A Study of Learner’s Mental Profile in Different Categories of Tasks Ramla Ghali and Claude Frasson Département d’informatique et de recherche opérationnelle Université de Montréal 2920 Chemin de la Tour, Montréal Québec, Canada, H3C 3J7 {ghaliram, frasson}@iro.umontreal.ca

Abstract: Adapting learning according to the learner’s profile is an essential characteristic of Intelligent Tutoring Systems. Several studies have proposed different approaches to that aim. These methods are based mainly on the learner’s individual traits, performances and emotions. However, few studies are interested to consider the learner’s behavior variation according to the nature and type of the presented task. In this paper, we have focused on the learner’s mental profile based on electroencephalogram signals analysis and classification, in different cognitive tasks. These tasks are composed of three main categories (memory, concentration and reasoning) and organized according to a varying difficulty level, from easiest to hardest. Primary results showed that learner’s performance depends on the category of a task. Furthermore, some mental states (engagement and workload) are correlated with the cognitive task category. Keywords: Memory cognitive tasks, Concentration cognitive tasks, reasoning cognitive tasks, EEG, engagement, workload.

1

Introduction

In Intelligent Tutoring Systems (ITS) adapting learning according to learner’s profile is a fundamental criterion of intelligence. In fact, several researchers have suggested to spend more effort in defining a precise architecture of learner’s profile and to adapt learning according to the different components of this profile [8, 9]. Indeed, defining a precise and a steady profile for a learner is very challenging for many reasons. Among these reasons, we all know that learning is a complex process. It can indeed be influenced by several factors; external factors related to the environment (interface quality, course organization, etc.) and internal factors related to the learner (current emotions while learning, learner’s motivation, learner’s engagement on a task, etc.). All these factors could have a direct influence on learner’s performance and consequently learning success. Thus, many studies from different disciplines (artificial intelligence, human computer interaction, cognition and neuroscience) have focused on detecting and assessing users’ mental profile based on different approaches, and more specifically electroencephalogram (EEG) signals analysis and classification [3, 4, 6]. The major part of these systems was based on two fundamental mental metrics, namely, mental workload and mental engagement. Mental workload refers to the por-

tion of operator information processing capacity or resource that is actually required to meet system demands [5]. Mental engagement is related to the level of mental vigilance, attention and alertness during the task. However, most of these approaches do take into consideration neither the brain specificities nor the type of cognitive tasks involved. In this paper we aim at assessing learner’s mental states variation in different categories of a set of cognitive tasks that we developed. These mental states are issued from signals treatment and analysis provided by the B-alert software [1]. Thus, we formulate the following hypothesis: 1) we think that the type of a task has an impact on the learner’s performance, 2) the type of a task has an influence on learner’s mental states, showing more or less cognitive workload and engagement.

2

Related Work

To date several studies were conducted to detect, assess and predict learner’s states evolution during interacting with e-learning environments which can influence learning. Among these states, we can quote learner’s emotions, motivation, behavior, etc. For example, to detect if a student is engaged or not in a task, Beck [2] has built a student model that is based on 3 parameters (response time, question difficulty and nature of answer (correct or not)). This model can trace learner’s engagement by calculating a probability based on learner’s previous performance and behavior. Besides, Johns and his colleagues [7] have used dichotomous Item Response Theory (IRT) models to estimate student’s proficiency in answering multiple choice questions. These approaches are mainly based on learners’ statistics while interacting with a system. On the other side, some researchers have considered data issued from certain physiological sensors, more specifically electroencephalogram (EEG), to detect learner’s states of engagement and disengagement. For example, Pope [10] has developed an EEG engagement index based on brainwave band power spectral densities and applied in a closed-loop system to modulate task allocation. Performance improvement was reported using this engagement index for task allocation mode (manual or automated).This index has been even effective to detect learner’s attention and vigilance in learning tasks [4]. Furthermore, Stevens and al [11] explored the feasibility of monitoring EEG indices of engagement and workload acquired and measured during performance of cognitive tests. Results showed an increase of engagement and workload during the encoding period of verbal and image learning and memory tests compared with the recognition period. They showed also that workload increased linearly with the level of difficulty. Moreover, Galan and Beal [6] evaluated positively the use of EEG for estimating attention and cognitive load (Workload) during math problems. They could be used for predicting learner’s success or failure by a combination of engagement and workload measures established by Stevens and his colleagues. In the same vein, we proposed in this work to adopt a sensor-based approach in order to detect learner’s mental evolution in different categories of cognitive tasks. We also used the last two metrics proposed by Stevens and al [11] to track learner’s mental states evolution. However, we think that these two metrics depend not only on the difficulty of a proposed task but also on the nature and the type of this task. In order to prove this assumption, we developed a set of different categories of cognitive

tasks. We conducted also, an experiment to gather learner’s EEG data. Our main goal from the primary experiment was to study learner’s mental state evolution (essentially engagement and workload) according to the nature of a proposed task, using the BAlert software [1]. In the following, we will describe briefly these cognitive tasks.

