The Personality Trait of Motor Impulsivity and Human Sweet Taste Preference Using Coffee Dongbo Che1 and Ching-Yun Chang2 Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota Previous studies have found a weak but significant correlation between the human personality trait of impulsivity and Body Mass Index (BMI), mediated by craving for high-calorie food, especially sugar. However, previous studies have failed to consider the effect of the sweet taste of sugar on people’s craving for food. Therefore, to differentiate the effect of the sweet taste from calories in sugar, the present study used decaffeinated coffee with zero-calorie sugar substitute to create four levels of sweet tastes without calories and test the correlation between sweet taste and motor impulsivity. Sixteen participants were recruited from a research methods class to be subjects in this experiment. The results, however, contradicted the hypothesis that people with high motor impulsivity would prefer sweeter tastes. This may indicate that people may have an ability of detecting the difference between beverages with and without calories. Future studies should use high-calorie food or beverages without particular tastes and invite more participants. Pages: 5-8

Lifetime obesity has become prevalent in the USA in recent years. From 1988-1994 to 2013-2014, the current-obese proportion among American adults increased from 22.5% to 37.4%, while the proportion of ever-obese adults rose from 33.7% to 51.2% (Stokes, Ni, & Preston, 2017). One way that scientists have studied the question of why people have become obese focused on the connection between the human body figure and a particular personality trait, impulsivity (e.g., Davis et al., 2017). Scientists have found evidence for a positive correlation between being overweight and having a particular personality trait, impulsivity, which is defined as having a rapid and thoughtless action or response tendency to stimuli (Kulendran, Patel, Darzi, & Vlaev, 2016). They used Body Mass Index (BMI) to measure the extent of participants’ obesity, with higher BMI scores corresponding to heavier body 1 Dongbo Che ([email protected]) is a senior in the College of Liberal Arts graduating in August 2019 with a B.A. in Psychology and a minor in Management. He wishes to pursue postgraduate studies in Industrial/Organizational Psychology, Counseling Psychology, and Human Resources & Industrial Relations. 2 Ching-Yun Chang ([email protected]) is a sophomore in the College of Liberal Arts. She will receive her B.A. in Psychology and minors in both Management and Family Social Science in May 2020. She wishes to pursue postgraduate studies in Family Social Science and Counseling Psychology.

VOLUME 16 – SPRING 2018 - www.psych.umn.edu/sentience © 2018 Regents of the University of Minnesota

weights. Researchers also used the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995), a widely used selfreport questionnaire, to measure impulsivity. Higher scores on the BIS indicate lack of self-control and more impulsive behaviors. Another more recent study conducted by Davis et al. (2017) studied the relationship between impulsivity and BMI. They found that the obese/overweight groups had both significantly higher scores on addictive behaviors (defined as “use for effect, one’s protection of supply, a preoccupation with the substance or activity, using more than the individual intended, and one’s increased capacity or tolerance for the behavior”, p. 579) and significantly higher personality-risk scores (combing BIS scores, high reward sensitivity, and anxiousness and negative affect) than the normal-weight control group. Moreover, they also found that it was high-risk personality that mediated the significant association between body weight and addictive behaviors. These results further supported the result of Kulendran et al. (2016) suggesting that high impulsivity is correlated with a larger mass to body-size ratio. After finding a possible correlation between BIS and BMI, other scientists focused on more in-depth connections between them. Researchers did this by dividing BIS into three lower level constructs consisting of attentional impulsivity (inability to concentrate), non-planning impulsivity (lack of future orientation), and motor impulsivity (acting without thinking; Meule & Platte, 2016). Their research showed that

