Psychologia, 1998, 41, 199-202

A STATIC IMAGE OF A RAPIDLY MOVING PATTERN CAN BE PERCEIVED BY MAKING A SACCADE Yuji TAKEDA, Masayoshi NAGAI, Koji KAZAI and Akihiro YAGI Kwansei Gakuin University, Japan

Ten undergraduate and two graduate students were required to identify the moving patterns. The two patterns were made of triangles; one with apices pointing up and the other with apices pointing down. A given pattern was moved rapidly on a CRT and the subject was asked to watch and say whether the triangles pointed up or down. When they fixated a single point, the correct responses were near chance. When they made a saccade across the moving pattern, their detection performance was significantly better. The subjects reported that they could perceive the static image of the pattern when the saccade was made. This result might be explained by the hypothesis that saccadic suppression reduces excessive visual input.

Key words:

saccade, moving pattern, pattern identification, static image, saccadic suppression

If you ride on a bicycle, you may observe the following phenomenon: When you gaze at the tread of tire on the rolling wheel of your own bicycle, you may not recognize the tread pattern on the tire. However, you can recognize the tread pattern at the moment if you make a saccade from the tread to another location. Thus, the saccade enhanced the ability to resolve the static pattern on the rapidly moving surface in apparent contrasts with the well known phenomenon of saccadic suppression (e.g., the reduction of visual sensitivity during saccadic eye movements. Latour, 1962; Volkmann, 1962). Saccades also decrease the sensitivity to motion perception (Bridgeman, Hendry, & Stark, 1975; Shioiri & Cavanagh, 1989; Stark, Kong, Schwartz, Hendry, & Bridgeman, 1976). This decrease in sensitivity begins before the onset of a saccade and lasts after the offset of the saccade (Shioiri & Cavanagh, 1989; Volkmann, Schick, & Riggs, 1968; Wallach & Lewis, 1966). The mechanism underlining saccadic suppression is not clear. However, there are two major possible explanations. The first hypothesis is that a "corollary discharge» related to the motor act suppresses the visual sensitivity (Riggs, Merton, & Morton, 1974; Sperry, 1950; Volkmann, 1962; Volkmann, Riggs, White, & Moore, 1978). The second is that the suppression is a form of masking secondary to overlapping retinal images during saccades. During saccades, not only is retinal sensitivity reduced, but also visual information processing seems to be suppressed at higher levels. For example, it was demonstrated that the identification of degraded patterns or the processing of mental We thank Dr. Enoch Callaway for a review of a preliminary version of the manuscript. Correspondence: Yuji Takeda, Department of Psychology, Kwansei Gakuin University, Uegahara 1155, Nishinomiya, Hyogo 662-8501, Japan (E-mail: [email protected]). 199

TAKADA, NAGAI, KAZAI & YAGI

200

rotation was suppressed during saccades (Sanders & Houtmans, 1985; Sanders & Rath, 1991). These properties of saccadic suppression seem to contradict with the bicycle tire phenomenon. Saccadic suppression generally ca,uses the reduction of sensitivity to visual stimuli during the saccade. On the contrary, saccadic eye movements improve the pattern identification in the bicycle tire phenomenon. In fact, however, they may be related each other. The present experiment offers a quantitative laboratory analog of the bicycle tire phenomenon. However, in the bicycle tire phenomenon, the visual image of the rapidly moving pattern is present only at a pre-saccadic time. In this experiment, the retinal image of the moving pattern is present before during and after the saccade.

METHOD

Twelve undergraduate and graduate student volunteers (8 males and 4 females) from Kwansei Gakuin University participated in this experiment as subjects. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Stimuli were generated and displayed on a IWATSU ISEL AV-Tachistoscope system (IS-701A) equipped with P31 phosphor. The stimuli were presented in a 31.25 x 6.25° display field (500 x 100 pixels) with green dots (34.7 cd/m 2). Subjects observed all stimuli at a 50 cm viewing distance in darkness. Each trial began with a tone pip and a visual warning stimulus; the latter was a green square filling the whole display field upon which a black fixation point was present. Each fixation point was located 7.5 ° horizontally from the center of the display field, either to the left or to the right. After the warning stimulus, a scrolling stimulus composed of a 25 X 5 array of triangles (upward or downward) was presented with the green fixation points for Is (see Fig. 1). During presentation of the scrolling stimulus, elements of the stimulus scrolled upward 31.25°/s (1 pixell 2 ms). Following the scrolling stimulus, the green square filling the display field was displayed until subject responded. Subjects were required to judge whether the triangle elements of the stimulus were pointing up or down, and to press one of two keys using two alternative forced choice. After approximately 1s from a response, the warning stimulus of the next trials was presented. During a presentation of the scrolling stimulus, subjects were required to gaze on one of the fixation points under fixation conditions (right-fixation conditions and left-fixation conditions), and to make a

the array of ~s

the array of \l s

15° Fig. 1.

