning. There appears to be something special about social interaction with a real live person that is not present from watching videos or hearing sound recordings of the same person. What is special about social interaction with a real person is not yet understood. One possibility is that social interaction increases infants’ motivation through enhanced attention and arousal. Social interaction also directs the adult trainer to focus on the learner’s individual needs and tailors the training content for the learner (see Kuhl, 2007, for review). In addition, by nine months, infants start to understand that pointing to, or looking in the direction of, an object indicates that this object is being referred to. This is one of the first building blocks of theory of mind (see Frith & Frith, 2007, for review), and this understanding is thought to be linked to the development of language, in particularly the acquisition of new vocabulary (see Akhtar & Gernsbacher 2007 for discussion).

The social brain Over the past 15 years, a large number of independent studies have shown remarkable consistency in identifying the brain regions that are involved in theory of mind or mentalising. These studies have employed a wide range of stimuli including stories, sentences, words, cartoons and animations, each designed to elicit the attribution of mental states (see Amodio & Frith, 2006, for review). In each case, the mentalising task resulted in the activation of a network of regions including the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) at the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ),

102 Iroise Dumontheil and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

the temporal poles and the dorsal MPFC (see Burnett & Blakemore, 2009b). The agreement between neuroimaging studies in this area is remarkable and the consistent localisation of activity within a network of regions including the pSTS/TPJ and MPFC, as well as the temporal poles, suggests that these regions are key to the process of mentalising.

Development of mentalising during adolescence There is a rich literature on the development of social cognition in infancy and childhood, pointing to step-wise changes in social cognitive abilities during the first five years of life (Frith & Frith, 2007). However, there has been surprisingly little empirical research on social cognitive development beyond childhood. Only recently have studies focused on development of the social brain beyond early childhood, and these support evidence from social psychology that adolescence represents a period of significant social development. Most researchers in the field use the onset of puberty as the starting point for adolescence. The end of adolescence is harder to define and there are significant cultural variations. However, the end of the teenage years represents a working consensus in Western countries. Adolescence is characterised by psychological changes in terms of identity, self-consciousness and relationships with others (Steinberg, 2010). Compared with children, adolescents are more sociable, form more complex and hierarchical peer relationships and are more sensitive to acceptance and rejection by peers (Steinberg & Morris, 2001). Although the underlying factors of these social changes are most likely to be multi-faceted, one possible cause is development of the social brain. Most developmental studies of social cognition focus on early childhood, possibly because children perform adequately in even quite complex mentalising tasks at around age four (Happé et al., 1995). On the one hand, this could be attributed to a lack of suitable paradigms: in order to create a mentalising task that does not elicit ceiling performance in children aged five and older, the linguistic and executive demands of the task may need to be increased. However, such increases in task complexity render any age-associated improvement in performance difficult to attribute solely to improved mentalising ability. On the other hand, the good performance of 4–5 year-olds could be attributed to qualitative differences in 2nd person perspective abilities, with basic abilities emerging very early, and more complex abilities, potentially associated with executive functioning, gradually improving during childhood and adolescence. The protracted structural (Giedd & Rapoport, 2010; Shaw et al., 2008) and functional (Blakemore, 2008) development of the brain regions involved in theory of mind in adolescence and early adulthood might indeed be expected to affect mental state understanding. In addition, evidence from social psychology studies shows substantial changes in social competence and social behaviour during adolescence (Steinberg, 2010), and this is hypothesised to rely on a more sophisticated manner of thinking about and relating to other people – including understanding their mental states. Recently, we adapted a task that requires the online use of theory of mind information when making decisions in a communication game, and which produces large numbers of errors even in adults (Keysar et al., 2003). In our computerised version of the task, participants view a set of shelves containing objects, which they are instructed to move by a “director,” who can see some but not all of the objects (Dumontheil et al., 2010a). Correct interpretation of the instructions requires participants to use the director’s perspective and only move objects that the director can see (the director condition) (see Figure 2a). We tested participants aged between 7 and 27 years and found that, while performance in the director and a control condition followed the same trajectory (improved accuracy) from mid-childhood until mid-adolescence, the mid-adolescent group made more errors than the adults in the director condition only (see Figure 2b). These results suggest that the ability to take another person’s perspective to direct appropriate behaviour is still undergoing development at this relatively late stage.

