1

Journal of Parkinson’s Disease xx (20xx) x–xx DOI 10.3233/JPD-140390 IOS Press

3

roo f

2

Quality of Life and Mild Cognitive Impairment in Early Parkinson’s Disease: Does Subtype Matter?

or P

1

5

Rachael A Lawsona,∗ , Alison J Yarnalla , Gordon W Duncana , Tien K Khoob , David P Breenc , Roger A Barkerc , Daniel Collertona , John-Paul Taylora and David J Burna

6

a Institute

7 8

b Griffith

for Ageing and Health, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK Health Institute & School of Medicine, Griffith University, Gold Coast, Australia c John van Geest Centre for Brain Repair, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

uth

4

16

Keywords: Parkinson’s disease, quality of life, mild cognitive impairment, subtypes

17

INTRODUCTION

18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30

cte

14

rre

13

For the individual living with Parkinson’s disease (PD), the development of dementia (PDD) has a significant impact upon quality of life (QoL) [1]. Mild cognitive impairment in PD (PD-MCI) may be a precursor to PDD, and thus may also impact on QoL [2]. Furthermore, QoL may be affected by particular subtypes of MCI [1], where impairments in multiple domains may have greater cognitive burden and thus may cause greater difficulties in activities of daily living (ADL) and poorer QoL [3, 4]. Additionally, specific domains, e.g. deficits in attention and executive function which are common in PD, may be more detrimental to ADL and QoL [4–6].

co

12

Un

11

dA

15

Abstract. We evaluated the association between mild cognitive impairment (MCI) subtypes and quality of life (QoL) in 219 newly diagnosed Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients without dementia. Participants completed neuropsychological tests of attention, executive function, visuospatial function, memory, and language, and reported QoL using the Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire. Impairments were most common in executive function, memory and attention. MCI subtypes were classified according to Movement Disorder Society Task Force criteria. More severe cognitive impairment was associated with poorer quality of life (p = 0.01), but subtype of impairment was not (p > 0.10), suggesting that the nature of cognitive impairment is less significant than its severity.

9 10

∗ Correspondence

to: Rachael A Lawson, Clinical Ageing Research Unit, Institute for Ageing and Health, Newcastle University, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE4 5PL, UK. Tel.: +44 191 208 1277; Fax: +44 191 208 1251; E-mail: [email protected].

To the authors’ knowledge, there are currently no studies investigating whether QoL in people with PDMCI differs between subtypes. Such information could help guide clinicians as to what pharmacological and non-pharmacological interventions might be particularly effective [7]; e.g. cholinesterase inhibitors for attention could improve QoL [5, 8]. We evaluated the impact of PD-MCI subtypes upon QoL in a large cohort of patients with newly diagnosed PD. We hypothesised that PD-MCI would negatively affect QoL and that the effect across the different MCI subtypes would not be equal, with executive dysfunction and attention having a greater impact. PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS Newly diagnosed idiopathic PD patients, as per Queen’s Square Brain Bank criteria [9], were recruited from community and outpatient clinics. Participants and age-sex matched healthy controls completed a schedule of neuropsychological assessments covering

ISSN 1877-7171/14/$27.50 © 2014 – IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved

31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43

44

45 46 47 48 49

57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95

96

97 98

roo f

56

or P

55

age (mean of 65.9 ± 9.7 vs 67.9 ± 8.2 years; p = 0.06), sex (males 63.9% vs 55%; p = 0.11), level of education (mean of 13.1 ± 3.4 vs 12.8 ± 3.6 years; p = 0.36) and NART scores (mean of 115.8 ± 8.7 vs 114.3 ± 10.3; p = 0.37). Mean duration of PD was 5.5 ± 5.0 months, 83% were rated as Hoehn and Yahr stage 1 or 2. Using our modified MDS criteria 66% (n = 144) were classified as having PD-MCI (≥1 SD below normative data); they were significantly older (p < 0.01), had completed fewer years of education (p < 0.01) and more severe motor symptoms (p < 0.01). QoL scores were significantly worse for participants with PD-MCI (mean of 24.3 ± 19.4 vs 31.2 ± 23.3; p = 0.03). Participants were grouped by MCI severity, participants scoring: ≥1 SD but <1.5 SD below normative data (PD-MCI 1SD, n = 51); ≥1.5 SD but <2 SD below normative data (PD-MCI 1.5 SD, n = 44); and ≥2 SD below normative data (PD-MCI 2 SD, n = 49). Post hoc analysis showed PDQ-39 scores were significantly worse using the 2 SD PD-MCI cut off (mean of 38.2 ± 25.2; p = 0.01) compared to 1 SD and 1.5 SD cut off (mean of 26.6 ± 21.9 and 28.8 ± 21.2 respectively), suggesting that more severe MCI was a major contributor to poor quality of life in the MCI group.

