Public Opinion Quarterly Advance Access published August 21, 2008 Public Opinion Quarterly 2008, pp. 1–9

AFFIRMATIVE ACTION PROGRAMS FOR WOMEN AND MINORITIES EXPRESSED SUPPORT AFFECTED BY QUESTION ORDER DAVID C. WILSON DAVID W. MOORE PATRICK F. MCKAY DEREK R. AVERY

Abstract This paper examines the effects of question ordering on support for affirmative action (AA) for women and racial minorities. Public opinion surveys show that the public expresses greater support for gender-targeted AA than for race-targeted AA, but no research has addressed the extent to which expressed support for one group influences expressed support toward the other. We examine this question by testing the effects of question order on reported support for race and gender targets. Using a split-ballot experimental design, national data show that presenting gender-targeted AA before race-targeted AA increases reported support for the racial target, and conversely, presenting racetargeted AA first decreases reported support for gender-targeted AA. The order effects imply that race still has a significant impact on these considerations. The study finds exceptions to this rule among blacks and liberals, whose support for AA programs is unaffected by the order in which the programs are considered.

is with the Department of Political Science and International Relations, University of Delaware, 347 Smith Hall, Newark, DE 19716, USA. PATRICK F. MCKAY is with the School of Management and Labor Relations, Rutgers, the State University of New Jersey, Janice H. Levin Building Room 207, 94 Rockafeller Road, Piscataway, NJ 08854, USA. DAVID W. MOORE is with the University of New Hampshire, The Carsey Institute, 73 Main Street, Huddleston Hall, Durham, NH 03824, USA. DEREK R. AVERY is with the Department of Psychology and Department of Management, University of Houston, 126 Heyne Building, Houston, TX 77004, USA. The authors would like to thank Jeff Jones and the Gallup Organization for providing valuable information about the data, and also Lawrence Bobo, Nancy Mathiowetz, and the anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions. Address correspondence to David C. Wilson; e-mail: [email protected]. DAVID C . WILSON

doi:10.1093/poq/nfn031  C The Author 2008. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the American Association for Public Opinion Research. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected]

2

Wilson et al.

Introduction Numerous studies have shown that Americans express more positive opinions about affirmative action (AA) policies targeting women than those targeting racial minorities (Bobo and Kluegel 1993; Kravitz and Platania 1993; Sniderman and Piazza 1993; Moore 1995; Steeh and Krysan 1996; Strolovitch 1998; Sidanius et al. 2000). In fact, among white samples in general, support for AA is lower when the program or policy is framed as benefitting blacks (Bobo and Kluegel 1993; Moore 1995; Clayton 1996). Few, if any studies, however, have examined how people’s views of one targeted beneficiary group (e.g., race) are affected by views of the other (e.g., gender). A June 12–18, 2003 Gallup survey focusing on the state of race relations in the United States provides an opportunity to examine this matter. It included a question-order experiment, in which half the sample was asked for their views on AA programs for women and then their views on AA programs for minorities, while the other half of the sample was presented the questions in reverse order (Moore 2003). The experiment allows us to determine people’s views on each type of AA program when considered in isolation and in the context of the other.

Data The poll was based on telephone interviews with 1,385 adults (aged 18 years and older) from the contiguous 48 states, including an oversample of blacks.1 The sample included 266 black respondents, 1,005 white respondents (477 white men and 528 white women), and 114 respondents other than black or white. Participants were identified through random digit dialing (RDD) and were assigned randomly to interviewers (i.e., no race of interviewer matching). The questionnaires, Form A and Form B, were assigned randomly to respondents. Form A (N = 687; 49.6 percent) presented a question about gender-based policies first and race-based policies second, while Form B (N = 698; 50.4 percent) posed the questions in the opposite sequence.2 1. The AAPOR response rate (RR3) for the survey was 21 percent (AAPOR 2006). Due to the lower response rate, we conducted one-sample chi-square tests comparing the population proportion to the unweighted sample proportions on the following demographics: race, age, gender, and education. The sample data were found to be representative of blacks and whites [χ 2 = 0.53(1), n.s.], and gender [χ 2 = 0.35(1), n.s.]; however, the mean age (47 years) and the percentage of persons with a college degree (36 percent) are respectively higher (age: t = 9.5, p < .01, and college education: χ 2 = 157 (1), p < .01) than those reported in the 2000 Census. 2. There was successful randomization of questionnaires (i.e., ballots) through the split-ballot (i.e., form) experimental design, even when controlling for race, gender, and their interaction. Overall, 49 percent (n = 1,183) of respondents were interviewed using form A (Women: Racial Minorities), and 51% (1,208) were interviewed using form B (Racial Minorities: Women). A one-sample chisquare (χ 2 = 0.26, df = 1, n.s.) indicated no significant differences in their real and hypothesized distributions (i.e., a good fit). Pearson chi-square statistics for gender (χ 2 = 0.48, df = 1, n.s.),