5

MOTOR IMPULSIVITY AND SWEET TASTE PREFERENCE

participants that had both higher scores on motor impulsivity and higher attentional bias scores toward high-calorie food pictures would gain weight after roughly six months. In other words, one major factor influencing body mass was the interaction of motor impulsivity and attentional bias toward high-calorie food pictures. In a separate study, Meule and Blechert (2016) found a significant correlation between BMI and both attentional and motor impulsivity. However, they found no interactive effect between attentional and motor impulsivity in predicting a person being overweight. Although these two studies had slightly different results, they both supported the hypothesis that craving for high-calorie food might mediate the effect of impulsivity, especially motor impulsivity, on overweight human bodies. The relationship between impulsivity and BMI, however, was weak. To study the weak connection between impulsivity and BMI, some researchers focused on possible mediators such as the craving for high-calorie food, especially sugar. Some researchers found that one study conducted by Saliba, Wragg, and Richardson (2009) used sweet wine and dry wine to test participants’ preferences. They found a significant difference of impulsivity scores (using The Adult Impulsiveness, Venturesomeness and Empathy Scale, or IVE) between the group who chose sweet wine and the group who chose dry wine. This result supported their hypothesis that impulsivity positively correlated with sweet taste preference. Another study conducted by Kakoschke, Kemps, and Tiggemann (2015) studied the connection between specific impulsivity traits (attentional, motor, and non-planning) and sweet food intake. Their results showed a significant correlation between motor impulsivity and sugary food intake. Based on their findings, they speculated that motor impulsivity might act as a major factor to increase human response to external sweet food cues, increasing short-term consumption of high-calorie food and causing humans to be overweight over the long-term. Both of these studies supported the second assumption of Meule (2017) that it was the motor impulsivity that directly correlated with obese people’s high-calorie food preference. Even though Saliba et al. (2009) found a significant difference in participants’ gross impulsivity scores between the group that chose sweet wine and the group that chose dry wine, their study only analyzed the gross impulsivity score without more in-depth analysis of sub-scales of impulsivity, such as motor impulsivity. Moreover, there was a well-established connection between high energy-density food and people’s food preference (Drewnowski & Specter, 2004), and it was not clear to what extent high energy-density and sweet taste each separately affected people’s preference of food. Thus, to address the question that whether motor impulsivity (but not general impulsivity) affects people’s preference, and the question as to what extent people’s preference for sugar stems from its sweet taste or contained energy, the present study intended to conduct a research experiment using zero-calorie sugar substitutes to study the relationship between motor impulsivity and human sweet taste preferences. Based on the

VOLUME 16 – SPRING 2018 - www.psych.umn.edu/sentience © 2018 Regents of the University of Minnesota

Che and Chang

findings in the past literature, it was hypothesized that people who had a higher score of motor impulsivity would choose sweeter coffee than those who score lower on motor impulsivity. METHOD Participants A total of 16 participants, 15 undergraduate students and one graduate student, participated in the present study. Five participants were male and 11 were female. Their ages ranged from 19 to 26 (M=20.81, SD= 2.01); 43.75% of the total participants were White, 50% were Asian, and 6.35% were Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander. Three participants knew the hypothesis in advance, and their results were removed after the experiment. This study used convenience sampling. Since this study was run during a predetermined lab time of a research methods course, only students who registered for that course were available to be run as participants in the study. None of the subjects were compensated for their participation in the study. Materials The present study used four Starbucks Travelers, which were convenient carriers each containing 96 fluid ounces of brewed Decaf Pike Place Roast. To create an acceptable taste, one bottle of milk (20.27 fluid ounces) provided by Starbucks was evenly divided and added into each of the four Starbucks Travelers. The independent variable was the number of packs of zero-calorie sweetener (each pack contains 0.8 g sweetener), which were purchased at the same Starbucks store. The first Starbucks Traveler contained no packs of sugar substitute, the second one contained ten packs, the third one contained twenty packs, and the fourth one contained thirty packs. The present study used Qualtrics (qualtrics.umn.edu) to create an online survey of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; see Appendix) from Patton et al. (1995) to measure motor impulsivity. BIS-11 is a questionnaire consisting of 30 items designed to measure different types of impulsivity. Each item has four choices, with 1 indicating lowest impulsivity, and 4 indicating highest impulsivity. Procedure At first, each participant was invited into a private room and researchers read them a paragraph of informed consent. Then each participant tasted four small cups of coffee from cup 1 to cup 4. The numbers on the cups matched the numbers on the four Starbucks Travelers, each containing different amounts of sugar substitute as mentioned in the materials section. Between tasting each cup, participants were asked to wash their mouth using a bottle of fresh water. After tasting all four cups of coffee, participants were invited to a second private room. There they answered the survey mentioned in the materials section, which consisted of a question about their preferred coffee choice, 30 questions from