dA

54

cte

53

rre

52

five cognitive domains: attention, executive function, visuospatial function, language and memory [10]. Attention was assessed using Power of Attention and Digit vigilance from the Cognitive Drug Research Battery [11]. Executive function was assessed using the One touch Tower of London from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) [12], phonemic fluency (number of words generated in 60 seconds beginning with the letter F) and semantic fluency (number of names of animals generated in 90 seconds) [13, 14]. Memory was assessed using Spatial and Pattern Recognition Memory, and Paired Associate Learning subsets from the CANTAB. Visuospatial dysfunction was measured using a modified scoring for copying interlocking pentagons [15]. Language dysfunction was measured using the naming and language scores from the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [16]. Premorbid intelligence was measured using the National Adult Reading Test (NART) [17]. As our study design predated the Movement Disorder Society (MDS) Task Force PD-MCI criteria, we used a modified Level II criteria, which is described in full elsewhere [10], at cut offs of 1.0, 1.5 or 2.0 standard deviations (SD) below normative values (controls) to classify PD-MCI. For non-normally distributed neuropsychological scores, an approximation to the normal distribution was used to calculate cut-offs. Single-domain PD-MCI was defined as impairment in two tests in the same domain; multi-domain classification required at least one impaired test in more than one domain [18]. Participants were then classified according to impairments in specific domains. The Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire (PDQ-39) single index score was used to assess QoL [19], where a score of 0 indicates best QoL and a maximum score of 100 indicates worst QoL. This questionnaire is widely used and recommended for measuring QoL in PD [20]. This study was approved by the Newcastle and North Tyneside Research Ethics Committee. All participants provided written informed consent. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Version 19.0. Comparisons of means between two groups or more than two groups were performed using independent t-tests, Mann-Whitney U test or KruskalWallis tests as appropriate..

co

51

Un

50

R.A. Lawson et al. / MCI Subtypes and Quality of Life

uth

2

RESULTS There were no significant differences between PD participants (n = 219) and controls (n = 99) in terms of

Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment The MDS PD-MCI criteria identified 22 subtypes (Fig. 1). Significantly more subjects were classified as having multi-domain than single-domain MCI (p < 0.01). Executive function (67%), memory (61%) and attention (51%) were most frequently impaired. The most common subtypes were memory+executive function (22%), attention+executive function (13%) and attention+memory+executive function (13%). Figure 1 also shows that the number of participants in each subtype was relatively small; the first seven subtypes accounted for the majority of participants, with those after the divide making up less than 5% of participants in each subgroup. Figure 2 shows a radar plot of the seven most frequent subtypes and PDQ-39 score by PD-MCI cut off; values extending from the centre of plot indicate worse QoL. In most subtypes, QoL decreased as cognitive impairment increased. Single-domain attention and multi-domain attention+memory+executive function tended to be associated with poorer QoL at the 2 SD cognitive cut off compared to other cut offs, but overall there were no significant differences in QoL between MCI subtypes (p > 0.10). We conducted a secondary analysis using classifications outlined by Marras, et al. [21], which required two

99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122

123

124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148

3

dA

uth

or P

roo f

R.A. Lawson et al. / MCI Subtypes and Quality of Life

152 153 154 155 156 157

158

159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169

rre

151

tests impaired in at least one domain. This identified fewer PD-MCI participants (≥1 SD below normative data; n = 104) with significantly more single-domain impairments (76%). The most common subtypes were single-domain executive function (25%) and memory (19%); memory+executive function remained the most common multiple-domain subtype (5%). There were no significant differences in QoL between subtypes (p > 0.10).

co

150

DISCUSSION

Un

149

cte

Fig. 1. Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in participants with newly diagnosed Parkinson’s disease. Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment (MCI): A = Attention; E = Executive function; L = Language; M = Memory; V = Visuospatial function.