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence 103

Figure 2. Social cognition development. (a) Online mentalising task. Participants have to take into account the perspective of another person (the Director) when following his instructions to move objects. The left panel shows the participant’s view of a set of shelves containing objects. The middle panel shows the Director’s view – some objects are occluded from his viewpoint. The right panel shows an example trial. The director cannot see the golf ball, so when he asks the participant to “move the small ball left” the participant should move the tennis ball. (b) Behavioural results. Adolescents aged 14–17 years old make more errors than adults when they are required to take into account the perspective of a speaker (the Director) asking them to move objects in a set of shelves. In contrast, no difference in accuracy is found between these two age groups in a non-social control task (No-Director condition) (adapted from Dumontheil et al., 2010a). (c) A section of the dorsal MPFC that is activated in studies of mentalising: Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) ‘y’ coordinates range from 30 to 60, and ‘z’ coordinates range from 0 to 40. Dots indicate voxels of decreased activity during six mentalising tasks between late childhood and adulthood (see Blakemore, 2008, for references). The mentalising tasks ranged from understanding irony, which requires separating the literal from the intended meaning of a comment, thinking about one’s own intentions, thinking about whether character traits describe oneself or another familiar other, watching animations in which characters appear to have intentions and emotions and thinking about social emotions such as guilt and embarrassment (Burnett et al., 2009b). (Adapted from Blakemore, 2008).

fMRI studies of mentalising during adolescence A number of fMRI studies have investigated the development during adolescence of the functional brain correlates of mentalising (see Blakemore, 2008 for review). These studies have used a wide variety of mentalising tasks – involving the spontaneous attribution of mental states to animated shapes, reflecting on one’s intentions to carry out certain actions, thinking about the preferences and dispositions of oneself or a fictitious story character, and judging the sincerity or sarcasm of another person’s

104 Iroise Dumontheil and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

communicative intentions. Despite the variety of mentalising tasks used, these studies of mental state attribution have consistently shown that MPFC activity during mentalising tasks decreases between adolescence and adulthood. These studies compared brain activity in young adolescents and adults while they were performing a task that involved thinking about mental states. In each of these studies, MPFC activity was greater in the adolescent group than in the adult group during the mentalising task compared to the control task (see Figure 2c and Blakemore, 2008). The mentalising tasks ranged from understanding irony, which requires separating the literal from the intended meaning of a comment (Wang et al., 2006), thinking about one’s own intentions (Blakemore et al., 2007), thinking about whether character traits describe oneself or another familiar other (Pfeifer et al., 2007; 2009), watching animations in which characters appear to have intentions and emotions (Moriguchi et al., 2007) and thinking about social emotions such as guilt and embarrassment (Burnett et al., 2009). In addition, there is evidence for differential functional connectivity between MPFC and other parts of the mentalising network across age (Burnett & Blakemore, 2009a). To summarise, a number of developmental neuroimaging studies of social cognition show striking consistency with respect to the direction of change in MPFC activity. It is not yet understood why MPFC activity decreases between adolescence and adulthood during mentalising tasks, but two non-mutually exclusive explanations have been put forward (see Blakemore, 2008, for details). One possibility is that the cognitive strategy for mentalising changes between adolescence and adulthood. For example, adults may rely more on previous experiences to interpret social situations than adolescents, who instead might base their judgement on novel computations performed in the MPFC. This possibility may be related to the skill learning hypothesis (Johnson, 2011), whereby one region first supports a certain function, but another brain region may take over later in development, and according to which the PFC may be particularly involved during the learning of new abilities. A second possibility is that the functional change with age is due to neuroanatomical changes that occur during this period. Decreases in activity are frequently interpreted as being due to developmental reductions in grey matter volume, presumably related to synaptic pruning. However, there is currently no direct way to test the relationship between number of synapses, synaptic activity and neural activity as measured by fMRI in humans (see Blakemore, 2008, for discussion, and Questions for future research section below).

Development of abstract thinking Abstract thinking can be considered as the manipulation of self-generated thoughts, or thoughts that are not directly connected to the environment. RLPFC is thought to be specifically involved in the elaboration, evaluation and maintenance of abstract rules and information (Amati & Shallice, 2007; Christoff & Gabrieli, 2000; Christoff et al., 2009; Koechlin et al., 2003; Ramnani & Owen, 2004). The ability to resist distraction from one’s environment is critical for abstract thinking. Recent work suggests that RLPFC also supports the ability to flexibly control whether one attends to the environment or to self-generated thoughts (Burgess et al., 2007). Two experimental paradigms (the Shapes task and the Alphabet task) were used to assess the development of abstract thinking, and the selection of abstract thoughts vs. environmentally-derived information, during adolescence. In the following section, we report the results of a large behavioural study of these tasks and then the results of neuroimaging studies.