3

Cognitive Tasks Categories

We developed a set of 2D cognitive tasks for studying the learners’ performances and their brain behavior. This set contains three categories of tasks (memory, concentration and reasoning) and three difficulty levels (easy, medium and hard) presented in an ascending order. The user can choose freely each time the category and the task to do but he has to complete all the tasks at least once. So, the tasks are ordered differently according to the user’s choice and are grouped by task category. Each category includes two to three subcategories of different tasks. In what follows, we will present these tasks ordered by category name. 3.1

Memory

The following category is mainly based on the popular task of Digit Span. In this task, we familiarize the learner with a series of numbers and ask him to remember and type them afterwards. We implemented two versions of this task: Forward Digit span (FDS) when it comes to typing the numbers retained in the same order in which they appeared on the screen and Backward Digit Span (BDS) when it comes to type numbers in the reverse order of their apparition. Each version has 6 difficulty levels ordered from easiest (L1) to hardest (L6). 3.2

Concentration

This category contains two subcategories of concentration tasks: Feature Match and Rotations described below. 3.2.1 Feature Match (FM) This task consists in identifying whether the two images appearing on the screen are identical or not according to their forms, numbers and colors. It has also six difficulty levels (ranging from L1 to L6) which vary in its geometrical number and forms (see figure 1).

Figure 1 Example of Feature Match for level 6

3.2.2 Rotations (RT) This task is similar to the previous task. It has five difficulty levels which vary depending on the complexity of the image content (number of shapes). It consists in identifying whether two images match or not in doing their rotations. 3.3

Reasoning

This category contains three sets of reasoning tasks: Arithmetic addition, Odd One Out, and Intuitive reasoning. 3.3.1 Arithmetic Addition (AA) In this task, we kindly ask the learner to add two variable numbers. Like any other task, this one has three levels of difficulty, where in each level we vary the number of digits to add going from 2 until 4. 3.3.2 Odd One Out (OO) This task has three difficulty levels. For each difficulty level, it has a fixed series of images. Every series has a certain correspondence between images (color, form, number, etc.) and one odd one out which is different according to one or more characteristics (see figure 2). Thus, the learner has to identify each time the odd one out image.

Figure 2 Example of Odd One Out task 3.3.3 Intuitive Reasoning (IR) This task has three levels of difficulty (varying according a time constraint: unlimited, 1mn and 30s) and 15 series in total; where every level contains 5 series of exercises. Unlike other tasks, this task is based on intuitive or analogical reasoning (see figure3).

Figure 3 Example of a series of intuitive reasoning task

4

Experiment

In order to study learners’ mental states variation in different tasks category, twenty participants (9 women and 11 men, mean age=28, standard deviation=4.67) were invited to play our cognitive tasks. This study lasted about 2 hours, mainly distributed

into three steps: (1) Initially, we installed the B-Alert X10 headset on the participant to set up the EEG, (2) the participant is invited to do 3 tasks of baseline defined by the manufacturers of this headset [14] to establish a classification of mental states, (3) the participant is finally invited to play our set of cognitive tasks which composed of 3 categories as described above. During all the experiment, electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded by using a Wi-Fi cap with a two linked mastoid references. 9 sensors (F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Poz and P4) were placed on the participant’s head using the international 10-20 system. EEG was sampled at a rate of 256 Hz and converted to Power spectral densities (Alpha, Beta, Theta and Sigma). EEG was processed by the B-Alert software [1]. This software allows us to obtain a real time classification of certain mental states (sleep Onset, Distraction, Low engagement, High Engagement and High workload). From this set, we selected in this study the Workload and Engagement states. Thus, we synchronized the EEG mental states of Engagement and Workload with all the categories of cognitive tasks developed. These mental states are labeled and synchronized with task category by using data (corresponding system time) from learner’s log files (during accomplishing the cognitive tasks) and B-Alert software (EEG mental states).