In this large incidence cohort, we found that QoL decreased as severity of MCI increased. We identified a number of PD-MCI subtypes and found more multidomain impairments than single-domain, although there were no significant differences in QoL between subtypes. This may suggest that in PD-MCI it is the severity of cognitive impairment, rather than the nature of impairment, that impacts on QoL. Impairment was most common in executive function, memory and attention. Two thirds of PD-MCI participants had executive dysfunction. A recent study

has also observed high frequencies of executive dysfunction in PD-MCI [22], which can impair ADL [8]. Memory was the second most common impaired domain, which was also reported by Goldman, et al. [22], and has been shown to negatively impact on QoL [23, 24]. Half of PD-MCI participants had attentional dysfunction, which was also the most common single-domain subtype. This has also been observed in a previous study [25] and has been shown to significantly impact physical functioning and social interaction [5]. Significantly, only 15% of PD-MCI subjects had single-domain impairment. The high proportion of multi-domain impairments has also been found in other studies [21, 22, 26, 27]. To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the relationship between QoL and PD-MCI subtypes. More severe impairment in single-domain attention and multi-domain attention+memory+executive function subtypes tended to be associated with poorer QoL, although we did not find any significant differences in QoL score between subtypes. Previous studies have variable findings between cognitive domains. Attention and memory problems have been associated with worse QoL [23], whilst another study found

170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193

R.A. Lawson et al. / MCI Subtypes and Quality of Life

cte

dA

uth

or P

roo f

4

196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214

visuospatial function, executive function and attention in PD participants affected QoL [4]. Impairment in specific domains can inhibit everyday functioning and ADL [5], or results in less effective coping strategies [6]. Therefore, there may be an interaction between PD-MCI and other lifestyle variables; those with higher premorbid functioning may find the challenge of having MCI greater [24]. The strength of this study is in the large cohort of newly diagnosed PD participants with an age-matched control group. Limitations include its design that preceded the publication of the MDS PD-MCI criteria. We therefore used a modified version with an unequal numbers of tests per domain comprising only one test for visuospatial function and more than two tests for memory and executive function. This has implications for subtyping and may increase the frequency of impairments in these domains. The small number of participants in each subtype reduced statistical power, decreasing the sensitivity of our study to detect subtle differences between subtypes.

co

195

Un

194

rre

Fig. 2. Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment and PDQ-39 scores. Radar plot of mean PDQ-39 scores of each cut-off, (1 SD, 1.5 SD and 2 SD below normative values) for the most frequent subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Scale on graph is PDQ-39 score, where higher values indicate worse quality of life. A = Attention; E = Executive function; L = Language; M = Memory; V = Visuospatial function; PDQ39 = Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire.

In summary, PD-MCI is complex with severity of impairment, rather than subtype, affecting QoL. It could be inferred that the current MDS guidelines for subtyping PD-MCI may not be optimal, we identified 22 subtypes using the proposed guidelines. Numerous subtypes may be impractical in clinical settings; this could indicate that subtyping is of no real significance to patients and that their QoL is not affected by the specific nature of the impairment. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The ICICLE-PD study group would like to thank all participants for their contribution towards the study. ICICLE-PD was funded by Parkinson’s UK. This work was also supported by the UK NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Ageing and Age-Related Disease, UK NIHR Biomedical Research Unit in Lewy Body Dementia awards to the Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and the UK NIHR Biomedical Research Centre award to the University

215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223

224

225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233

R.A. Lawson et al. / MCI Subtypes and Quality of Life

237 238

[12]

[13] [14]

239

240

241

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST The authors have no conflict of interest to report.