Relational reasoning development during adolescence The elaboration and manipulation of abstract rules has been investigated using tasks that require relational reasoning. The relational reasoning demands of a problem can be defined

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence 105

in terms of the number of dimensions, or sources of variation, that need to be considered simultaneously to reach a correct solution. Children under 5 years can solve 0- and 1-relational problems, but fail to solve 2-relational problems (Halford et al., 1998). Early improvements in relational reasoning may ref lect a shift from a focus on object similarity to relational similarity (Rattermann & Gentner, 1998). Further improvements during childhood and adolescence may relate to increased relational knowledge or increased working memory capacity (Crone et al., 2009; Sternberg & Rif kin, 1979: see Richland et al., 2006, for discussion). We recently employed the Shapes task, a relational reasoning task (Christoff et al., 2003; Bunge et al., 2009), which reliably activates RLPFC at the single subject level in adults (Smith et al., 2007), to investigate relational reasoning development during adolescence in a large sample of healthy participants (Dumontheil et al., 2010c). The Shapes task required participants to assess whether two pairs of items, which could vary in shape and/or texture, differed or changed along the same dimension. The pairs of items could both show texture differences or both show shape differences, in which case participants were asked to respond yes – i.e., the pairs change along the same dimension (match). Alternatively, one pair of items differed in texture while the other pair differed in shape, in which case participants were asked to respond no – i.e., the pairs change along different dimensions (no-match) (see Figure 3a). One hundred and seventy-nine female participants aged 7 to 27 years old participated in the study. The results show a non-linear pattern of improvement in accuracy in the Shapes task across age. After an early improvement in accuracy, with 9–11 year olds performing at adult levels, performance dips in the 11–14 year olds and gradually improves again to adult levels throughout late adolescence (see Figure 3b).

Development of the flexible selection of self-generated thoughts An important aspect of the manipulation of abstract thought resides in the ability to modulate the balance between cognition that is provoked by perceptual experience (stimulus-oriented, SO) and that which occurs in the absence of sensory input (self-generated, or stimulus-independent, SI) (Burgess et al., 2007). In the same large sample of participants described above, we used a single task (“Alphabet” task) that could be performed on the basis of either SO or SI information, without high working memory requirements (Gilbert et al., 2005, 2007, 2008). Participants were asked to classify letters of the alphabet according to whether the upper case letter contained a curve or not. In SO blocks consecutive letters of the alphabet were presented on the screen, while in SI blocks either no letter (No-distractor condition) or distracting non-consecutive letters (Distractor condition) were presented on the screen. In SI blocks the participant were asked to continue going through the alphabet sequence in their head and continue responding (see Figure 4a). Different patterns of development were observed for the different aspects of this task. Resistance to visual distractors exhibited small improvements with age, both in accuracy and speed of responding, while the manipulation of SI thoughts and switching between SI and SO thoughts showed steeper speed of responding improvements extending into late adolescence (see Figure 4b). Thus behavioural changes in performance, in terms of either accuracy or reaction times, were observed in both the Shapes and Alphabet tasks, but the developmental trajectories were taskand condition-specific. In a second set of studies, using the same tasks, we collected functional and structural MRI data to investigate the developmental changes in brain function associated with abstract thought during adolescence, and how these changes in brain function relate to developmental changes in brain structure.

106 Iroise Dumontheil and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

Figure 3. Relational reasoning development. (a) Shapes task. In the control condition, participants are asked whether the shape at the bottom is present at the top (here, on the left, the answer is yes); in the relational reasoning condition, participants indicate whether the top and bottom pairs of items change along the same dimension (here, on the right, the top pair changes texture while the bottom pair changes shape, so the answer is no). (b) Behavioural results. Adolescents aged 11–17 years old make more errors than adults and younger adolescents (9–11 years old) during a 2-level relational reasoning task (adapted from Dumontheil et al., 2010c). (c) fMRI results. The main effect of relational reasoning vs. the control condition over the whole age range is presented (family-wise error (FWE) corrected threshold, p<0.05), highlighting activity in the left posterior RLPFC (top).RLPFC activity in this contrast is plotted in the three age groups (bottom). The decrease in activation between mid-adolescent and adult groups was significant, while the increase in activation between early and mid-adolescent groups was marginally significant. Both effects were accounted for by performance and grey and white matter differences between the groups (see Dumontheil et al., 2010c).

