G Model

ARTICLE IN PRESS

NSY 3003 1–5

Neuropsychologia xxx (2008) xxx–xxx 1

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

1

Note

4

Steven M. Platek ∗ , Austen L. Krill, Benjamin Wilson

5

School of Biological Sciences, The University of Liverpool, Crown Street, Liverpool L697ZB, UK

7

a r t i c l e

i n f o

a b s t r a c t

8

11 12 13

Article history: Received 30 April 2008 Received in revised form 11 June 2008 Accepted 15 July 2008 Available online xxx

On the basis of Hamilton’s (Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 17–52) theory of inclusive fitness, self-facial resemblance is hypothesized as a mechanism for self-referent phenotypic matching by which humans can detect kin. To understand the mechanisms underlying pro-sociality toward self-resembling faces, we investigated the neural correlates of implicit trustworthiness ratings for self-resembling faces. Here we show that idiosyncratic trustworthiness ratings of self-resembling faces predict brain activation in the ventral inferior, middle and medial frontal gyri, substrates involved in reward processing. These findings demonstrate that neural reward centers are implicated in evaluating implicit pro-social behaviors toward self-resembling faces. These findings suggest that humans have evolved to use neurocomputational architecture dedicated to face processing and reward evaluation for the differentiation of kin, which drives implicit idiosyncratic affectively regulated social interactions. © 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

DP

9 10

14

21

1. Introduction

22

Because incorrect assignment of kinship can be dire (inbreeding depression and cuckoldry), neural mechanisms that drive prosocial behavior toward kin are predicted to have evolved (Platek, Keenan, & Mohamed, 2005). Kin discrimination mechanisms allow individuals to modify their behavior with respect to genetic relatedness (Hamilton, 1964) and have been shown to impact social (DeBruine, Jones, Little, & Perrett, 2008; Platek, 2007), sexual (DeBruine, 2005; DeBruine, Jones, & Perrett, 2005), and parental behavior (Daly, 1982, 1998; Platek et al., 2003; Platek, Burch, Panyavin, Wasserman, & Gallup, 2002; Volk and Quinsey, 2002, 2007) in animals (Hain & Neff, 2006) and humans. Impression formation on the basis of facial processing occurs very quickly (Willis & Todorov, 2006) and implicit trustworthiness judgments have been associated with activation in the amygdala (Engell, Haxby, & Todorov, 2007; Winston, Strange, O’Doherty, & Dolan, 2002). Here we investigated activation associated with implicit trustworthiness judgments of faces that expressed varying levels of self-resemblance.

23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39

EC

19

RR

18

CO

17

UN

16

TE

20

Keywords: Self-face Self-reference Evolutionary cognitive neuroscience Amygdala Impression formation

15

RO

6

OF

3

Implicit trustworthiness ratings of self-resembling faces activate brain centers involved in reward

2

∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 151 795 4517. E-mail address: [email protected] (S.M. Platek).

2. Methods

40

2.1. Participants/stimuli

41

We transformed 11 (seven female) participants’ photographs to resemble them in three ways. Participant faces were: combined with another face of the same sex and ethnicity to create a 50% composite (self-morph), transformed to appear as a member of the opposite sex (self2sex), and transformed to appear as a member of another ethnicity (self2ethnic) (transformed to black face if white participant n = 9; transformed to white face if black participant n = 2). Facial morphing and transformations was carried out using specialist software (Tiddeman, Burt, & Perrett, 2001). One hundred seventy nine points were delineated on each face. We used these points to calculate the difference between each participant’s face and an average composite face of the same race and sex, same sex other race, and same race opposite sex. The resulting difference was then warped onto an average face to create self-resembling morphed and transformed images. All pictures were taken under standard lighting conditions and with the same camera. All participants provided written informed consent and the study was approved by the University of Liverpool Committee on Research Ethics.

