ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2000, 60, 181–185 doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1445, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

The importance of hatching date for dominance in young shags ALBERTO VELANDO

Departamento de Ecoloxi´a e Bioloxi´a Animal, Universidade de Vigo (Received 10 August 1999; initial acceptance 27 September 1999; final acceptance 24 March 2000; MS. number: 6321)

In birds, conditions and experience in the nest may influence the social rank of juveniles after they become independent. Three main factors may be involved: (1) mass at fledging, larger birds being better able to compete; (2) brood hierarchy, dominance in the brood continuing after the birds leave the nest; and (3) hatching date, birds hatching earlier having more experience and a greater ability to compete. I studied the effect of these factors on the agonistic behaviour of juvenile European shags, Phalacrocorax aristotelis, in cre`ches. Of all the factors, only hatching date had a significant influence on the dominance rank of juveniles. Birds that hatched earlier had a higher rank. Additionally, males were somewhat more aggressive than females. The importance of an individual’s hatching date for its social rank may have consequences for its future reproductive value. 

(1) Mass at fledging. The relative mass of juveniles at fledging can determine the direction of dominance. In stable groups, the largest birds are usually dominant (e.g. Ketterson 1979; Richner et al. 1989). In great tits, Parus major, for example, the weight of the fledgling has the largest long-term effect on its social rank (Garnett 1981). (2) Brood hierarchy. A size hierarchy within a brood caused by asynchronous hatching may determine the dominance rank of individuals during independence (Boag & Alway 1980). In the blue-footed booby, Sula nebouxii, early social experience also influences the brood hierarchy, the older chick dominating its sibling: in an experiment in which unrelated chicks were paired, senior chicks dominated junior ones, even though the latter were, on average, 32% heavier (Drummond & Osorno 1992; see also Drummond & Canales 1998). European shag chicks hatch over a 2-day interval, which leads to a hierarchy determined by size at a very early age (Stockland & Amundsen 1988). This hierarchy could affect agonistic behaviour during independence. (3) Hatching date. Hatching date, and consequently early social experience in the group, can influence the social rank of juveniles. In the genus Parus, for example, the first fledglings that settle in an area dominate birds that fledge later (review in Matthysen 1990). This suggests that juveniles need social experience to develop competitive skills. Individuals must compete for dominance and they learn from their first encounters with opponents (Huntingford & Turner 1987; Clutton-Brock &

Agonistic behaviour, which is common in species living in groups, results in individuals forming a social hierarchy (Huntingford & Turner 1987). Thus, dominant animals are better able to compete and take advantage of resources (e.g. Ekman & Askenmo 1984; Arcese & Smith 1985; Westman 1990). In many birds, the period of independence after fledging leads to their introduction into a social group and the determination of a new hierarchy among the juveniles (Garnett 1981). In this way, factors existing prior to fledging may affect social rank during independence (Boag & Alway 1980). In many birds, after fledging, juveniles gather on perches that serve as cre`ches (e.g. Davis 1982; Evans 1984; Carter & Hobson 1988), where parental care is continued (Burger 1980). In the European shag, Phalacrocorax aristotelis, the cre`ches are stable social units: only 8% of juveniles move away from the perches where they normally reside (unpublished data). In the cre`ches, fledglings are often aggressive, especially while being fed by the adults (Snow 1963). Since cre`ches are stable social units, an individual’s dominance rank may affect its ability to compete for resources. Hatching date, brood hierarchy and size of chicks at fledging may influence the social hierarchy and their recruitment into the population (Watts & Stokes 1971; Boag & Alway 1980; Spear & Nur 1994). I tested the following three hypotheses in juvenile European shags. Correspondence: A. Velando, Departamento de Ecoloxi´a e Bioloxi´a Animal, Facultade de Ciencias, Campus Lagoas-Marcosende, Universidade de Vigo, 36200 Vigo, Spain (email: [email protected]). 0003–3472/00/080181+05 $35.00/0

2000 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour

181



2000 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour

182

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 60, 2

Parker 1995). These hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, however, and some species of birds show positive correlations between mass at fledging, hatching date and brood rank (O’Connor 1984). METHODS