[15]

REFERENCES [16]

246

[2]

247 248 249 250 251 252

[3]

253 254 255

[4]

256 257 258 259

[5]

260 261 262 263 264

[6]

265 266 267 268

[7]

269 270 271

[8]

272 273 274 275

[9]

276 277 278 279

[10]

280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290

[11]

[17]

[18]

dA

245

[19]

cte

244

rre

243

Leroi I, McDonald K, Pantula H, & Harbishettar V (2012) Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson Disease: Impact on Quality of Life, Disability, and Caregiver Burden. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol, 25, 208-214. Reginold W, Duff-Canning S, Meaney C, Armstrong MJ, Fox S, Rothberg B, Zadikoff C, Kennedy N, Gill D, Eslinger P, Marshall F, Mapstone M, Chou KL, Persad C, Litvan I, Mast B, Tang-Wai D, Lang AE, & Marras C (2013) Impact of Mild Cognitive Impairment on Health-Related Quality of Life in Parkinson’s Disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 36, 67-75. Goldman JG, Weis H, Stebbins G, Bernard B, & Goetz CG (2012) Clinical differences among mild cognitive impairment subtypes in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord, 27, 1129-1136. Klepac N, Trkulja V, Relja M, & Babic T (2008) Is quality of life in non-demented Parkinson’s disease patients related to cognitive performance? A clinic-based cross-sectional study. Eur J Neurol, 15, 128-133. Bronnick K, Ehrt U, Emre M, De Deyn PP, Wesnes K, Tekin S, & Aarsland D (2006) Attentional deficits affect activities of daily living in dementia-associated with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 77, 11361142. Kudlicka A, Clare L, & Hindle JV (2013) Quality of life, health status and caregiver burden in Parkinson’s disease: Relationship to executive functioning. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. Hindle JV, Petrelli A, Clare L, & Kalbe E (2013) Nonpharmacological enhancement of cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review. Mov Disord, 28, 1034-1049. Kehagia AA, Barker RA, & Robbins TW (2010) Neuropsychological and clinical heterogeneity of cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol, 9, 1200-1213. Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, & Lees AJ (1992) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: A clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 55, 181-184. Yarnall AJ, Breen DP, Duncan GW, Khoo TK, Coleman SY, Firbank MJ, Nombela C, Winder-Rhodes S, Evans JR, Rowe JB, Mollenhauer B, Kruse N, Hudson G, Chinnery PF, O’Brien JT, Robbins TW, Wesnes K, Brooks DJ, Barker RA, & Burn DJ (2013) Characterizing mild cognitive impairment in incident Parkinson disease: The ICICLE-PD Study. Neurology. Nicholl CG, Lynch S, Kelly CA, White L, Simpson PM, Wesnes KA, & Pitt BMN (1995) The cognitive drug research computerized assessment system in the evaluation of early dementia-is speed of the essence? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 10, 199-206.

co

[1]