42

2.2. Imaging parameters

57

Participants were asked to make perceptual discriminations about ethnicity and sex during rapid event-related fMRI (2 × 300 42 slice (10% gap) full brain coverage volumes; TR = 2.5, TE = 30 (ms), FoV = 19.2 cm, flip angle = 85) at 3 T. Images were presented for 750 ms. Participants were able to respond within a window of 2000 ms, including the face presentation time. A jittered interstimulus interval between 2.5 and 25 s was used by incorporating null events that consisted of a scrambled luminance matched face. Participants were instructed not to respond to the null event. Each face was presented 20 times. A high-resolution structural MP-RAGE (176 1 mm3 voxel sagittal slices, TR = 2040, TE = 5.57, FoV = 256, flip angle = 8) scan was also collected and was used to coregister functional images. Functional images were first realigned using FSL’s

58

0028-3932/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.018

Please cite this article in press as: Platek, S. M., et al. Implicit trustworthiness ratings of self-resembling faces activate brain centers involved in reward. Neuropsychologia (2008), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.018

43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56

59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68

G Model

ARTICLE IN PRESS

NSY 3003 1–5

S.M. Platek et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2008) xxx–xxx

UN

CO

RR

EC

TE

DP

RO

OF

2

Please cite this article in press as: Platek, S. M., et al. Implicit trustworthiness ratings of self-resembling faces activate brain centers involved in reward. Neuropsychologia (2008), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.018

G Model

ARTICLE IN PRESS

NSY 3003 1–5

S.M. Platek et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2008) xxx–xxx

2.3. Behavioral procedures

72

83

Four weeks after scanning, participants rated each face they saw during scanning on trustworthiness (idiosyncratic ratings) (see Engell et al., 2007; Winston et al., 2002). Additionally, we asked an independent group of 40 participants (20 female) to rate all the faces on trustworthiness to obtain consensus trustworthiness ratings (see Engell et al., 2007; Winston et al., 2002). Idiosyncratic and consensus trustworthiness ratings were entered as separate regressors into a parametric analysis used to model brain activation using FSL-FEAT (Smith et al., 2004). For each face transformation condition, we used mixed effects general linear models (Beckmann, Jenkinson, & Smith, 2003; Woolrich, Behrens, Beckmann, Jenkinson, & Smith, 2004; Worsley, Evans, Marrett, & Neelin, 1992) for activation as a function of both idiosyncratic and consensus trustworthiness ratings. Statistical parametric threshold was set to uncorrected p < .005.

84

3. Results

85

Consensus ratings for self-morph faces were negatively associated with activation in the left parahippocampal gyrus, whereas idiosyncratic ratings showed a positive association with activation in right ventral middle frontal gyrus (Fig. 1a and b; Table 1). Consensus ratings for self2sex faces were associated with activation in right superior temporal sulcus, right insula, and right parahippocampal gyrus, where as idiosyncratic ratings of self2sex were associated with activation in left ventral superior frontal gyrus, right ventral inferior frontal gyrus, and left medial frontal gyrus. Consensus ratings were negatively associated with activation in a region of interest (ROI) at the parahippocampal/uncus/amygdala, while idiosyncratic ratings were positively associated with activation in an ROI at the ventral medial prefrontal cortex (Fig. 1c and d; Table 1). Consensus ratings for self2black transformed face were associated with activation in the left thalamus, let cingulate gyrus, left medial frontal gyrus, right lingual gyrus, right postcentral gyrus, right anterior parahippocampal gyrus/uncus, left middle frontal gyrus, and right amygdala, whereas idiosyncratic ratings were associated with activation in the right inferior temporal gyrus, left anterior cingulate, and right parahippocampal gyrus. Consensus trustworthiness ratings were negatively associated with activation in an ROI at the right parahippocampal/uncus/amygdala and fusiform gyrus ROI. Idiosyncratic ratings were negatively associated with activation in the right parahippocampal gyrus and medial prefrontal cortex (Fig. 1e and f; Table 1). Consensus ratings for white distracter faces were associated with activation in right superior temporal sulcus, right insula, left cingulate gyrus, right parahippocampal gyrus, left lingual gyrus, and left inferior frontal gyrus, whereas idiosyncratic ratings of white faces were associated with activation in left middle occipital gyrus, right middle temporal gyrus, right superior frontal gyrus, right fusiform gyrus, right cuneus, left inferior occipital gyrus, and left cingulate gyrus (Table 1). Consensus ratings for black faces were associated with activation in left precuneus, left inferior parietal lobe, left fusiform