Study Area and Nestling Data I studied shags on the Islas Ci´es (Ri´a de Vigo, Galicia, northwest Spain) between March and June 1996. The Islas Ci´es holds a nesting population of ca. 1000 pairs of shags, which nest in hollows underneath rocks (Velando et al. 1999). During the hatching period, I visited 56 nests daily. I recorded the order, date and time of hatching of each chick in the brood either directly or estimating them from the prehatching stages of each egg (cracked, surface broken, distinct hole cut by chick; see Stokland & Amundsen 1988). Brood size was two or, usually, three chicks. I marked the bill of the chicks with indelible colour markers while they were breaking the shell or immediately after hatching. Each chick was differentiated in the nest by the coloured marks. When the chicks were 5 days old, I also tagged each one with a strip of coloured velcro across the tarsus. These strips were adapted to tarsus size during growth. At 15 days old, the chicks were tagged with a plastic ring with an individual two-digit combination to facilitate identification from a distance. When the chicks were 50 days old, I weighed them with a spring balance to the nearest 10 g. I measured the culmen, head, tarsus and wing of each chick about seven times during the first 15 days of life and subsequently every 4 days until they first flew. These morphometric measurements allowed us to sex the chicks by means of a discriminant analysis (A. Velando, J. Graves & J. Freire, unpublished data).

Observations and Data Analysis After fledging, the juveniles gathered on rocks near the sea beneath the colonies, where they formed cre`ches. I observed a single cre`che, located over a flat rock above sea level in the Isla do Faro, continuously in 1-h blocks. I carried out 70 h of observations from 28 June to 17 July (all the chicks studied fledged between 1 and 25 June). I studied the agonistic behaviour of 37 tagged juveniles. The following categories were recorded: give peck, receive peck, vertical threat gape and horizontal threat gape. In a horizontal threat gape the chick extended its neck horizontally towards another bird that was not close by, with the bill open or closed (Snow 1963; van Tets 1965). I converted behavioural categories into frequencies by dividing the number of actions of each behaviour by the number of hours that the bird was in the cre`che. All statistical tests were two tailed. Data are expressed as meansSE.

Ethical Note In the Islas Ci´es, the main source of variability in breeding performance of shags was due to one or more

eggs failing to hatch in 38% of the nests. Thus, to avoid disturbing the birds, I studied colonies that had not been visited during the incubation period. In these colonies, the main cause of yearly chick mortality was the adverse weather (Velando et al. 1999) and for this reason I did not visit them on rainy and windy days. During the hatching period, the colonies were visited daily. During this period each visit lasted less than 1 h to avoid the chicks cooling when they were dependent on parental warmth (Østnes & Bech 1997). In Islas Ci´es, there was no predation on eggs or chicks in 6 years of study. During the study there were no desertions and reproductive success in the colonies was similar to that in the nonvisited control areas (visited areas: 1.83 chicks/nest; controls: 1.79; Mann– Whitney test: Z=0.06, N1 =56, N2 =26, P=0.95). When I measured a brood, I removed and returned all the chicks at the same time. The shags fledged at 55–62 days and my visits did not appear to cause premature fledging (in the U.K. the chicks fledge at 48–58 days, Snow 1960). The study was carried out in Parque Natural Islas Ci´es, with permission from the Direccio´ n General de Conservacio´ n de la Naturaleza (Ministerio de Medio Ambiente) and Servicio de Medio Ambiente Natural (Xunta de Galicia). RESULTS

Weight at Fledging I carried out a GLM analysis to identify the effect of hatching date, hatching order and sex on weight of chicks at fledging. Before leaving the nest, male chicks weighed significantly more than females (males: 1722 14 g, N=51; females: 147316 g, N=47; F1,88 =10.51, P=0.002). Chick weight decreased with hatching date (F1,88 =4.99, P=0.028), but within sexes this was significant only for females (Fig. 1). The hatching order had no effect on the weight of fledglings (F2,88 =0.37, P=0.69; Fig. 1), and all interaction terms were nonsignificant (sex*hatching date: F1,88 =0.51, P=0.48; sex*hatching order: F2,88 =2.31, P=0.10; hatching date*hatching order: F2,88 =0.89, P=0.41).