5

Statistical Results

We recall that in this work, we want to consider the following points: (1) examine the learner’s performance variation depending on the category of task, (2) study the variations of mental states depending on the category of task. 5.1

Learners’ performance and category of task

First, we computed for each category and for each task, the average of the learners’ scores, as well as their standard deviation (see table 1). The scores of tasks are calculated as follows: Each correct answer is worth 1 point and each response incorrect is 0. Then, for each task, we calculated the percentage of total score achieved in the task (TST: Total Score in Task), as well as the percentage of total score achieved in the task category (TSTC: Total Score in Task category). Table 1 Distribution of scores between tasks Task Category Memory

Mean (SD) of TSTC 65.66 (2.46)

Concentration

81.90 (1.35)

Reasoning

58.14 (2.52)

Task Name FDS BDS FM RT AA OO RI

Mean (SD) of TST 64.61 (3.14) 67.64 (3.96) 82.20 (1.70) 81.32 (2.24) 67.79 (4.47) 63.03 (5.85) 49.47 (2.97)

From this table, we can notice that the category of concentration has the best percentage of scores for all the learners, as well as Feature Match task. However, reasoning category is ranked the last. This can be explained by the fact that the category

of task could have an impact on learner’s performance. In fact, concentration tasks do not require much mental workload comparing to reasoning tasks that require a lot of concentration, memory work, arithmetic calculation, etc. To confirm this hypothesis, we conducted a one way ANOVA test after checking the normal distribution of scores for each category (using SPSS Q-QPlot). This test allows us to obtain a very significant result (F(2,477)=31.1,p=0.000*). So, we can assume that learner’s performance depends on the category of task. 5.2

Learner’s mental profile and category of task

To analyze the relationship between mental states and task category, we conduct first a descriptive analysis by comparing the distribution of engagement, workload and a category of task (see table 2). Task Category Memory Concentration Reasoning

Min 33.06 36.14 33.34

Engagement (%) Max Mean (SD) 80.11 59.44(1.09) 92.15 61.14(1.47) 98.70 64.86(1.84)

Min 53.13 21.43 26.02

Workload (%) Max Mean (SD) 84.53 69.36(0.75) 77.66 60.78(1.2) 79.23 66.09(1.21)

From this table, we can notice that memory task is the most difficult task. It has the highest workload (69.36%) comparing to other tasks. However, concentration task is the easiest one. Furthermore, we can see that reasoning tasks are the most engaged tasks. This result can be explained by the fact that reasoning tasks are challenging and increase learners’ interest and involvement. Second, we used three one way ANOVA tests. For the mental state of workload, results are very significant (F(2,224)=18.33, p=0.000*). Indeed, for the mental state of engagement, results are also significant (F(2,224)=3.32, p=0.04*). Furthermore, we obtained an average correlation between workload and engagement states (R=0.4, p=0.00*). So, we can conclude that workload and engagement states depend on task category. This result is very consistent since learner’s concentration and mental activity increases according to the nature of proposed task; more the nature or category of the task is interesting, more he is engaged on the task. Moreover, more the learner is engaged on the task, more he tries to reason and learn, so his workload increases.

6

Conclusion

In the present study we have shown that both learner’s performance and mental profile (which is composed of engagement and workload states) depend on the category of cognitive task involved. So, we can confirm that the type or the nature of a proposed task in learning has a significant impact on learner’s mental states and consequently his performance. This finding leads us to take into consideration learner’s cognitive capacity before proposing a task and then try to adapt learning depending to the evolution of some selected outputs issued from EEG signal. More specifically, our future work will focus on real time learner’s adaptation according the learner’s mental states variation and the type of proposed task.

Acknowledgments We acknowledge the Fonds Québécois de la Recherche sur la Nature et les Technologies (FQRNT), CRSH (LEADS project) and NSERC for funding this work.

References 1. Advanced Brain Monitoring, B-Alert X10, 2013. http://advancedbrainmonitoring.com/xseries/x10/. 2. Beck, J.: engagement tracing: using response times to model student disengagement. The international Conference on Artificial Intelligence in education, 88-95 (2005). 3. Berka, C., Levendowski, D.J. et al.: Real-Time Analysis of EEG Indexes of Alertness, Cognition, and Memory Acquired With a Wireless EEG Headset. International Journal of HCI, 17, 151 - 170 (2004) 4. Chaouachi, M., Chalfoun, P., Jraidi, I., Frasson, C. : Affect and mental engagement : Towards adapatabilityfor intelligent systems, FLAIRS, 355-361 (2010). 5. Eggemeier, F.T., Wilson, G.F. et al.: Workload assessment in multi-task environments. Multiple task performance. D.L. Damos. London, GB, Taylor & Francis, Ltd.: 207-216 (1991). 6. Galon, F., & Beal, C. R.: EEG estimates of engagement and cognitive workload predict math problem solving outcomes. Proceedings of UMAP, Montreal Canada (2012) 7. Johns, J., Mahadevan, S., Woolf. B.: Estimating Student's Proficiency using an Item Response Theory Model, International Conference on Intelligent Tutoring System (2006). 8. Murray, T. Authoring intelligent tutoring systems: An analysis of the state of the art. Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 10, 98-129 (1999). 9. Oppermann, R., Rasher, R. Adaptability and adaptivity in learning systems. Knowledge Transfer (1997). 10. Pope, A.T., Bogart, E.H., Bartolome, D.S.: Biocybernetic system evaluates indices of operator engagement in automated task. Biological Psychology 40, 187-195 (1995) 11. Stevens, R., Galloway, T., Berka, C.: EEG-Related Changes in Cognitive Workload, Engagement and Distraction as Students Acquire Problem Solving Skills. In: Conati, C., McCoy, K., Paliouras, G. (eds.) User Modeling 2007, vol. 4511. pp. 187-196. Springer Berlin / Heidelberg (2007)