Question Order Effect on Support for Affirmative Action

3

Table 1. Affirmative Action Programs for Women

Favor Oppose Unsure (no opinion) Total

Question asked first (%)

Question asked after similar question about racial minorities (%)

63 29 8 100 (n = 687)

57 37 6 100 (n = 698)

NOTES.—χ 2 = 16 (df = 2); p < .01.

Table 2. Affirmative Action Programs for Racial Minorities

Favor Oppose Unsure (no opinion) Total

Question asked first (%)

Question asked after a similar question about women (%)

50 43 7 100 (n = 698)

57 36 7 100 (n = 687)

NOTES.—χ 2 = 15 (df = 2), p < .01.

The two questions are worded as follows: Do you generally favor or oppose affirmative action programs for racial minorities? Do you generally favor or oppose affirmative action programs for women?

Findings The results in Tables 1, 2, and 3 show that question order makes a significant difference in the measured responses. Table 1 presents the results for the gender AA question. Respondents who were asked about AA for women first were considerably more likely to favor than to oppose it (63 percent versus 29 percent – a 34 percentage point difference). But the other half of the sample, asked the same question after a similar question about racial minorities, favored it by a much smaller margin: 57 percent to 37 percent, a 20-point difference.3 A reverse pattern, shown in Table 2, is found on the questions about racial minorities. When the question about racial minorities is asked first, race (blacks and whites only) (χ 2 = 0.78, df = 1, n.s.), and the gender by race interaction (χ 2 = 1.26, df = 1, n.s.) also showed no significant differences across form, indicating that the random assignment on the selected demographics was successful. 3. The statistical tests are based on the unweighted samples sizes of the data; thus, the reported percentages reflect the actual sample’s response distributions across the experimental treatment.

4

Wilson et al.

Table 3. Support for Affirmative Action Compared by Question Order

Question order Mentioned first–noncomparative context Mentioned second– comparative context

Percent favor AA programs for women

Percent favor AA Gap in favor of programs for racial AA programs minorities for women ∗∗

63 (687)

50 (698)

+13

57 (698)

57 (687)

0

z 4.92

0.0

NOTES.—Analysis includes all respondents. Numbers in parentheses are sample sizes for the question (in column) for a given question order (in row); ∗∗ p < .01.

Americans are evenly split with 50 percent in favor and 50 percent either opposed (43 percent) or unsure (7 percent). But when respondents were asked about AA for women first, they expressed majority support for AA for racial minorities – by 57 percent to 36 percent (with 7 percent unsure). The theoretical explanation for this shift in views is what Schuman and Presser (1981, p. 28; also see Moore 2002, pp. 82–3, for an operational definition) term “consistency” effects. When asked first about either type of AA program before being asked about the other type (what Moore termed a “noncomparative context”), people make their evaluations based on whatever criteria they bring to mind. But when asked about the second type of AA program after having been asked about the first type (a “comparative context”), many people will make their evaluation of the second type of AA program in comparison with their evaluation of the first. Thus, many respondents who first said they support AA programs for women then feel obligated (when asked the second question) to express support for AA programs targeted to racial minorities. Similarly, people who first said they oppose AA for racial minorities are then less inclined to turn around and support it for women (when the latter question is asked second). The comparative context thus elicits a “norm of reciprocity” (Schuman and Presser 1981, p. 28) leading to more consistent expressions of support for each type of AA program than are found in the noncomparative context. The results of this survey show just this pattern. As shown in Table 3 below, in the noncomparative context, people are more likely to support AA programs for women (63 percent) than for racial minorities (50 percent) – a statistically significant 13-point difference.4 But in the comparative context, people 4. Because the focus of this study is on the factors that affect “support” for AA, in the subsequent analysis we dichotomized all responses, classifying people who said they opposed AA, as well as those who were not sure (i.e., expressed no opinion), as nonsupporters. Only people who said they favored AA are classified as supporters.