78 79 80 81 82

86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120

TE

77

EC

76

RR

75

CO

74

4. Discussion

132

Important social judgments about faces can be made quickly (Willis & Todorov, 2006) and without need to deeply process or act on the judgments (Bargh & Chartland, 2005). Several studies have demonstrated negative associations between trustworthiness ratings and activation in the medial temporal lobe (amygdala, uncus and parahippocampal gyrus) (Engell et al., 2007; Winston et al., 2002). The amygdala appears to be involved in automatic face coding with respect to impression management and is predicted to drive social responses toward individuals. Similarly, several studies have demonstrated that attractive faces activate neural substrates implicated in reward processing (O’Doherty et al., 2003; Winston, O’Doherty, Kilner, Perrett, & Dolan, 2007) and that amygdala activation to faces based on attractiveness indicates the social value of the face (Winston et al., 2007). Our findings show that idiosyncratic ratings of trustworthiness in self-resembling faces, a hypothesized cue to kinship, are associated parametrically with increased activation in neural substrates implicated in reward. Self-face resemblance has been shown to increase ratings of attractiveness and trust. The current findings ratify these behavioral studies by showing that the affectively regulated pro-social behavior toward self-resembling faces is caused by parametrically activated neural reward circuitry. This effect is marked in same ethnicity faces regardless of sex, but is not upheld in differing ethnic faces. These results are consistent with studies showing increased activation in the amygdala and fusiform gyrus in response to ethnically different faces and extend these previous findings by showing that ethnically different faces also exaggerate amygdala-related activation associated with implicit trustworthiness ratings. That same and opposite sex same ethnicity self-resembling faces activate reward centers suggests that appetitive responses would be directed toward kin of either sex (i.e., brothers/sisters, nieces/nephews). Because these reward regions are also activated in response to facial attractiveness, our findings extend the KIi model (Lieberman, 2007), which hypothesizes the presence of dedicated neurocomputational architecture for kin detection. Our findings suggest that humans use existing neurocomputational architecture involved in face processing (Gobbini & Haxby, 2007), reward evaluation (Aharon et al., 2001; Breiter & Rosen, 1999; O’Doherty, Kringelbach,

UN

73

121

OF

71

gyrus, left superior frontal gyrus, right cingulate gyrus, and left inferior frontal gyrus, whereas idiosyncratic rating of black faces showed activation only in the left middle and superior occipital gyrus (Table 1). Collapsing across all other distractor faces (white and black) revealed activation associated with trust rating in left inferior occipital gyrus and right middle frontal gyrus, whereas idiosyncratic ratings of faces collapsing across white and black were associated with activation in left lingual gyrus, right precuneus, left superior temporal gyrus, right posterior cingulate, and left cuneus (Table 1).

RO

McFLIRT program. Images were then intensity normalized and smoothed (full width half maximum = 6) using mixed effects modelling in FEAT (Smith et al., 2004).