Changes in RLPFC structure and activity during adolescence There is evidence that the developmental time course of changes in RLPFC structure can have an impact on cognitive performance. Indeed, in a large MRI study of development, Shaw and colleagues (2006) stratified their participants on the basis of Weschler intelligence scales, which give a standardized intelligence quotient (IQ) based on subtests assessing verbal and non-verbal knowledge and reasoning. Shaw et al. (2006) demonstrated that, in the absence of large differences in brain structure in childhood or in adulthood, different developmental trajectories of grey matter structure in groups of average and superior intelligence led to striking differences in cortical thickness in the superior frontal cortex and RLPFC specifically during adolescence.

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence 107

Figure 4. Development of the flexible switching between selecting thoughts derived from the environment and abstract thoughts. (a) Alphabet task. Participants classify letters of the alphabet according to their shape (line or curve). When the letter is red, participants judge the letter presented on the screen (stimulus-oriented/SO blocks). When the letter is blue (or when there is no letter) participants continue reciting the alphabet in their head and judge the shape of the letter in their head (stimulus-independent (SI) blocks), while ignoring the distracting letter presented on the screen (Distractor condition), or in the absence of a letter on the screen (No-Distractor condition). Performance in the two types of blocks (SI vs. SO) and the two conditions (Distractor vs. No-Distractor), and performance in switch trials (first trial of a SO or SI block) and subsequent trials (stay trials) were compared. (b) Behavioural results. The speed of responding in SI vs. SO, and in switch vs. stay trials continued to increase during adolescence. The speed of responding in the presence of Distractors also improved but followed a flatter linear developmental function (adapted from Dumontheil et al., 2010b). (c) fMRI results. The main effect of switching between SO and SI conditions vs. a simple change of colour of the stimuli over the whole age range is presented (FWE, p < 0.05), highlighting the right superior RLPFC activation (top). RLPFC activity in this contrast is plotted against age (bottom). There was a significant decrease in activity during adolescence, which was not purely a consequence of differences in performance and brain structure between the participants and could reflect the maturation of neurocognitive strategies (see Dumontheil et al., 2010b).

In the past decade, functional MRI studies of lateral PFC development have observed a less consistent pattern of change during adolescence than the MPFC studies, with reports of both increases and decreases of task-related activations (see Luna et al., 2010 for review). As in the MPFC literature, decreases are frequently interpreted as being due to developmental reductions in grey matter volume, presumably related to synaptic pruning. Increases are thought to relate

108 Iroise Dumontheil and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

to improved and more localized task-specific processing, potentially facilitated by faster longrange connections due to increased axonal myelination and size (Luna et al., 2010). Understanding the link between structural and functional changes is critical in understanding the mechanisms of neurocognitive development, yet very few studies have directly compared structural and functional data within the same individuals (Olesen et al., 2003; Lu et al., 2009). We attempted to address this issue of the relationship between brain structure and function, with the RLPFC as a particular region of interest, and tested variants of the two paradigms described above using fMRI, comparing participants aged 11–18 years with adults. One previous study had provided evidence of changes in RLPFC activation between childhood (age 8–12 years) and adulthood using a relational reasoning Raven’s matrices task (Crone et al., 2009). fMRI and MRI data were collected in a group of 37 participants aged 11 to 30 years old (Dumontheil et al., 2010b, 2010c). Analysis of the Shapes task revealed that, although a consistent set of RLPFC and parietal cortex regions were activated across age groups, the fMRI results revealed a complex picture of linear and non-linear changes with age. In the posterior left RLPFC, there was an increase in activity between early and mid-adolescence, followed by a decrease in activity between mid-adolescence and adulthood (see Figure 3c). In terms of structure, grey matter volumes in this region significantly decreased between age 14–18 and adulthood, while no changes in white matter volumes were observed. Importantly, when combining functional and structural (as well as behavioural) data, we observed that the differences in performance and brain structure between the age groups accounted for most of these functional developmental changes in RLPFC activity. Analysis of the Alphabet task revealed a different pattern of developmental changes in the RLPFC. The right superior RLPFC exhibited a decrease in activity with age when participants switched between SO and SI thoughts (Figure 4c). Here again analyses of brain structure showed that grey matter volumes in this region decreased with age. However, in this case, the functional changes observed during adolescence were not purely consequences of structural maturation or performance changes with age. These results thus offer a complex picture of the relationship between structural and functional brain maturation and developmental changes in behaviour and show that changes observed, and their interplay, will be dependent on the particular paradigm used.