Question Order Effect on Support for Affirmative Action

5

Table 4. Support for Affirmative Action Compared by Question Order and Race Percent favor AA programs for women Race Blacks

Whites

Question order Mentioned first– noncomparative context Mentioned second –comparative context Mentioned first– noncomparative context Mentioned second – comparative context

Percent favor Gap in favor of AA programs AA programs for racial for women minorities

z

75 (131)

71 (135)

+4

0.73

76 (135)

70 (131)

+6

1.10

59 (502)

44 (503)

+15∗∗

4.81

53 (503)

52 (502)

+1

0.32

NOTES.—Numbers in parentheses are sample sizes for the question (in column) for a given question order (in row); ∗∗ p < .01.

are equally likely to support the race-based as the gender-based programs (57 percent each). The results suggest that for the American public as a whole, support for one type of AA program is indeed affected by whether that program is considered by itself or in the context of both types of AA programs. Further analysis, presented in Table 4, finds that the pattern of consistency effects shown for the public as a whole is not found among blacks. This group of respondents supports both race-based and gender-based AA programs at about the same level, whether each is considered by itself or in the context of the other. The shift in support is found only among whites. Also in Table 4, note that in the noncomparative context, 75 percent of blacks support AA programs for women and 71 percent support AA programs for minorities. In the comparative context, the respective percentages are 76 percent and 70 percent – essentially the same as in the comparative context. For whites, the pattern is quite different and of course mirrors the pattern found for the sample as a whole (since white respondents make up 78 percent of the unweighted sample). Among all whites, 59 percent support AA programs for women in the noncomparative context, while only 44 percent support AA programs for racial minorities – a 15-point gap. In the comparative context, the respective support is 53 percent and 52 percent, respectively, just a one-point difference.

6

Wilson et al.

Table 5. Support for Affirmative Action Compared by Question Order and Sex Percent favor AA programs for women Sex Males

Females

Percent favor Gap in favor of AA programs AA programs for racial for women minorities

Question order Mentioned first – noncomparative context Mentioned second – comparative context Mentioned first– noncomparative context Mentioned second – comparative context

z ∗∗

56 (235)

39 (242)

+17

47 (242)

49 (235)

−2

0.32

62 (267)

49 (261)

+13∗∗

3.02

59 (261)

55 (267)

+4

0.93

3.76

NOTES.—Analysis includes only white respondents. Numbers in parentheses are sample sizes for the question (in column) for a given question order (in row); ∗∗ p < .01.

This pattern of consistency effects is found among both white men and white women, though it is more pronounced among men than women. The results presented in Table 5 show that in the noncomparative context, white men are more likely to support AA for women than for racial minorities by 17 percentage points (56 percent versus 39 percent). In the comparative context, support for each type of AA program is about the same (47 percent and 49 percent). Among white women, support for female-based AA in the noncomparative context is greater than for race-based AA by 13 points (62 percent versus 49 percent). In the comparative context, the gap decreases to four points (59 percent versus 55 percent). Thus, for men, there is a 19-point change in the gap (from 17 points in favor of AA for women to −2 points), while for women there is just a 9-point change (from 13 points in favor of AA for women to 4 points). These results suggest that white men are more likely than white women to be affected by whether each type of AA program is considered in isolation or in context of the other type. One other finding of the study shows that ideology – like race – has an interactive effect with question order (Table 6). Among self-described liberals, support for AA programs is essentially constant, whether each type of program is considered alone or in a comparative context. Among moderates and conservatives, however, large consistency effects are found. For moderates, preference for female-based AA over minorities-based AA goes from 16 points in the