The scrolling stimuli. The scrolling stimulus which was a 25 x 5 array of triangles was scrolled upward 31.25°/s. A pair of "+"s were fixation points.

PERCEPTION BY A SACCADE

201

saccade from one of the fixation points to another one under saccade conditions (right-to-Ieft saccade conditions and left-to-right saccade conditions). The saccade was began after the onset of the scroll stimulus and ended before the offset of it. Subjects were instructed to restrain their eye blinks while the scrolling stimuli were being presented. Each session consisted of 10 practice and 20 real trials. Subjects were tested in two sessions for each conditions in random order. Eye movements and eye blinks were monitored using EOG electrodes. Subjects received feedback information when incorrect responses were made .

. RESULTS

Mean percentages of correct responses for the fixation conditions and the saccade conditions are shown in Table 1 (collapsed across two fixation points for the fixation conditions and two directions of the saccadic eye movements for the saccade conditions). No vertical eye movements were observed in any of the conditions. Correct responses under the saccade condition averaged 70.8%. Correct responses under the fixation condition averaged approximately 50%. Thus, the recognition of scrolled elements in the saccade conditions was less difficult than that in the fixation conditions. A t-test indicated a significant difference between the fixation conditions and the saccade conditions (t( 11) == 3.66, P< 0.01).

DISCUSSION

This experiment demonstrates that elements of a moving pattern that cannot be resolved by fixed gaze become recognizable when a saccade is made. The recording of the vertical EOG showed that no vertical movement occurred, when subjects made saccades horizontally. Thus, there were no visual pursuit movements that followed the pattern movement. Previous studies reported that the visual perception during saccades was inhibited (Latour, 1962; Volkmann, 1962). However, the present study demonstrated that saccadic eye movements could facilitate the identification for the rapidly moving patterns. This phenomenon could not be explained by any of previously proposed theories (e.g., Volkmann, 1986). We suggest that subjects could not recognize the rapidly moving elements in the fixation conditions because the rate of input exceeded information processing capacity. We will use the term "traffic" to indicate the rate of information being delivered, as that term is used in a similar way in discussing computer networks. Table 1.

Mean Percentages and SDs of Correct Responses SACCADE

FIXATION

mean

0.708

0.531

SD

0.137

0.071

Note: The data of the "SACCADE" was collapsed across two directions of saccade, and that of the "FIXATION" was collapsed across two fixation points. The statistical analysis shows significant difference.

202

TAKADA, NAGAI, KAZAI & YAGI

When traffic exceeds information processing capacity, processing probably does not go on beyond a low-level. Although we can make a spatial selection of information by attention, we cannot make a temporal selection of information. Thus temporal overload might simply prevent the system from operating properly. Improved recognition during a saccade may reflect a reduction of traffic and correction of overload by means of saccadic suppression. In the bicycle tire phenomenon, saccadic suppression might provide a 'snap shot' of the moving pattern by suppressing the weaker visual information from the period just preceding the saccade, such as a strobe light provides a kind of 'snap shot' of a moving object. In the bicycle tire phenomenon, there is no overloading information following the saccade, nor is there information on the nature of moving pattern. In the present experiment, the overloading input is present before, during and after the saccade. Under these conditions, it remains to be determined whether the snap shot involves data from before, during or after the saccade.

REFERENCES Bridgeman, B., Hendry, D., & Stark, L. 1975. Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movement. Vision Research, 15, 719-722. Latour, P. L. 1962. Visual threshold during eye movements. Vision Research, 2, 261-262. Riggs, L. A., Merton, P. A., & Morton, H. B. 1974. Suppression of visual phosphenes during saccadic eye movements. Vision Research, 14, 997-1011. Sanders A. F., & Houtmans, M.]. M. 1985. There is no central stimulus encoding during saccadic eye shift: A case against general parallel processing notions. Acta Psychologica, 60, 323-338. Sanders, A. F., & Rath, A. M. 1991. Perceptual processing and speed-accuracy trade-off. Acta Psychologica, 77, 275-291. Shioiri, S., & Cavanagh, P. 1989. Saccadic suppression of low-level motion. Vision Research, 29,915-928. Sperry, R. W. 1950. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 482-489. Stark, L., Kong, R., Schwartz, S., Hendry,D., & Bridgeman, B. 1976. Saccadic suppression of image displacement. Vision Research, 16, 1185-1187. V olkmann, F. C. 1962. Vision during voluntary saccadic eye movements. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 52, 571-578. Volkmann, F. C. 1986. Human visual suppression. Vision Research, 26, 1401-1416. Volkmann, F. C., Riggs, L. A., White, K. D., & Moore, R. K. 1978. Contrastsensitivityduringsaccadic eye movements. Vision Research, 18, 1193-1199. Volkmann, F. C., Schick, A. M., & Riggs, L. A. 1968. Time course of visual inhibition during voluntary saccades. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 58, 562-569. Wallach, H., & Lewis, C. 1966. The effect of abnormal displacement of the retinal image during eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 1, 25-29. (Manuscript received January 9, 1998; Revision accepted June 5, 1998)

a static image of a rapidly moving pattern can be ...

information processing seems to be suppressed at higher levels. For example, it was demonstrated .... shift: A case against general parallel processing notions.