69

DP

70

3

Fig. 1. Activation maps for idiosyncratic and consensus parametric ratings of trustworthiness in self-morph, self2sex, and self2ethnic faces. (a) Activation in the ventral middle frontal gyrus associated with idiosyncratic ratings of trustworthiness in self-morph faces and plot of parameter estimates showing positive association between trustworthiness ratings and activation in this region. (b) Activation in the parahippocampal gyrus associated with consensus ratings of trustworthiness and plot of parameter estimates showing negative association between trustworthiness ratings and activation in this region. (c) Activation in the ventral medial frontal gyrus associated with idiosyncratic ratings of trustworthiness in self2sex faces and plot of parameter estimates showing positive association between trustworthiness ratings and activation in this region. (d) Activation in the parahippocampal gyrus/uncus associated with consensus ratings of trustworthiness in self2sex faces and plot of parameter estimates showing positive negative between trustworthiness ratings and activation in this region. (e) Activation in the parahippocampal gyrus (yellow) and medial prefrontal gyrus (green) associated with idiosyncratic ratings of trustworthiness in self2ethnic faces and plot of parameter estimates showing negative association between trustworthiness ratings and activation in this region. (f) Activation in the parahippocampal gyrus/uncus/amygdala (red) and fusiform gyrus (red) associated with consensus ratings of trustworthiness in self2ethnic faces and plot of parameter estimates showing negative association between trustworthiness ratings and activation in the parahippocampal gyrus/uncus/amygdala and no relationship, but heightened activation in the fusiform gyrus.

Please cite this article in press as: Platek, S. M., et al. Implicit trustworthiness ratings of self-resembling faces activate brain centers involved in reward. Neuropsychologia (2008), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.018

122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131

133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171

G Model

ARTICLE IN PRESS

NSY 3003 1–5

4

S.M. Platek et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2008) xxx–xxx

Table 1 List of parametrically activated substrates and Z scores for statistical parametric maps Substrate

Z score

MNI Coordinates (x, y, z)

Self-morph Consensus Idiosyncratic

Left parahippocampal gyrus Right ventral middle frontal gyrus

2.8 2.48

−24, −40, −16 36, 40, −19

Right superior temporal sulcus Right insula Right parahippocampal gyrus

2.98 2.54 2.76

58, −54, 20 34, 18, 10 34, 10, −28

Left ventral superior frontal gyrus Right ventral inferior frontal gyrus Left medial frontal gyrus

2.6 2.58 2.64

−18, 50, −18 26, 32, −14 −6, 50, 8

Self2ethnic Consensus

Idiosyncratic

Non-self-resembling faces Distracter white faces Consensus

Right inferior temporal sulcus Left anterior cingulate Right parahippocampal gyrus

Right superior temporal sulcus Right insula Left cingulate gyrus Right parahippocampal gyrus Left lingual gyrus Left inferior frontal gyrus Left middle occipital gyrus Right middle temporal gyrus Right superior frontal gyrus Right fusiform Right cuneus Left inferior occipital gyrus Left cingulate gyrus

Idiosyncratic

Left precuneus Left inferior parietal lobe Left fusiform gyrus Left superior frontal gyrus Right cingulate gyrus and left inferior frontal gyrus

RR

Distractor black faces Consensus

EC

TE

Idiosyncratic

Left thalamus left cingulate gyrus Left medial frontal gyrus Right lingual gyrus Right postcentral gyrus Right ant. parahippocampal gyrus Left middle frontal gyrus Right amygdala

RO

Idiosyncratic

DP

Self2sex Consensus

OF

Contrast

Left middle Superior occipital gyrus

Collapsing across white and black distracter faces Consensus Left inferior occipital gyrus Right middle frontal gyrus

174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181

Rolls, Hornack, & Andrews, 2001), and prediction (Berns, McClure, Pagnoni, & Mantague, 2001; Hsu, Bhatt, Adolphs, Tranel, & Camerer, 2005) in a combinatorial fashion to make adaptive decisions about faces that may represent members of their kin group (see Platek et al., 2005, 2004, in press). It seems more plausible that evaluation of kinship would recruit several neural systems because the consequences of incorrect kin assignment can be dire (e.g., inbreeding depression and cuckoldry). Recent evidence suggests dopamine D2 receptor density in the medial prefrontal cortex is associated with differences in learning from one’s own errors (Klein et al., 2007).