Question Order Effect on Support for Affirmative Action

7

Table 6. Support for Affirmative Action Compared by Question Order and Ideology

Political ideology Liberals

Question order

Mentioned first – noncomparative context Mentioned second – comparative context Moderates Mentioned first – noncomparative context Mentioned second – comparative context Conservatives Mentioned first noncomparative context Mentioned second – comparative context

Percent favor Percent favor AA AA programs for for racial women minorities

Gap in favor of AA programs for women

z

64 (89)

65 (108)

−1

0.14

68 (108)

63 (89)

+5

0.73

62 (194)

46 (195)

+16∗∗

3.20

53 (195)

56 (194)

−3

0.59

55 (214)

32 (199)

+23∗∗

4.84

45 (199)

45 (214)

0

0.0

NOTES.—Analysis includes only white respondents. Numbers in parentheses are sample sizes for the question (in column) for a given question order (in row); ∗∗ p < .01.

noncomparative context (62 percent versus 46 percent) to about even (53 percent versus 56 percent) in the comparative context. And conservatives’ preference for AA for women over AA for minorities moves from a gap of 23 points (55 percent versus 32 percent respectively) in the noncomparative context to completely even (45 percent support for each) in the comparative context. In both cases, the differences are statistically significant.

Conclusions While racial and ethnic minorities have benefited greatly from AA policies, statistically, white women have benefited more than any other group (Hill 1983; Wise 1998; Crosby et al. 2003). Yet, when AA programs are debated, they commonly are framed in terms of racial preference or minority quotas (Sniderman and Piazza 1993; Steeh and Krysan 1996; Swain 2001; Crosby, Iyer, and Sincharoen 2006). Our findings reinforce the suggestion that race is not the most favorable frame for discussing AA programs (Fine 1992; Bobo

8

Wilson et al.

and Kluegel 1993; Steeh and Krysan 1996). When discussions about AA focus only on racial minorities, the expressed support for this program is about evenly divided. Moreover, if the focus is only on AA for women, public support is substantially higher. Finally, if the context of the debate is about AA for both women and minorities, support is compromised as many people try to be consistent – giving greater support to the program for minorities because they support it for women or offering greater opposition to the program for women because they oppose it for minorities. Our findings also suggest that the expressed support of AA among blacks and liberals is an exception to this general pattern, as they tend to support AA programs for women and for minorities at about the same level, whether the programs are considered in isolation or in comparison with each other. Still, the findings here point to the ambiguous or potentially threat-related feelings that much of the public has about race-based AA programs, and how those feelings spill over to support for gender-based AA programs as well.

References American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAPOR). 2006. Standard Definitions: Final Dispositions of Case Codes and Outcome Rates for Surveys. Ann Arbor, MI: AAPOR. Bobo, Lawrence, and James R. Kluegel. 1993. “Opposition to Race Targeting: Self-Interest, Stratification Ideology, or Racial Attitudes?” American Sociological Review 58:443–64. Clayton, Susan. 1996. “Reactions to Social Categorization: Evaluating One Argument Against Affirmative Action.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 26:1472–93. Crosby, Faye J., Aarti Iyer, Susan Clayton, and R. Downing. 2003. “Affirmative Action: Psychological Data and the Policy Debates.” American Psychology 58:93–115. Crosby, Faye J., Aarti Iyer, and Sirinda Sincharoen. 2006. “Understanding Affirmative Action.” Annual Review of Psychology 57:585–611. Fine, Terri S. 1992. “The Impact of Issue Framing on Public Opinion Toward Affirmative Action Programs.” Social Science Journal 29:323–34. Hill, Richard J. 1983. “Minorities, Women, and Institutional Change: Some Administrative Concerns.” Sociological Perspectives 26:17–28. Kravitz, David A., and Judith Platania. 1993. Attitudes and Beliefs about Affirmative Action: Effects of Target and of Respondent Sex and Ethnicity. Journal of Applied Psychology 78:928– 38. Moore, David W. 1995. “Americans Today are Dubious about Affirmative Action.” Gallup Poll Monthly March:36–8. Moore, David W. 2002. “Measuring New Types of Question-Order Effects: Additive and Subtractive.” Public Opinion Quarterly 66:80–91. Moore, David W. 2003. “How Question Order Affects Attitudes on Affirmative Action.” PollTalk (on the Gallup website) (July 1). Schuman, Howard, and Stanley Presser. 1981. Questions and Answers in Attitude Surveys: Experiments on Question Form, Wording, and Context. New York: Academic Press. Sidanius, James, Pam Singh, John J. Hetts, and C. Chris Federico. 2000. “It’s not the Affirmative Action, It’s the African-Americans: The Continuing Relevance of Race in Attitudes Toward Race-targeted Policies.” In Racialized Politics: Values, Ideology, and Prejudice in American Public Opinion, eds. David Sears, James Sidanius, and Lawrence Bobo. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Question Order Effect on Support for Affirmative Action