Un

242

Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, McInnes L, & Rabbitt P (1994) Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB): A factor analytic study of a large sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia, 5, 266-281. Benton A (1968) Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia, 6, 53-60. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, & Rees L (1999) Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 14, 167-177. Ala TA, Hughes LF, Kyrouac GA, Ghobrial MW, & Elble RJ (2001) Pentagon copying is more impaired in dementia with Lewy bodies than in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 70, 483-488. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, B´edirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, & Chertkow H (2005) The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc, 53, 695-699. Mathias JL, Bowden SC, & Barrett-Woodbridge M (2007) Accuracy of the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) and National Adult Reading Test (NART) when estimating IQ in a healthy Australian sample. Aust Psychol, 42, 49-56. Litvan I, Goldman JG, Tr¨oster AI, Schmand BA, Weintraub D, Petersen RC, Mollenhauer B, Adler CH, Marder K, WilliamsGray CH, Aarsland D, Kulisevsky J, Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Burn DJ, Barker RA, & Emre M (2012) Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Movement Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Mov Disord, 27, 349356. Jenkinson C, Fitzpatrick RAY, Peto VIV, Greenhall R, & Hyman N (1997) The Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire (PDQ-39): Development and validation of a Parkinson’s disease summary index score. Age Ageing, 26, 353-357. Martinez-Martin P, Jeukens-Visser M, Lyons KE, RodriguezBlazquez C, Selai C, Siderowf A, Welsh M, Poewe W, Rascol O, Sampaio C, Stebbins GT, Goetz CG, & Schrag A (2011) Health-related quality-of-life scales in Parkinson’s disease: Critique and recommendations. Movement Disorders, 26, 2371-2380. Marras C, Armstrong MJ, Meaney CA, Fox S, Rothberg B, Reginold W, Tang-Wai DF, Gill D, Eslinger PJ, Zadikoff C, Kennedy N, Marshall FJ, Mapstone M, Chou KL, Persad C, Litvan I, Mast BT, Gerstenecker AT, Weintraub S, & DuffCanning S (2013) Measuring mild cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord, 28, 626-633. Goldman JG, Holden S, Bernard B, Ouyang B, Goetz CG, & Stebbins GT (2013) Defining optimal cutoff scores for cognitive impairment using Movement Disorder Society Task Force criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. Barone P, Antonini A, Colosimo C, Marconi R, Morgante L, Avarello TP, Bottacchi E, Cannas A, Ceravolo G, Ceravolo R, Cicarelli G, Gaglio RM, Giglia RM, Iemolo F, Manfredi M, Meco G, Nicoletti A, Pederzoli M, Petrone A, Pisani A, Pontieri FE, Quatrale R, Ramat S, Scala R, Volpe G, Zappulla S, Bentivoglio AR, Stocchi F, Trianni G, & Dotto PD (2009) The PRIAMO study: A multicenter assessment of nonmotor symptoms and their impact on quality of life in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord, 24, 1641-1649. Trigg R, Watts S, Jones R, & Tod A (2011) Predictors of quality of life ratings from persons with dementia: The role of insight. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 26, 83-91.

roo f

236

of Cambridge/Addenbrooke’s Hospital. DPB has been recipient of a Raymond and Beverly Sackler Studentship. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

or P

235

uth

234

[20]

[21]

[22]

[23]

[24]

5

291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355

363

or P

362

[26]

Janvin CC, Larsen JP, Aarsland D, & Hugdahl K (2006) Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Progression to dementia. Mov Disord, 21, 1343-1349.

uth

361

[27]

dA

360

cte

359

rre

358

Dalrymple-Alford JC, Livingston L, MacAskill MR, Graham C, Melzer TR, Porter RJ, Watts R, & Anderson TJ (2011) Characterizing mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord, 26, 629-636. Aarsland D, Brønnick K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB, Alves G, & For the Norwegian Park West Study G (2009) Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: The Norwegian ParkWest Study. Neurology, 72, 1121-1126.

co

[25]

357

Un

356

R.A. Lawson et al. / MCI Subtypes and Quality of Life

roo f

6

364 365 366

Uncorrected Author Proof

Uncorrected Author Proof. Journal of Parkinson's Disease xx (20xx) x–xx. DOI 10.3233/JPD-140390. IOS Press. 1. Quality of Life and Mild Cognitive. Impairment ...

204KB Sizes 1 Downloads 146 Views

Recommend Documents

Uncorrected Author Proof
used, but differences of experimental design and analysis tend to obscure their interpretation. ... capable of generating genuinely quantitative data, but. 53.