318KB Sizes 0 Downloads 92 Views

Recommend Documents

can there be a feminist ethnography? - ScienceDirect
Synopsis-Many feminist scholars have tdenttfied ethnographtc methods as ideally smted to femi- nist research because us contextual, experiential approach to ...

Can There be a Theory of Law?
My aim in this book has been to further the understanding of law, coercion, and morality ...... The fact that if challenged to defend an action of mine I will have to ...

segmentation and tracking of static and moving objects ... - IEEE Xplore
ABSTRACT. In this paper we present a real-time object tracking system for monocular video sequences with static camera. The work flow is based on a pixel-based foreground detection system followed by foreground object tracking. The foreground detecti

POSITION OVERVIEW A rapidly expanding ... -
Work with latest technologies: As we continue to lead the industry, we require expertise across a broad spectrum of technologies including short and long-range wireless communication, video surveillance, lighting and HVAC automation, web development,

display-homes-in-canberra-can-be-a-good-selling-point.pdf
here for the taking and you will be amazed to see, how good they. are in what they do. Find them by going over the internet and you will be glad to have. made the right decision. The prices are right and so will be the. offered homes. Wait no more an

getting-suitable-fitness-promotional-products-can-be-a-good-move ...
getting-suitable-fitness-promotional-products-can-be-a-good-move.pdf. getting-suitable-fitness-promotional-products-can-be-a-good-move.pdf. Open. Extract.

No Adverse Inference Can Be Drawn Against A Candidate From ...
is not a case of concealment or misrepresentation on his part. and once the FIR registered against him, which in his view was. a case of false implication, has been quashed by this Court, he. Page 3 of 7. Main menu. Displaying No Adverse Inference Ca

Disrupt You? You, Too, Can Be a Disruptor ... - Automotive Digest
If you see yourself as being a business leader as your career path gains more ... The Editors at Automotive Information Network recommend that you read this ... and how you must get with all the technology around to disrupt and succeed.

Health promotion and education can be a more ...
Fukuoka University School of Medicine. I. n. m. CONTENTS .... nutritionists, school teachers and so on), volun .... Rl: "Hospital, administration, health center,.

User experience can be captured with a single ... Developers
metrics and track the ones that are important to your users' experience. MYTH 2. User experience can be captured with a single “representative user.” Real-world performance is highly variable due to differences in users' devices, network connecti

The Retinal Detachment Can Be Treated With a Professional.pdf ...
Find them and get your needs catered in no time. Page 1 of 1. The Retinal Detachment Can Be Treated With a Professional.pdf. The Retinal Detachment Can Be ...

Nonrigid Image Deformation Using Moving ... - Semantic Scholar
500×500). We compare our method to a state-of-the-art method which is modeled by rigid ... Schematic illustration of image deformation. Left: the original image.

Disrupt You? You, Too, Can Be a Disruptor ... - Automotive Digest
If you see yourself as being a business leader as your career path gains more ... and how you must get with all the technology around to disrupt and succeed.

(>
(An book reader may be a software application to be used over a laptop, like. Microsoft's totally free Reader software, or a book-sized laptop that is certainly ...

Presuppositions Can Be Disruptors Too: A Case ...
I don't think [John]F read something interesting too. ... A function f of type is Strawson-DE iff for all ..... Intervention data provide a way to tease apart local.

You Can Be a Stock Market Genius
... reviews and industry blogs 1 I celebrate myself and sing myself And what I assume you shall assume For every atom belonging to me as good belongs to you I ...

The Retinal Detachment Can Be Treated With a Professional.pdf ...
Find them and get your needs catered in no time. Page 1 of 1. The Retinal Detachment Can Be Treated With a Professional.pdf. The Retinal Detachment Can Be ...

Download Full PDF Can a City Be Sustainable?
... copy Tabtight professional free when you need it VPN service In this chapter ... it up and running again soon Newsletter with articles on current research finds ...

Nonrigid Image Deformation Using Moving ... - Semantic Scholar
To illustrate, consider Fig. 1 where we are given an image of Burning. Candle and we aim to deform its flame. To this end, we first choose a set of control points, ...