UN

172 173

Left lingual gyrus Right precuneus Left superior temporal gyrus Right posterior cingulate and left cuneus

CO

Idiosyncratic

3.61 4.67 3.7 4.67 3.21 2.61 3.32 2.61

−14, −18, 16 −8, −8, 48 −6, 8, 52 −8, −8, 48 24, −34, 50 24, −2, −14 −30, 58, 12 28, −4, −18

3.28 2.69 2.67

52, 0, −36 −8, 44, −4 32, −10, −32

2.83 2.67 3.01 2.78 2.74 2.34

62, 12, −2 40, −6, 8 −12, −4, 50 30, −28, −26 −18, −84, −4 −36, 22, 6

2.87 2.79 2.79 2.95 2.51 2.82 2.58

−28, −84, 18 34, −76, 16 20, 62, 14 28, −58, −14 20, −90, 18 −28, −86, −8 0, −28, 30

3.02 2.75 3.14 2.79 2.66 2.84

−30, −68, 46 −56, −44, 46 −46, −38, −22 −24, 14, 56 2, 10, 44 −40, 32, 14

2.89 2.83

−26, −80, 12 −36, −90, 22

2.45 2.66

−36, −94, −8 48, 22, 30

2.81 2.98 2.93 2.71 2.83

−12, −60, 0 4, −72, 18 −54, −28, 2 18, −58, 12 −14, −76, 10

If the findings of Klein et al. can be extended (from their investigation of addictive behaviors/tendencies) to interpersonal based social behaviors, then one might speculate that this neurogenetic loci is also involved in monitoring behavioral outcomes between social group members, specifically those outcomes that bear on kin-appetitive/aversive outcomes. Our findings suggest that part of the neural network involved in differentiating kin from nonkin members of one’s social group involves recruitment of several substrates involved in generalized face (Gobbini and Haxby, 2006, 2007; Gobinni, Leibenluft, Satiago, & Haxby, 2004; Golby, Gabrieli,

Please cite this article in press as: Platek, S. M., et al. Implicit trustworthiness ratings of self-resembling faces activate brain centers involved in reward. Neuropsychologia (2008), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.018

182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191

G Model

ARTICLE IN PRESS

NSY 3003 1–5

S.M. Platek et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2008) xxx–xxx

195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203

204

205

206

207

Q1 Uncited references

Hassin et al. (2005). Acknowledgement This work was supported by the Pioneer Fund, Inc. References

209

Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32(3), 537–551. Bargh, J. A., & Chartland, T. (2000). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 41, 521–528. Beckmann, C., Jenkinson, M., & Smith, S. M. (2003). General multi-level linear modelling for group analysis in fMRI. Neuro Image, 20, 1052–1063. Berns, G. S., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G., & Mantague, P. R. (2001). Predictability modulates human brain response to reward. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 2793–2798. Breiter, H. C., & Rosen, B. R. (1999). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of brain reward circuitry in the human. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 877, 523–547. Daly, M. (1982). Whom are new born babies said to resemble? Ethology and Sociobiology, 3, 69–78. Daly, M. (1998). The truth about Cinderella: A Darwinian view of parental love. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. DeBruine, L. M. (2005). Trustworthy but not lust-worthy, context specific effects of facial resemblance. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 272(1566), 919–922. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Women’s attractiveness judgments of self-resembling faces change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 47(4), 379–383. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Social perception of facial resemblance in humans. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(1), 64–77. Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1508–1519. Gobbini, M. I., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia, 45, 32–41. Gobbini, M. I., & Haxby, J. V. (2006). Neural response to the visual familiarity of faces. Brain Research Bulletin, 71(1-3), 76–82. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.08.003. Gobinni, M. I., Leibenluft, E., Satiago, N., & Haxby, J. V. (2004). Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces. Neuro Image, 22, 1628–1635. Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D., Chiao, J. Y., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2001). Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nature Neuroscience, 4(8), 845–850.