9

Steeh, Charlotte, and Maria Krysan. 1996. “Affirmative Action and the Public.” Public Opinion Quarterly 60:128–58. Strolovitch, Dara Z. 1998. “Playing Favorites: Public Attitudes Toward Race- and Gender-Targeted Anti-Discrimination Policy.” NWSA Journal 10:27–53. Swain, Carol M. 2001. “Affirmative Action: Legislative History, Judicial Interpretations, Public Consensus.” In America Becoming, Vol. 1, eds. Neil J. Smelser, William J. Wilson, and Faith Mitchell. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Wise, Tim. 1998. “Is Sisterhood Conditional? White Women and the Rollback of Affirmative Action.” NWSA Journal 10(3):1–26.

affirmative action programs for women and minorities ...

Aug 21, 2008 - for gender-targeted AA than for race-targeted AA, but no research has ... Numerous studies have shown that Americans express more positive ...

65KB Sizes 1 Downloads 175 Views

Recommend Documents

1 DOES TEMPORARY AFFIRMATIVE ACTION ...
Mar 10, 2011 - future will lead to lower minority enrollment rates at elite colleges. ... changes in employment shares for a single year only with linearly interpolated values from ...... Evidence from California and Texas,” Industrial and Labor.

Affirmative Action as an Implementation Problem - CiteSeerX
certain community is offered an option to buy an unemployment insurance package at the time he makes his ..... hence the name \type" packages. .... As we mentioned in Section 3, the domain of the social planner's objective function may not ...

Affirmative Action as an Implementation Problem - CiteSeerX
5Ma (1988) proposed a mechanism to implement the second-best outcome in .... certain community is offered an option to buy an unemployment insurance ...... As we mentioned in Section 3, the domain of the social planner's objective function ...... gro

Endogenous Race in Brazil: Affirmative Action and the ...
parents' race, family socioeconomic status, gender, and racial quotas have a significant effect on self-reported race. The evidence indicates that students in mixed-race families are systematically more likely to identify with their mother's race tha

The Hidden Perils of Affirmative Action: Sabotage in Handicap Contests
Dec 12, 2014 - Email: [email protected]. ‡ ... Email: [email protected] ... competitions such as advertising or patent races (see Konrad ...

How Costly is Diversity? Affirmative Action in Light of ... - Offnews.info
at Harvard (Niederle), Harvard Business School (Segal) and CEBR at Copenhagen Business School. (Vesterlund). .... 7 For example, corporations such as Ernst and Young, Goldman Sachs, IBM, and PricewaterhouseCoopers have all adopted .... 13 The program

Affirmative Action Form 07-11-14.pdf
Page 1. Whoops! There was a problem loading more pages. Retrying... Affirmative Action Form 07-11-14.pdf. Affirmative Action Form 07-11-14.pdf. Open. Extract.

Affirmative Action Form 07-11-14.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. Affirmative ...

The Effect of Banning Affirmative Action on College ...
quality (as measured by expected first- year college GPA) appears to have remained ... in student quality will be misleading if, for example, universities trade .... Letting Ai = 1 if an applicant to a given school is admitted and Ai = 0 if the appli

How Costly is Diversity? Affirmative Action in Light of ... - Offnews.info
White, president of University of Illinois, “Getting more women into MBA programs means. 1 Similarly only ... When instituting programs to alter the gender composition in certain jobs it is of course important .... 10 Gneezy, Niederle and Rustichin