A Study of Learner's Mental Profile in Different ...

... de la Tour, Montréal. Québec, Canada, H3C 3J7 ... during the encoding period of verbal and image learning and memory tests compared with the recognition ...

97KB Sizes 2 Downloads 204 Views

Recommend Documents

Epub Different Brains, Different Learners: How to ...
Read PDF Different Brains, Different Learners: How to Reach the Hard to Reach, ... The author demonstrates how to effectively guide students with learning ... Different Brains, Different Learners: How to Reach the Hard to Reach For ios by }.

pDF Mental Health in Schools: Engaging Learners ...
... make a differenceGain the knowledge you need to develop or improve your school ... Preventing Problems, and Improving Schools For android by Howard S.

Friends of Different Learners News and Notes November 2016..pdf ...
http://www.fpcbirmingham.org/. JOIN NOW FOR 2016-17. Page 3 of 4. Friends of Different Learners News and Notes November 2016..pdf. Friends of Different ...

Mental imagery generation in different modalities ...
labelled using the Talairach Daemon Database. Within anatomical regions of ... imagery can involve the recruitment of a multi-modal network, reflecting a ...

A Comparative Study of Different Presentation ...
Doctor Aiguader 80, 08003 Barcelona, Spain. david.andreu@ upf.edu. ..... Blocking of free binding sites was performed ... ducted using Microsoft Excel software.

A comparative study of different feature sets for ...
8. (b). Fig 6. An illustration for computation of the row wise longest–run feature. .... [1] A. Amin, “Off-line Arabic Character Recognition: the State of the Art”, Pattern ...

A comparative study of different feature sets for ...
On experimentation with a database of. 3000 samples, the .... respectively. Once a feature set is fixed up, it is left with the design of a mapping (δ) as follows:.

Motor Imagery in Mental Rotation: An fMRI Study - ScienceDirect.com
ferent sorts of stimulus pairs, viz. pictures of hands and pictures of tools, which ... aging data replicate classic areas of activation in men- tal rotation for hands and ...

Individual Performance Profile RN Mental Health ...
BSN. Test Date: 11/26/2012. # of Questions: 60. Individual Score: 91.7%. Practice Time: ... Score. Description. Foundational Thinking in Nursing (RN 2010). 16.

Study of Data Warehouse Modeling and its different design ... - IJRIT
Requirement gathering can happen as Joint Application Development (JAD) ... the users with applications and data access tools that are appropriate for their ...

Study of Data Warehouse Modeling and its different design approaches
Though multidimensional data models and star schema are relevant for warehouse .... Several efficient multidimensional data structures such as dwarfs [47, 48], ...

Cohort Profile: The Malawi Longitudinal Study of ...
Mar 16, 2014 - Philip Anglewicz,3 Iliana V Kohler,4 Rebecca L Thornton,5 ... established in 1998 to study social network influences on fertility behaviours and ...

A Different Perspective
Apr 28, 2013 - promises and the power of God. ... change anything by worrying? • Can God change anything by His power? ... They don't plant or harvest or.

Profile of a Graduate.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. Profile of a ...

a different shade of blue.pdf
a different shade of blue.pdf. a different shade of blue.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu. Displaying a different shade of blue.pdf. Page 1 of 2.

A Different Perspective
Apr 28, 2013 - with thanksgiving, present your requests to God. ... Trusting that He will make all things right .... and lean not on your own understanding;.

Salesian System of Education: a Psychological Profile
invitation of Jesus may have been the driving force that spurred Don Bosco to ..... recovery, the recognition and acceptance of that which before was confused ...

Salesian System of Education: a Psychological Profile
THE SALESIAN SYSTEM OF EDUCATION. A PSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE. Godfrey D'Sa, SDB. Divyadaan: Journal of Philosophy and Education 17/1 (2006) ...