Dominance Rank of Juveniles To determine the dominance hierarchy, I carried out a principal component analysis on the correlation matrix of frequencies of the aggressive categories recorded. The first principal component (PC1) accounted for 56% of the variance, with an eigenvalue of 2.23 and Pearson correlation coefficients of 0.868 (P<0.001), 0.816 (P<0.001), 0.732 (P<0.001) and 0.522 (P<0.01) for frequency of give peck, vertical threat gape, horizontal threat gape and receive peck, respectively (df=35 in all cases). The resulting PC1 was a synoptic descriptor of dominance rank of juveniles in the cre`che. A GLM analysis showed that males had a slightly and significantly higher dominance rank than females (F1,26 =6.10, P=0.021); however, neither hatching order nor weight of juveniles had a significant effect on the dominance rank (PC1) in the cre`che (hatching order:

VELANDO: DOMINANCE IN YOUNG SHAGS

2000

3

(a)

(a)

2

1800

1 1600

0

Fledging weight (g)

1400

–1

1200 0 1800

10 20 Hatching date

–2 1200

30

3 (b)

1800

(b)

22 14

Dominance rank (PC1)

15

1700

1600 12 1500

21

14

1400

1400 1600 Fledging weight (g)

2 4

7

1

5

7

5

9

0

–1

1

2 Hatching order

3

Figure 1. Relationship between fledging weight (measured at 50 days old) of males (x) and females (C) and (a) hatching date (day 1=1 April; linear trend of both sexes is shown: males: R2 =0.02, N=51, NS; females: R2 =0.13, N=47, P<0.05) and (b) the hatching order (X±SE, N).

F2,26 =0.45, P=0.64; fledging weight: F1,26 =0.25, P=0.62; Fig. 2). The effect of hatching date was significant, birds that hatched earlier having a higher dominance rank (F1,26 =26.15; P<0.001; Fig. 2). The interaction terms were all nonsignificant (hatching order*fledging weight: F2,26 = 0.03, P=0.97; hatching order*hatching date: F2,26 =0.36, P=0.70; hatching date*fledging weight: F1,26 =0.05, P=0.83).

–2

3

I found a clear relationship between hatching date and agonistic behaviour of juvenile European shags during independence. Birds that hatched earlier had a higher dominance rank than those that hatched later. However, brood hierarchy and fledging weight did not affect dominance rank. The year of study, 1996, was a good year for breeding shags in Islas Cíes, with low chick mortality (Velando et al. 1999) and apparently abundant sandeels (Ammodytidae),

2 Hatching order

3

(c)

2

1

0

–1

–2 0

DISCUSSION

1

10

20 Hatching date

30

Figure 2. Relationship between the dominance rank (PC1 from a principal components analysis) of male (x) and female (C) juveniles in the cre`che and (a) fledging weight (no linear trend was observed in either sex), (b) hatching order (X±SE, N) and (c) hatching date (day 1=1 April; linear trend of both sexes is shown: males: R2 =0.47, N=16, P<0.01; females: R2 =0.61, N=21, P<0.05).