Questions for future research Our neuroimaging findings combining structural and functional MRI data suggest that it is unlikely that functional changes reflect a single developmental process, but rather a multitude of processes such as local changes in grey and white matter structure, the maturation of complex balancing brain networks (e.g., the subcortical emotional processing and top-down prefrontal regulation systems (Hare et al., 2008)) as well as the establishment of functional synchronization across networks of brain regions (Uhlhaas et al., 2009) and changes in the cognitive strategy applied to perform the tasks. Our results reinforce the importance of combining a variety of measures when studying cognitive development; however, further work will be needed to better identify the contribution of these different maturational processes to functional changes in the PFC. In the Shapes task, both behavioural and fMRI data showed non-linear patterns of changes with age. Non-linear developmental changes have been reported previously during adolescence, in a variety of cognitive tasks including face processing and match-to-sample tasks (e.g., Carey et al., 1980; Diamond et al., 1983; McGivern et al., 2002). Typically, a dip in performance is observed around the start of puberty (age 11–12 years old) and its timing can differ

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence 109

between genders (McGivern et al., 2002). This coincides with the “educational dip” that is often observed in terms of educational attainment (West et al., 2010; Whitby et al., 2006) Further work will be needed to better understand how dips in performance during adolescence relate to puberty and biological factors such as brain structure and neurotransmitter systems, or social and environmental factors such as motivation and a move to a new school (West et al., 2010; Whitby et al., 2006).

Future possibilities for education policy and practice There are many questions that remain to be investigated in this new and rapidly expanding field. The study of neural development during adolescence is likely to have important implications for society in relation to education and the legal treatment of teenagers, as well as a variety of mental illnesses that often have their onset in adolescence. Knowledge of how the brain develops and learns has the potential to have a profound impact on education in the future. Understanding the brain mechanisms that underlie learning and memory, and the effects of genetics, the environment, emotion and age on learning could transform educational strategies and enable us to design programs that optimise learning for people of all ages and of all needs. Neuroscience can now offer some understanding of how the brain learns new information and processes this information throughout life (see Blakemore & Frith, 2005; Royal Society Brain Waves Report, 2011). As described above, social interaction with a real live person is critical for at least some types of early learning (Kuhl et al., 2003), suggesting that, while not necessarily harmful, DVDs and CDs aimed at teaching babies and young children may not be associated with optimal learning. More importantly, the time spent watching DVDs is time that could otherwise be spent in social interaction with a real person, and denying the developing brain of this might have negative consequences. We need to ask whether online social networking, which is particularly popular with teenagers, is the same as real live interaction, or whether it might be denying the developing teenage brain important real life interactions? There is as yet no research on this important question. There is a growing industry for the development of robot nannies, robot carers and robot companions for the elderly in ageing societies such as Japan. But are robot companions the same as real friends? Does social interaction with robots determine happiness in the same way as social relationships with people (Argyle, 2001)? Can distance learning via a computer ever compare with learning from a real human teacher? These are open questions, ripe for research. Understanding the brain basis of social functioning and social development may improve the fostering of social competence inside and outside the classroom, in support of previous social psychological research in this domain (e.g., Durlak et al. in press). Social functioning plays a role in shaping learning and academic performance (and vice versa), and understanding the neural basis of social behaviour may contribute to understanding the origins and process of schooling success and failure. The finding that changes in brain structure continue into adolescence (and beyond) has challenged accepted views, and has given rise to a recent spate of investigations into the way cognition (including social cognition) might change as a consequence. Research suggests that adolescence is a key time for the development of regions of the brain involved in social cognition and self-awareness, as well as in problem solving and abstract thinking. This is likely to be due to the interplay between a number of factors, including changes in the social environment and in puberty hormones, as well as structural and functional brain development and improvements in social cognition and reasoning abilities. If early childhood is seen as a major opportunity – or a “sensitive period” – for teaching, so too should the teenage years. During both periods, significant brain reorganisation is taking place. The idea that teenagers should still go to school and be educated is relatively new. And yet

110 Iroise Dumontheil and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

the brain is still developing during this period, is adaptable, and can be moulded and shaped. Perhaps the aims of education for adolescents might usefully include a focus on abilities that are controlled by the parts of the brain that undergo most change during adolescence, including those described in this review: social cognition and the understanding and awareness of the potentially different perspective of others, abstract thinking and reasoning, and the ability to focus on one’s own thoughts in spite of environmental distraction. Finally, it might be fruitful to include in the curriculum some teaching on the changes occurring in the brain during adolescence. Adolescents might be interested in, and could benefit from, learning about the changes that are going on in their own brains.