6

MOTOR IMPULSIVITY AND SWEET TASTE PREFERENCE

the BIS-11 scales, and some demographic items. After finishing the questionnaire, participants were debriefed. RESULTS Scores of 11 items in BIS-11 (2, 3, 4, 16, 17, 19, 21, 22, 23, 25, 30) were averaged to calculate motor impulsivity. Participants who had a higher score than the mean motor impulsivity of all participants (M=2.08, SD=0.30) were categorized as high motor impulsivity group, and all the other participants were categorized as low motor impulsivity group. The one-tailed t-test showed that coffee choices of those who had a lower motor impulsivity (M = 2.67, SD = 1.03) and coffee choices of those who had a higher motor impulsivity (M = 2.14, SD = 1.46) did not differ significantly, t(11) = 0.753, p = 0.234. DISCUSSION The results failed to support the hypothesis that the group that had higher motor impulsivity would prefer sweeter coffee. Moreover, the results of the present study also failed to corroborate the results of the study conducted by Saliba et al. (2009). One possible reason for this failure was due to the lack of relationship between motor impulsivity and sweet taste preference. However, since the present study used a zero-sugar substitute instead of sugar to create a sweet taste, the results of the present study might also stem from a fact that humans prefer sugar with calories more than substitutes without calories. If people did prefer sugar more than substitutes, it probably stemmed from an evolved human ability of distinguishing food with high-calories from those containing no-calories. Further studies could focus on human preference for calories but not sweet taste preferences. There were several limitations in the present study. The first limitation was that due to the time limit and situation constraints, only 16 participants from the same research methods course participated in the present study, and 3 of them were removed for knowing the hypothesis in advance. With this small sample, any results might contain relatively large sample bias. Another pitfall came from the procedure of adding milk in all the coffee drinks to create a better flavor. Since milk contains a high level of calories, it was also possible that the calories contained in the milk interfered with the human taste system on sweet preference and modulated the results. To better study the relationship between motor impulsivity and sweet taste preference, future studies need to recruit a larger group of people from varied backgrounds. More suitable beverages or study designs are also necessary. For example, using materials that are completely without calories would be preferable (e.g., Coke Zero that does not inherently have a high caloric count). As established in the previous literature, human body figure has been shown to be correlated with high motor impulsivity, but how these two aspects are linked to each other is still unclear. The present study had many limitations, and

VOLUME 16 – SPRING 2018 - www.psych.umn.edu/sentience © 2018 Regents of the University of Minnesota

Che and Chang

these limitations might cause the failure of replicating the result of Saliba et al. (2009). Future studies that address these limitations can better assess the relationship between motor impulsivity and sweet taste preference, and its further effect on BMI. After understanding a clear pathway between motor impulsivity and being overweight, people can take actions to decrease the effect of high motor impulsivity and live much healthier and happier lives. APPENDIX BIS-11 (Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995): DIRECTIONS: People differ in the ways they act and think in different situations. This is a test to measure some of the ways in which you act and think. Read each statement and put an X on the appropriate circle on the right side of this page. Do not spend too much time on any statement. Answer quickly and honestly. Rarely/ Never

Occasionally

Often

Almost Always/ Always

2) I do things without thinking. 3) I make-up my mind quickly. 4) I am happy-go-lucky. 16) I change jobs. 17) I act “on impulse”. 19) I act on the spur of the moment. 21) I change residences. 22) I buy things on impulse. 23) I can only think about one thing at a time. 25) I spend or charge more than I earn. 30) I am future oriented.

REFERENCES Davis, C., Mackew, L., Levitan, R.D., Kaplan, A.S., Carter, J.C., & Kennedy, J.L. (2017). Binge eating disorder (BED) in relation to addictive behaviors and personality risk factors. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 579. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00579 Drewnowski, A., & Specter, S. (2004). Poverty and obesity: The role of energy density and energy costs. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 79, 6-16. doi:10.1093/ajcn/79.1.6 Kakoschke, N., Kemps, E., & Tiggemann, M. (2015). External eating mediates the relationship between impulsivity and unhealthy food intake. Physiology & Behavior, 147, 117-121. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.04. 030 Kulendran, M., Patel, K., Darzi, A., & Vlaev, I. (2016). Diagnostic validity of behavioral and psychometric impulsivity measures: An assessment in adolescent and adult populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 347-352. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.026 Meule, A. (2017). Commentary: Questionnaire and behavioral task measures of impulsivity and differentially associated with body mass index: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. doi:10.3389/ fpsyg.2017.01222