183

184

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 60, 2

the most important prey in the breeding season (Velando & Freire 1999). Thus, in this year, there was not much variation in weight between individuals, especially males. In carrion crows, Corvus corone, experimentally underfed chicks achieved lower social status in the flock (Richner et al. 1989). In the European shag, fledging weight and brood hierarchy may thus influence juvenile dominance only in years when food is scarce. In a study of great tits, hatching date affected the dominance rank of juveniles in the first week after fledging, but size was more important in the second week (Garnett 1981; but see Matthysen 1990). In my study, however, although the hatching date had a slight effect on weight at fledging, the weight of juveniles did not significantly influence their social rank. Sex also had an effect on rank, with males being slightly more dominant than females. During the period of independence, shags acquire secondary sex traits such as calls (males make an ‘ark’ sound, while females are silent; Snow 1963) which may cause the intensity of male threats to escalate. In several species, males in groups are more aggressive than females (e.g. Kettersen 1979; Piper & Wiley 1989). The effect of hatching date on dominance of juvenile shags suggests that social experience in the group may determine the dominance hierarchy, which is established during the first few encounters between individuals, as happens in other animals (Huntingford & Turner 1987). In flocks of many species, the birds that start the group dominate those arriving later (e.g. Holberton et al. 1990). In shags, when late chicks fly from the nest to the cre`che, they presumably lose their first fights and have a low social rank. Learning submissive behaviour, as shown by subordinates, has been described as an imposed weakness (Drummond & Osorno 1992). Moreover, aggressive tendencies and dominance status are correlated with slight differences in adrenal gland activity and brain chemistry (Brown 1975). In cre`ches of the European shag subordinate juveniles are less skilled in warding off other juveniles during feeding and the birds whose food is successfully stolen are those with a lower social rank (unpublished data). Dominant birds, therefore, are at an advantage when competing for resources. Thus, the hatching date may have long-term fitness consequences causing the future reproductive value of chicks to decline throughout the season. In addition to the advantages of early breeding discussed elsewhere (e.g. territory quality, mate quality and mating success, see Møller 1994), the chicks of early breeders may thus attain a higher social status and have better chances of survival (Perrins 1966; Nisbet & Drury 1972; Harris et al. 1992, 1994; Spear & Nur 1994).

Acknowledgments I am grateful to Xunta’s Wildlife Service (Servicio de Medio Ambiente Natural, MAN) for help, and thank Juan Freire, Nick Klomp, Eric W. M. Steinen, Jose Andre´ s, Jorge Domínguez, Luis Sampedro and Angela Turner for their valuable comments on the manuscript. This study was partially supported by a University of Vigo grant.

References Arcese, P. & Smith, J. N. H. 1985. Phenotypic correlates and ecological consequences of dominance in song sparrows. Journal of Animal Ecology, 54, 817–830. Boag, D. A. & Alway, J. H. 1980. Effects of social environment within the brood and dominance rank in gallinaceous birds. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 58, 44–49. Brown, J. L. 1975. The Evolution of Behavior. New York: Norton. Burger, J. 1980. The transition to independence and postfledging parental care in seabirds. In: Behavior of Marine Animals Vol. 9 (Ed. by J. Burger, B. L. Olla & H. E. Winn), pp. 367–447. New York: Plenum. Carter, H. R. & Hobson, K. A. 1988. Creching behavior of Brand’s cormorant chicks. Condor, 90, 395–400. Clutton-Brock, T. H. & Parker, G. A. 1995. Punishment in animal societies. Nature, 373, 206–216. Davis, L. S. 1982. Creching behaviour of Adelie penguin chicks (Pygoscelis adeliae). New Zealand Journal of Zoology, 9, 279–286. Drummond, H. & Canales, C. 1998. Dominance between booby nestlings involves winner and loser effects. Animal Behaviour, 55, 1669–1676. Drummond, H. & Osorno, J. L. 1992. Training siblings to be submissive losers: dominance between booby nestlings. Animal Behaviour, 44, 881–893. Ekman, J. & Askenmo, C. E. H. 1984. Social rank and habitat use in willow tit groups. Animal Behaviour, 32, 508–514. Evans, R. M. 1984. Some causal and functional correlates of creching in young white pelicans. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 62, 808–813. Garnett, M. C. 1981. Body size, its heritability and influence on juvenile survival among great tits Parus major. Ibis, 123, 31–41. Harris, M. P., Halley, D. J. & Wanless, S. 1992. The postfledging survival of young guillemots Uria aalge in relation to hatching date and growth. Ibis, 134, 335–339. Harris, M. P., Buckland, S. T., Russell, S. M. & Wanless, S. 1994. Post fledging survival to breeding age of shags Phalacrocorax aristotelis in relation to year, date of fledging and brood size. Journal of Avian Biology, 25, 268–274. Holberton, R. L., Hanano, R. & Able, K. P. 1990. Age-related dominance in male dark-eyed juncos: effects of plumage and prior residence. Animal Behaviour, 40, 573–579. Huntingford, F. & Turner, A. 1987. Animal Conflict. London: Chapman & Hall. Ketterson, E. D. 1979. Aggressive behavior in wintering dark-eyed juncos: determinants of dominance and their possible relation to geographic variation. Wilson Bulletin, 91, 371–383. Matthysen, E. 1990. Nonbreeding social organization in Parus. Current Ornithology, 7, 209–249. Møller, A. P. 1994. Sexual Selection and the Barn Swallow. London: Oxford University Press. Nisbet, I. C. T. & Drury, W. H. 1972. Post-fledging survival in herring gulls in relation to brood-size and date of hatching. Bird Banding, 43, 161–240. O’Connor, R. J. 1984. The Growth and Development of Birds. Chichester: J. Wiley. Østnes, J. E. & Bech, C. 1997. The early emergence of cold sensation in shag nestlings Phalacrocorax aristotelis. Journal of Avian Biology, 28, 4–30. Perrins, C. M. 1966. Survival of young Manx shearwaters Puffinus puffinus in relation to their presumed date of hatching. Ibis, 108, 132–135. Piper, W. H. & Wiley, R. H. 1989. Correlates of dominance in wintering white-throated sparrows: age, sex and location. Animal Behaviour, 37, 298–310. Richner, H., Schneiter, P. & Stirnimann, H. 1989. Life-history consequences of growth rate depression: an experimental study