Acknowledgements Sarah-Jayne Blakemore is supported by a Royal Society University Research Fellowship. Iroise Dumontheil was supported by a Marie Curie Research Fellowship.

References Akhtar N, Gernsbacher MA. (2007) Joint attention and vocabulary development: A critical look. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1, 195–207. Amaral, D. J., & Price, J. L. (1984). Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 230, 465–496. Amati, D., & Shallice, T. (2007) On the emergence of modern humans. Cognition, 103, 358–385. Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 268–277. Andersen, R. A., Asanuma, C., & Cowan, W. M. (1985). Callosal and prefrontal associational projecting cell populations in area 7A of the macaque monkey: A study using retrogradely transported fluorescent dyes. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 232, 443-455. Argyle, M. (2001). The Psychology of Happiness. (2nd Ed.). Routledge. Arikuni, T., Sako, H., & Murata, A. (1994). Ipsilateral connections of the anterior cingulate cortex with the frontal and medial temporal cortices in the macaque monkey. Neuroscience Research, 21, 19–39. Bachevalier, J., Meunier, M., Lu, M. X., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1997). Thalamic and temporal cortex input to medial prefrontal cortex in rhesus monkeys. Experimental Brain Research, 115, 430–444. Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110–118. Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107–154 Blakemore, S. J., & Frith, U. (2005). The Learning Brain: Lessons for Education. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Blakemore S. J., den Ouden, H., Choudhury, S., & Frith, C. (2007). Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 130–139. Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 267–277. Brodmann K. (1908) Beitraege zur histologischen Lokalisation der Grosshirnrinde. VI. Mitteilung: die Cortex-gliederung des Menschen. Journal fur Psychologie und Neurologie, 10, 231–246. Brodmann K. (1909) Vergleichende Localisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren prinzipien Dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig, Germany: Barth. Bunge, S. A., Helskog, E. H., & Wendelken, C. (2009). Left, but not right, rostrolateral prefrontal cortex meets a stringent test of the relational integration hypothesis. Neuroimage, 46, 338–342. Burnett, S., & Blakemore, S. J. (2009a). Functional connectivity during a social emotion task in adolescents and in adults. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1294–1301. Burnett, S., & Blakemore, S. J. (2009b). The development of adolescent social cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167, 51–56.

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence 111 Burnett, S., Bird, G., Moll, J., Frith, C., Blakemore, S. J. (2009). Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1736–1750. Burgess, P. W., Dumontheil, I., & Gilbert, S. J. (2007). The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 290–298. Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. (2000) Neural bases of learning and memory: Functional neuroimaging evidence. Current Opinion in Neurology, 13, 415-421. Carey, S., Diamond, R., & Woods, R. (1980). Development of face recognition – A maturational component? Developmental Psychology, 16, 257–269. Christoff, K., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology, 28, 168–186. Christoff, K., Keramatian, K., Gordon, A. M., Smith, R., & Mädler, B. (2009). Prefrontal organization of cognitive control according to levels of abstraction. Brain Research, 1286, 94–105. Christoff, K., Ream, J. M., Geddes, L. P., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2003). Evaluating self-generated information: Anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 1161–1168. Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., van Leijenhorst, L, Honomichl, R. D., Christoff, K., & Bunge, S. A. (2009). Neurocognitive development of relational reasoning. Developmental Science, 12, 55–66. Diamond, R., Carey, S., & Back, K. J. (1983). Genetic influences on the development of spatial skills during early adolescence. Cognition, 13, 167–185. Dumontheil, I., Burgess, P. W., & Blakemore, S. J. (2008). Development of rostral prefrontal cortex and cognitive and behavioural disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 50, 168–181. Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S.J. (2010a). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13, 331–338. Dumontheil, I., Hassan, B., Gilbert, S., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010b) Development of the selection and manipulation of self-generated thoughts in adolescence. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 7664-7671. Dumontheil, I., Houlton, R., Christoff, K., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010c). Development of relational reasoning during adolescence. Developmental Science, 13, F15–24. Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., Schellinger, K. B. The Impact of Enhancing Students’ Social and Emotional Learning: A Meta-analysis of School-based Universal Interventions. Child Development (in press). Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. (2007). Social cognition in humans. Current Biology, 17, 724–732. Giedd, J.N. & Rapoport, J.L (2010). Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron, 67, 728–734. Gilbert, S. J., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2008). Atypical recruitment of medial prefrontal cortex in autism spectrum disorders: An fMRI study of two executive function tasks. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2281–2291. Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2005). Involvement of rostral prefrontal cortex in selection between stimulus-oriented and stimulus-independent thought. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 1423–1431. Gilbert, S. J., Spengler, S., Simons, J. S., Steele, J. D., Lawrie, S. M., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006). Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): A meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 932–948. Gilbert, S. J., Williamson, I. D., Dumontheil, I., Simons, J. S., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2007). Distinct regions of medial rostral prefrontal cortex supporting social and nonsocial functions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 217–226. Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Philips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–831. Happé, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855. Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Voss, H. U., Glover, G. H., & Casey, B. J. (2008). Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 927–934.