216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242

TE

215

EC

214

RR

213

CO

212

UN

211

DP

208

210

Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy Science USA, 98(7), 4259–4264. Hain, T. J. A., & Neff, B. D. (2006). Promiscuity drives self-referent kin recognition. Current Biology: CB, 16(18), 1807–1811. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 17–52. Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2005). The new unconscious. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press. Hsu, M., Bhatt, M., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Camerer, C. F. (2005). Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science (N.Y.), 310(5754), 1680–1683. Klein, T. A., Neumann, J., Reuter, M., Hennig, J., von Cramon, D. Y., & Ullsperger, M. (2007). Genetically determined differences in learning from errors. Science, 318(5856), 1642. Leveroni, C. L., Seidenberg, M., Mayer, A. R., Mead, L. A., Binder, J. R., & Rao, S. M. (2000). Neural systems underlying the recognition of familiar and newly learned faces. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(2), 878–886. Lieberman, D. (2007). The architecture of human kin detection. Nature, 445, 727–731. O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornack, J., & Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitalfrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 95–102. O’Doherty, J., Winston, J. S., Critchley, H. D., Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Beauty in a smile: The role of the medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41., 147–155. Platek, S. M., Critton, S. R., Burch, R. L., Frederick, D. A., Myers, T., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2003). How much paternal resemblance is enough? Sex differences in the reaction to resemblance but no in ability to detect resemblance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 81–87. Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P., & Mohamed, F. B. (2005). Sex differences in the neural correlates of child facial resemblance: An event-related fMRI study. Neuro Image, 25., 1336–1344. Platek, S. M., Burch, R. L., Panyavin, I. S., Wasserman, B. H., & Gallup, G. G. (2002). Reactions to children’s faces resemblance affects males more than females. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(3), 159–166. Platek, S. M. (2007). Facial resemblance exaggerates sex-specific jealousy-based decisions. Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 223–231. Platek, S. M., Raines, D. M., Gallup, J. G. G., Mohamed, F. B., Thomson, J. W., Myers, T. E., et al. (2004). Reactions to children’s faces: Males are more affected by resemblance than females are, and so are their brains. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(6), 394–405. Platek, S. M., Krill, A. L., & Kemp, S. M. (in press). The neural basis of facial resem- Q2 blance. Neuroscience Letters, Corrected Proof doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2008.03.040. Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuro Image, 1(Suppl. 23), S208–S219. Tiddeman, B., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Computer graphics in facial perception research. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 21, 42–50. Volk, A., & Quinsey, V. L. (2002). The influence of infant facial cues on adoption preference. Human Nature, 13, 437–455. Volk, A., & Quinsey, V. L. (2007). Parental investment and resemblance: Replications, refinements, and revisions. Evolutionary Psychology, 5(1), 1–14. Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17, 592–598. Winston, J. S., O’Doherty, J., Kilner, J. M., Perrett, D. I., & Dolan, R. J. (2007). Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 45, 195–206. Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 277–283. Woolrich, M. W., Behrens, T. E., Beckmann, M., Jenkinson, M., & Smith, S. M. (2004). Multi-level linear modelling for fMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. Neuro Image, 21, 1732–1747. Worsley, K. J., Evans, A. C., Marrett, S., & Neelin, P. (1992). A three-dimensional statistical analysis for CBF activation studies in human brain. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 12.

OF

194

Chiao, & Eberhardt, 2001; Gusnard, Akbudak, Shulman, & Raichle, 2001; Leveroni et al., 2000) (and other social sensory, e.g., olfactory) processing circuitry as a recursively monitored sub-circuitry which feeds a kinship estimator. This process may not be limited simply to differentiation of kin from non-kin and same race from other race faces, but may form the basis for a neural substrate associated with identification of group membership more broadly speaking. As such, this could represent a hierarchical mechanism involved in selection acting at various levels of group membership including such characteristics as ethnic, cultural and even possibly linguistic similarities. This suggestion demands further research.

RO

192 193

5

Please cite this article in press as: Platek, S. M., et al. Implicit trustworthiness ratings of self-resembling faces activate brain centers involved in reward. Neuropsychologia (2008), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.018

243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307

trustworthiness in self-resembling faces

School of Biological Sciences, The University of Liverpool, Crown Street, Liverpool L697ZB, ... Available online xxx ...... IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 21, 42–50. ... Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O'Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002).