Uncorrected Proof
Feb 2, 2010 - The suitability of the proposed numerical scheme is tested against an analytical solution and the general performance of the stochastic model is ...

uncorrected proof
ANSWER ALL QUERIES ON PROOFS (Queries are attached as the last page of your proof.) §. List all corrections and send back via e-mail or post to the submitting editor as detailed in the covering e-mail, or mark all ...... Publications: College Park,

Uncorrected Proof
Jun 26, 2007 - of California Press, 1936) but paid to claims for a role for Platonic ... even guided by divinely ordained laws of motion, to produce all the ... 5 Stephen Menn, Descartes and Augustine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, ...

uncorrected proof
was whether people can be meaningfully differentiated by social ... Although people with a prevention focus can use risk-averse or .... subset of people suffering from social anxiety reporting ..... During the 3-month assessment period, 100%.

uncorrected proof
Jay Hooperb, Gregory Mertzc. 4 a Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, 2000 9th Avenue South, Southern Research Institute, Birmingham, ...

uncorrected proof
Internet Service Providers (ISPs) on the other hand, have to face a considerable ... complexity of setting up an e-mail server, and the virtually zero cost of sending.

uncorrected proof!
Secure international recognition as sovereign states with the dissolution of the Socialist .... kingdom of Carantania – including progressive legal rights for women! The ..... politics, does not have access to the company of eight Central European.

uncorrected proof
Dec 28, 2005 - Disk Used ... The rate of failure was not significantly affected by target ampli- ..... indicators (impulsion modality: reach time R, rate of failure F; ...

uncorrected proof
+598 2929 0106; fax: +598 2924 1906. Q1. ∗∗ Corresponding ... [12,13], and recently several papers have described the reduction. 24 of the carbonyl group by ...

uncorrected proof
social simulation methodology to sociologists of religion. 133 and religious studies researchers. But one wonders, would. 134 that purpose not be better served by introducing these. 135 researchers to a standard agent-based social simulation. 136 pac

uncorrected proof
indicated that growth decline and the degree of crown dieback were the .... 0.01 mm with a computer-compatible increment tree ....

uncorrected proof
3), we achieve a diacritic error rate of 5.1%, a segment error rate 8.5%, and a word error rate of ... Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ... bank corpus. ...... data extracted from LDC Arabic Treebank corpus, which is considered good ...

uncorrected proof
... the frequency of the voltage source is very large or very small as compare of the values ... 65 to mobile beams with springs of constants ki. ... mobile beam (m1) ...... is achieved when the variations of the variables and i go to zero as the tim

uncorrected proof
Jun 9, 2009 - The software component of a VR system manages the hardware ... VRE offers a number of advantages over in vivo or imaginal exposure. Firstly .... The NeuroVR Editor is built using a custom Graphical User Interface (GUI) for.

uncorrected proof
The data are collected from high- and low-proficiency pupils at each of the three grades in each ... good from poor readers. The linguistic ..... Analysis. We used NVivo, a software package for qualitative analysis, to process our data. In order.

uncorrected proof
(b) Lateral view. Focal plane at z ... (a) Pictorial view ..... 360 was presented. This transducer achieved a resolution of about. 361 ... A unified view of imag-. 376.

uncorrected proof
For high dimensional data sets the sample covariance matrix is usually ... The number of applied problems where such an estimate is required is large, e.g. ...

Uncorrected Proof
measurements, with particular focus on their applicability to landscape-scale ..... only multiple buried detectors and a control system for data storage (Tang et al.

uncorrected proof
+56 2 978 7392; fax: +56 2 272 7363. ... sent the ancestral T. cruzi lineages, and genetic recombination. 57 .... Intestinal contents were free of fresh blood,. 97.

uncorrected proof
Jul 6, 2007 - At the end of the paper I offer a new partitive account of plural definite descriptions ...... (18) Every man who had a credit card paid the bill with it;.

Uncorrected Proof
US Forest Service, Northern Research Station , 1831 Hwy 169 E., Grand Rapids , MN 55744 ..... approach, we hope to identify potential areas for improvement and thereby ... Southern Research Station, Asheville, NC ... Curtis , PS , Hanson , PJ , Bolst

uncorrected proof - Steve Borgatti
Earlier versions of this paper were presented at the 1991 NSF Conference on Measurement Theory and Networks. (Irvine, CA), 1992 annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association (San Francisco), and the 1993 Sunbelt. XII International Social

uncorrected proof
Jun 9, 2009 - Collins, R.L., Kashdan, T.B., & Gollnisch, G. (2003). The feasibility of using cellular phones ... CyberPsychology and Behavior, 9(6),. 711Б712.