112 Iroise Dumontheil and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore Johnson, M.H. (2011). Interactive Specialization: A domain-general framework for human functional brain development? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 7–21. Keysar, B., Lin, S., & Barr, D. J. (2003). Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition, 89, 25–41. Koechlin, E., Ody, C., & Kouneiher, F. (2003). The architecture of cognitive control in human prefrontal cortex. Science, 302, 1181–1185. Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 9096–9101. Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110–120. Lu, L. H., Dapretto, M., O’Hare, E. D., Kan, E., McCourt, S. T., Thompson, P. M., Toga, A. W., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Sowell, E. R. (2009). Relationships between brain activation and brain structure in normally developing children. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2595–2604. Luna, B., Padmanabhan, A., & O’Hearn, K. (2010). What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence? Brain and Cognition, 72, 101–113. McGivern, R.F., Andersen, J., Byrd, D., Mutter, K. L., & Reilly, J. (2002). Cognitive efficiency on a match to sample task decreases at the onset of puberty in children. Brain and Cognition, 50, 73–89. Moran, M. A., Mufson, E. J., & Mesulam, M. M. (1987). Neural inputs into the temporopolar cortex of the rhesus monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 256, 88–103. Morecraft, R.J., & Van Hoesen, G.W. (1993). Frontal granular cortex input to the cingulate (M3), supplementary (M2) and primary (M1) motor cortices in the rhesus monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 337, 669–689. Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Mori, T., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. (2007). Changes of brain activity in the neural substrates for theory of mind during childhood and adolescence. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61, 355–363. Olesen, P. J., Nagy, Z., Westerberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2003). Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a fronto-parietal network. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 48–57. Passingham, R. E., Stephan, K. E., & Kotter, R. (2002). The anatomical basis of functional localization in the cortex. Nature Review Neuroscience, 3, 606–616. Petrides, M., & Pandya, D. N. (1999). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. European Journal Neuroscience, 11, 1011–1036. Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., Borofsky, L. A., Dapretto, M., Fuligni, A. J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2009). Neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescents and adults: When social perspective-taking informs self-perception. Child Development, 80, 1016–1038. Pfeifer, J. H., Lieberman, M. D., & Dapretto, M. (2007). “I know you are but what am I?!”: Neural bases of self and social knowledge retrieval in children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1323–1337. Ramnani, M., & Owen, A. M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 184–194. Rattermann, M. J. & Gentner, D. (1998). More evidence for a relational shift in the development of analogy: Children’s performance on a causal-mapping task. Cognitive Development, 13, 453–478. Richland, L. E., Morrison, R. G., & Holyoak, K. J. (2006). Children’s development of analogical reasoning: Insights from scene analogy problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 249–273. Royal Society Brain Waves Report (2011) Brain Waves Module 2: Neuroscience: implications for education and lifelong learning, The Royal Society, London, UK. Retrieved 25 October 2011 from www.royalsociety.org/uploadedFiles/Royal_Society/Policy_and_Influence/Module_2_Neuroscience_Education_ Full_Report_Appendices.pdf Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., & Van Hoesen, G.W. (2001). Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 114, 224–241.