692KB Sizes 0 Downloads 161 Views

Recommend Documents

Cooperation and Trustworthiness in Repeated Interaction
Jun 1, 2015 - Most contributions discussing the effects of institutions or social ... Hence, group members' benefits from cooperation depend .... of punishment in fostering cooperation depends on monitoring ...... media on cooperation.

Estimating the Trustworthiness of Web Sources.pdf
other tasks that involve data integration and data cleaning. 2. PROBLEM DEFINITION AND OVERVIEW. In this section, we start with a formal definition of ...

Trustworthiness and competitive altruism can also solve ...
Individuals may be altruistic because it will make them more trustworthy. .... extra incentive for being the most altruistic individual. 2. Methods ... recipients' decisions, participants indicated how much money they would return for each possible .

Learning affective values for faces is expressed in ...
and to a high degree shaped by responses engendered by specific social encounters. The precise ...... fusiform circuit in the CR, possibly as a teaching signal. (prediction error). ... Supplementary data are available at SCAN online. 116.

Face Recognition in Surgically Altered Faces Using Optimization and ...
translation and scale invariance [3]. Russell C.Eberhart (2006) emphasis that the ... Each particle in the search space evolves its candidate solution over time, making use of its individual memory and knowledge gained by the swarm as a ... exchange

Quantifying the trustworthiness of social media content
data but also a method to quantify the value of content. Is the information ... using two data sets consisting of health articles from Wikipedia and advice shared on.

FACES(2014).pdf
through the eyes of non-YWAMers. We asked them to share with us, and with. you, their own personal experience of YWAM Singapore and the YWAMers who.

JavaServer™ Faces Specification
This package contains APIs that are used to link an application's business logic .... Call the render() method on the Lifecycle instance that was acquired. This.

Trustworthiness and competitive altruism can also solve ...
exchanged at the end of the experiment to Canadian dollars at a rate of 15:1, with a 1 in 36 chance ..... Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature ...

Quantifying the trustworthiness of social media content
ular social media site with collaboratively contributed content, wikiHow. The article ... most popular social media health resource followed by forums and social networks. ..... Consider the case where three classes have scores of 10, 5, and 1.

Estimating the Trustworthiness of Web Sources.pdf
Based on the single-truth assumption, we define a latent variable. Vd ∈ dom(d) for each data item to present the true value for d,. where dom(d) is the domain (set of possible values) for data item. d. Let V = {Vd} and note that we can derive T = {

Trust, Trustworthiness, and Business Success: Lab ...
I also thank the sales marketing department at New Tel LLC, the local office managers of. Mobicom, the ... Email: [email protected] ...... the counterpart's responses (t2 as function of t1) are identical to those of the sender. 0. 2000.

Evaluating the Trustworthiness of Wikipedia Articles ...
However, search relevance does not indicate whether the content is trustworthy. With the advent of social media content contributed by unknown users, it is necessary to assess the trustworthiness of every piece of content as trust cannot be placed on

Does Trust Beget Trustworthiness? Trust and ...
Nov 2, 2006 - of Science and by the Russell Sage Foundation Trust. Project, New York City. .... In an alternative strategy employed to tease out the specific ...

Quantifying Utility and Trustworthiness for Advice ...
The proliferation of Social Media portals in recent years has resulted in a torrent of user-shared content on the web. Articles about politics, history, and health are available on wikis and blogs. Advice is being solicited on question-answer sites a

Faces-of-Spina-Bifida.pdf
Page 1 of 21. Faces of Spina Bifida. Lieven Bauwens. Secretary General IF. Member of Food For=fica=on Ini=a=ve EMT. Minbuza lunchseminar – 17 December 2015. Page 1 of 21 ...

Social attributions from faces bias human choices
ders we select and the individuals we choose to trust. Nevertheless, our impressions of people are shaped by their facial appearances and, consequently, so too ...