VELANDO: DOMINANCE IN YOUNG SHAGS

on carrion crows (Corvus corone corone L.). Functional Ecology, 3, 617–624. Snow, B. 1960. The breeding biology of the shag Phalacrocorax aristotelis on the island of Lundy, Bristol Channel. Ibis, 102, 554–575. Snow, B. 1963. The behaviour of the shag. British Birds, 56, 77–103, 164–186. Spear, L. B. & Nur, N. 1994. Brood size, hatching order and hatching date: effects on four life-history stages from hatching to recruitment in western gulls. Journal of Animal Ecology, 63, 283–298. Stokland, J. N. & Amundsen, T. 1988. Initial size hierarchy in broods of the shag: relative significance of egg size and hatching asynchrony. Auk, 105, 308–315.

van Tets, G. F. 1965. A comparative study of some social communication patterns in the Pelecaniformes. Ornithological Monographs, 2, 1–88. Velando, A. & Freire, J. 1999. Intercolony and seasonal differences in the breeding diet of European shags on the Galician coast (NW Spain). Marine Ecology Progress Series, 188, 225–236. Velando, A., Ortega-Ruano, J. E. & Freire, J. 1999. Chick mortality in European shag Stictocarbo aristotelis related to food limitations during adverse weather events. Ardea, 87, 51–59. Watts, C. R. & Stokes, A. W. 1971. The social order of turkeys. Scientific American, 224, 112–118. Westman, B. 1990. Environmental effect on dominance in young great tits Parus major: a cross-fostering experiment. Ornis Scandinavica, 21, 46–51.

185

The importance of hatching date for dominance in ...

independence after fledging leads to their introduction ... normally reside (unpublished data). In the ... dominance rank of individuals during independence.

110KB Sizes 0 Downloads 225 Views

Recommend Documents

The importance of hatching date for dominance in young ... - CiteSeerX
and (3) hatching date, birds hatching earlier having more experience and a greater ... normally reside (unpublished data). ... Observations and Data Analysis.

The Increasing Dominance of Teams in Production of ...
Apr 12, 2007 - data covers research publications in science and engineering since 1955, the social ... complexity, and costs of big science. Surprisingly then, we ... of the hard sciences on lab scale and capital-intensive equipment, showed a ...

The Importance of Rapid Cultural Convergence in the Evolution of ...
Page 1 ... Adam Ferguson Building, 40 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9LL ... Recent work by Oliphant [5, 6], building on pioneering work by Hurford [2], ...

The Increasing Dominance of Teams in Production of ...
Apr 12, 2007 - engineering (with 171 subfields), social sciences (with 54 subfields) and the .... education), although even here teams currently have a large advantage in .... Society for Information Science and Technology, 54, 855. (2003). 6.