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence 113 Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., Clasen, L., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., Evans, A., Rapoport, J., & Giedd, J. (2006). Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature, 440, 676–679. Shaw, P., Kabani ,N.J., Lerch, J.P., Eckstrand, K., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., Greenstein, D., Clasen, L., Evans, A., Rapoport, J.L., Giedd, J.N., & Wise, S.P. (2008) Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 3586–3594. Smith, R., Keramatian, K., & Christoff, K. (2007). Localizing the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex at the individual level. Neuroimage, 36, 1387–1396. Steinberg, L. (2010). Adolescence (9th Ed.) New York: McGraw-Hill Higher Education. Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual Review in Psychology, 52, 83–110. Sternberg, R. J., & Rifkin, B. (1979). The development of analogical reasoning processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 195–232. Uhlhaas, P. J., Roux, F., Singer, W., Haenschel, C., Sireteanu, R., & Rodriguez, E. (2009). The development of neural synchrony reflects late maturation and restructuring of functional networks in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 9866–9871. Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 107–121. West, P., Sweeting, H., & Young, R. (2010). Transition matters: Pupils’ experiences of the primary-secondary school transition in the West of Scotland and consequences for well-being and attainment. Research Papers in Education, 25, 21–50. Whitby, K., Lord, P., O’Donnell, S., & Grayson, H. (2006). Thematic Probe. Dips in performance and motivation: A purely English perception? INCA: The International Review of Curriculum and Assessment Frameworks Internet Archive. London: Qualifications and Curriculum Authority (QCA) . Available from inca.org.uk/pdf/Final_Dip_Report.pdf

Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence

ere is evidence that the developmental time course of changes in RLPFC structure can .... relationships with people (Argyle, 2001)? Can distance learning via a ...

608KB Sizes 0 Downloads 276 Views

Recommend Documents

Social cognition in humans and robots - socSMCs
Collectives”. 16:00 - 16:30 Coffee break. 16:30 - 18:00 Contributed talks. 19:00. Social dinner at MS Cap San Diego. (en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cap_San_Diego).

The social brain in adolescence - Nature
College London, ... Abstract | The term 'social brain' refers to the network of brain regions that are .... plex network of regions is involved in the recognition.

Implicit and Explicit Processes in Social Cognition
In this review we consider research on social cognition in which implicit processes can be compared and contrasted with explicit, conscious processes. In each case, their function is distinct, sometimes comple- mentary and sometimes oppositional. We

Implicit and Explicit Processes in Social Cognition
that was threatened by a poverty of good experimental data. Another crucial field ... People respond automatically to the presentation of a fearful face with a fear ...

The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from ...
2. Functional neuroimaging and behavioural studies of social cognition in adolescence. .... ter traits, and finally using this knowledge to guide interactions. (Frith and Frith, 2008, ..... using a modified Ultimatum Game showed that the tendency to

Frames of reference in social cognition
Jul 30, 2007 - I call this assumption the single-perspective view. We can contrast the .... what I call interaction oriented. One uses .... the chairman of a conference is pointing at you, you .... would forecast for the earning of a company. Despite

Social cognitive development during adolescence - Oxford Journals
However, the development of social cognition during adolescence and its neural underpinnings remains .... context of the Social Information Processing Network.

Social cognition in the we-mode.pdf
... standing of the behaviour of their partners, and thus of. options available for action, by representing aspects of the. interactive scene in the we-mode (Box 1).

Social cognition on the Internet - testing constraints on social ...
as such: it is about social complexity and the limits. placed on ... Page 3 of 10. Social cognition on the Internet - testing constraints on social network size.pdf.

Social cognition, artefacts, and stigmergy revisited - Concepts of ...
Social cognition, artefacts, and stigmergy revisited - Concepts of coordination.pdf. Social cognition, artefacts, and stigmergy revisited - Concepts of coordination.

36-Emotions and social cognition-8-Matallana.pdf
36-Emotions and social cognition-8-Matallana.pdf. 36-Emotions and social cognition-8-Matallana.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu.

Distributed Cognition - Where the Cognitive and the Social Merge.pdf ...
]Distributed Cognition - Where the Cognitive and the Social Merge.pdf. ]Distributed Cognition - Where the Cognitive and the Social Merge.pdf. Open. Extract.

31-Emotions and social cognition-3-D'Angelo.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. 31-Emotions ...

Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition
http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/364/1528/2359.full.html#related-urls ... Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the ...

Mechanisms of Social Cognition
Aug 11, 2011 - are automatic and implicit and enable learning by observation. Learning from others ..... when they see an adult interacting with a novel object.

Cognition in the lab and cognition in the wild 00 p. 1 ...
Georgia Institute of Technology. Mike Martin. University of ... comparing subjects of roughly the same age range X young adulthood to (late) middle age. .... tend to benefit more for schema0consistent information, Shi et al., 2012, this issue) 00 or

Social Influence on Risk Perception During Adolescence - Sites
phone call while driving or not wear a seatbelt for a short drive? People are constantly ..... We found a significant three-way interaction of social-influence group ...