7

MOTOR IMPULSIVITY AND SWEET TASTE PREFERENCE

Che and Chang

Meule, A., & Blechert, J. (2016). Trait impulsivity and body mass index: A cross-sectional investigation in 3073 individuals reveals positive, but very small relationships. Health Psychology Open, 3. doi:10.1177/ 2055102916659164 Meule, A., & Platte, P. (2016). Attentional bias toward high-calorie food-cues and trait motor impulsivity interactively predict weight gain. Health Psychology Open, 3. doi:10.1177/2055102916649585 Patton, J.H., Stanford, M.S., & Barratt, E.S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768-774. doi:10.1037/t05661-000 Saliba, A.J., Wragg, K., & Richardson, P. (2009). Sweet taste preference and personality traits using a white wine. Food Quality and Preference, 20, 572-575. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.05.009 Stokes, A., Ni, Y., & Preston, S.H. (2017). Prevalence and trends in lifetime obesity in the U.S., 1988-2014. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 53, 567-575. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2017.06.008

VOLUME 16 – SPRING 2018 - www.psych.umn.edu/sentience © 2018 Regents of the University of Minnesota

8

The Personality Trait of Motor Impulsivity and Human ...

tasting all four cups of coffee, participants were invited to a second private room. There they answered the survey mentioned in the materials section, which ...

159KB Sizes 1 Downloads 177 Views

Recommend Documents

The molecular genetic architecture of human personality
Mar 14, 2006 - computer games that go beyond the narrow self-report questionnaire ... tionnaire (TPQ)1,2 Novelty Seeking3 and NEO-PI-R ... and Scheinfeld Center for Genetic Studies, in the Social Sciences, ... 21 years of age) scored higher on Novelt

dissociation of action and intention in human motor ...
neuron system and reflects the motor program encoding the observed action. Whether motor resonance ... observer's own natural grasping program, regardless of the avatar's action – as the ..... abstract intentional content of the action. The above .

adhd symptoms, impulsivity and entrepreneurial action ... - SURFACE
affect individuals experience during opportunity recognition. Specifically, anxiety is ...... Second, we develop and test a fine-grained conceptual model linking ADHD symptoms ...... To thine own self be true: Images of self, images of opportunity ..

trait theory of leadership pdf
Page 1 of 1. File: Trait theory of leadership pdf. Download now. Click here if your download doesn't start automatically. Page 1 of 1. trait theory of leadership pdf.

Personality and the Strength and Direction of Partisan ...
during the 2008 campaign (abortion, civil unions, health care, and taxes). Finally, in column ..... Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, Inc., 273-296. Brewer, Marilynn B.

Artificial manipulation of human motor memories
Comm Tech ; 5ICTEAM and IoNS, Univ catholique Louvain, Belgium ... Participants performed 12 blocks of 18 reaching movements (training period) during ...

Dynamic Modulation of Human Motor Activity When Observing Actions
Feb 23, 2011 - College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom. Previous studies ... analyses, and to David Bradbury for technical assistance. We also ...

Memory for Angry Faces, Impulsivity, and Problematic ... - Springer Link
Published online: 23 January 2007. C Springer ... a junior secondary school were presented with angry and happy faces and were later asked to recognize ... For instance, aggressive children and adolescents are prone to attributing hostile ...

Does the Human Motor System Simulate Pinocchio's ... - SAGE Journals
1Institute of Neuroscience, National Yang Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan; 2Laboratories for Cognitive Neuroscience,. National Yang Ming University, Taipei, ...

Personality and Patterns of Facebook Usage
traits. Openness to experience measures a person's imagination, curiosity, seeking of new experiences and interest in culture, ideas, and aesthetics. It is related to emotional sensitivity, tolerance and political liberalism. People high on Openness

Does the Human Motor System Simulate Pinocchio's ...
sent in the individual go/no-go task because only one response code was formed and no conflict between spatial codes occurred. Address correspondence to Chia-Chin Tsai, Institute of Neurosci- ence, National Yang Ming University, 155, Sec 2, Li Long S

Personality and the coherence of psychotherapy ...
Mar 6, 2007 - Indeed, in an investigation of people's life stories, Lieblich (2004) found that her participants spontaneously brought up their experiences of psychotherapy when recounting their lives and often referred to these experiences as key sit

The Effect of Emotion and Personality on Olfactory ...
Mar 23, 2005 - Olfactory intensity was rated on a scale of 1–9 (from ex- tremely mild to extremely ... data sheet (Physical and Theoretical Chemistry Laboratory,.