The importance of history in definitions of culture ...
resolution to such a question was to simply define cul- ... Multiple analyses using phylogenetic ..... Analyses of genetic data have confirmed that East Afri-.

The importance of history in definitions of culture ...
(cladistic) methods, however, have been shown not to support the genetic proposition. Rather, such ..... computer algorithms to determine the most parsimoni- ...... In T. Ingold (Ed.), Companion encyclopedia of an- thropology (pp. 350-365).

The Importance of Scale in Determining the Human ...
terrestrial systems such as Pacific atolls, the apparent patterns of these kinds of decisions may ..... Merlin, M., A. Capelle, T. Keene, J. Juvik & J. Maragos. 1994.

The Importance of History for Philosophy of Psychiatry ...
Sep 12, 2011 - ... Disorder (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1995). .... For a summary of how DSM-I classifications map onto DSM-. II classifications, see ...

The Relative Importance of Aspects of Intellectual Capital for Software ...
used in the development of software today. It covers the ... IC is an increasingly important resource for companies ... ious models and indicators of IC [20, 19, 10].

Importance of extremists for the structure of social ...
May 20, 2005 - Examples are the Internet, the World Wide Web (WWW), social networks of ... mechanism finds its roots in an old idea of Price [4], based on the so-called ... relationships, for instance, the best looking people usually have the ...

The Importance of Scale in Determining the Human ...
that Ailinginae Atoll had never been permanently inhabited (supported by past census ..... the Marshall Islands. marshall.csu.edu.au Charles Sturt. University ...

The Importance of Region 2.1 in Sustaining the ...
cantly affected by each of the substitutions of A at elevated temperature. The growth defect was ... revealed four regions of homology (1, 2). Two of them, des-.

The Importance of Professional Land Surveyor in The Land ...
Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. The Importance of Professional Land Surveyor in The Land Development Process.pdf.

The (un)importance of geographical mobility in the Great Recession
Stanford University, Economics Department, 579 Serra Mall, Stanford, CA ... years, measure job-related mobility more carefully, and account for the fact that ...

The importance of proofs of security for key ... - Semantic Scholar
Dec 7, 2005 - Information Security Institute, Queensland University of Technology, GPO Box 2434, ... examples of errors found in many such protocols years.

The Relative Importance of Aspects of Intellectual Capital for Software ...
The Relative Importance of Aspects of Intellectual Capital for Software ... 2School of Information Systems, Technology and Management ..... Human Resources. 1.

The Importance of History for Philosophy of Psychiatry ...
Sep 12, 2011 - Research support from the Center of Excellence in the. Arts and ..... toms that arise from a particular (dysfunctional) causal process, which can ..... Cross and Aetna were in contact with Spitzer's task force, and encouraged.

The Importance of History for Philosophy of Psychiatry ...
11 Murphy, Psychiatry in the Scientific Image, 349—350. ..... Spitzer, M. Sheehy and J. Endicott, “DSM-III: Guiding Principles” in V. M. Rakoff, H. C. Stancer, and.

The Importance of Hermeneutic Theory in ...
research relies on textual rather than numerical data, the wording and language requires a means of interpretation; hermeneutics provides this methodology of interpretation. Like qualitative research, hermeneutics aims to analyze textual material der

The-Importance-Of-Being-Alice-A-Matchmaker-In-Wonderland ...
A baron of dubious wealth—and not-so-dubious debt—Elliot Ainslie is just ... on-line electronic library that provides entry to great number of PDF file guide catalog. ... Morning Herald-13 hours in the past I do not watch this being a shortcoming

Importance of Solution Equilibria in the Directed ...
catalysis,6 and host guest chemistry7,8 to low-k dielectric coatings.9 Though oxide-based materials have attracted most of the attention,10,11 porous nonoxidic ...

The Undenied Importance of Roof Insulation in Melbourne.pdf ...
Page 3 of 55. UR SC ST OBC TOTAL Ex-SM VH OH HH VH OH HH. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21. Suitability for Persons with. disability Cat. No. Stages of Examination Name of the. Post. Level in 7th CPC. (erstwhile Pay. Band & GP in