Does Increasing Interactions Involving Less Frequent Contact, Low Emotional Intensity, and Limited Intimacy Impact Happiness? Caroline Haut1, Jenna Malzacher2, Chloe Sunde3, and Kathleen Martin4 Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota Previous research suggests that increasing weak tie interactions increases an individual’s rating of his/her subjective well-being and belonging. The purpose of this study was to see whether increasing weak tie interactions increases happiness levels. Participants were given a worksheet to keep track of their weak tie interactions in week 1 and completed the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire (OHQ). In week 2, participants increased their weak tie interactions by two each day and took the OHQ again. Researchers found that participants did not differ significantly in their happiness levels from week 1 to week 2. These results indicate that increasing weak tie interactions does not impact overall happiness levels. Pages: 1-4

Increasingly, policymakers and leaders are measuring the progress of our country through human wellbeing and happiness (Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2013). Happiness offers immense benefits to an individual as well as to a society, such as staying married longer and obtaining more success in business and in life (Gunderman, 2008). Additionally, happiness has been linked to greater achievement in the classroom, less risky behaviors, more financial stability, and more positive contributions to 1 Caroline Haut ([email protected]) received her B.S. in Nutrition with a minor in psychology in May 2015. She is currently pursuing a master’s degree in the Counseling and Student Personnel Psychology Program in the Department of Educational Psychology at The University of Minnesota. Caroline works as a research coordinator, assessor, and volunteer supervisor for the Minnesota Center for Eating Disorders Research. 2 Jenna Malzacher ([email protected]) is a junior in the College of Liberal Arts. Next year she will receive her B.A. in Psychology with a minor in Cultural Studies and Comparative Literature. 3 Chloe Sunde ([email protected]) is a senior graduating in May, 2016 with a B.A. in Psychology, a B.A. in Sociology of Law, Crime and Deviance and a minor in Neuroscience. After graduation, she plans on joining a Christian ministry that shares the Gospel with and provides support to those currently serving time in prison. 4

Kathleen Martin ([email protected]) is a senior in the College of Liberal Arts. She will graduate in May 2013 with a B.S. in Psychology and a minor in Neuroscience. She plans to pursue graduate studies in clinical psychology with a focus on trauma and stress-related disorders.

VOLUME 12 – FALL 2015 - www.psych.umn.edu/sentience © 2015 Regents of the University of Minnesota

society (Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, & Schellinger, 2011). Positive psychology research has found that happy people perceive the world as safer and are more confident, decisive, and cooperative (Myers, 2015). It has also been found that happy people tend to savor their positive past experiences without dwelling on the negative, and feel more socially connected with others (Myers, 2015). Previous research has demonstrated that people report more positive affect when they are engaged in social rather than non-social activities (Pavot, Diener, & Fujita, 1990). Social activities may involve interactions and relationships among strong ties, which include close family and friends, or weak ties. The theory of weak ties was founded by Granovetter in 1973 and states that without weak tie interactions we would be deprived of new information from distant parts of the social system and would be confined to the views of only our close friends. According to Sandstorm and Dunn (2014), weak ties are defined as relationships involving less frequent contact, low emotional intensity, limited intimacy, and are linked to subjective well-being and belonging. In their study, they rated the amount of interactions students had with other students and found that the more weak tie interactions, the higher they rated their subjective well-being and belonging. Weak tie interactions were assessed by having participants carry a clicker to record the number of social interactions per day. Additionally, there have been multiple studies that demonstrate the benefits of weak tie interactions beyond wellbeing and belonging. Baer (2010) has found that weak tie

1

EFFECT OF WEAK TIES ON STUDENT HAPPINESS

interactions, as measured through surveys asking participants to rate their closeness, duration, and frequency of relationships, improved creativity in the workplace. The participants’ creativity levels were rated by their supervisors using three items derived from Subramaniam and Youndt (2005) asking about employees’ breakthrough ideas and ideas that make existing knowledge about current products/services obsolete. Similarly, Hansen (1999) has found weak tie interactions improve the transfer of knowledge across organizational subunits. Most social psychology literature has focused on the interactions among close friends and family but there are relatively few studies on the importance of weak tie interactions. Additionally, a review of the literature suggests that there are no studies to date that look at whether increasing the amount of weak tie interactions increases happiness. Our study attempted to examine whether increasing weak tie interactions improves overall happiness. We distributed the Oxford Happiness Inventory to undergraduate students from the University of Minnesota to record baseline information regarding their level of happiness. The participants were told to record the number of weak tie interactions they took part in over the course of three days. We defined weak ties as interactions such as short conversations or comments lasting less than two minutes with people they have never met before, not including casual greetings. The next week, they were told to increase the number of weak tie interactions they engaged in and the Oxford Happiness Inventory was taken again after they increased the number of weak tie interactions. Higher scores on this questionnaire indicated higher levels of happiness. Based on the findings from the previous literature, we hypothesized that people would be happier after increasing the number of weak tie interactions throughout their week, regardless of how many weak tie interactions they typically engage in.

METHOD Participants We collected data from 20 University of Minnesota undergraduate students using convenience sampling of friends, roommates, and acquaintances of the researchers. Participants were approached by the researchers, informed about the study, and asked if they were interested in participating. The participants’ ages ranged from 18–24 years old (M = 20.45, SD =1.43). Seventy percent of participants identified as female and 30% identified as male. Eighty-five percent of participants self-reported as Caucasian and 15% of participants selfreported as Pacific Islander/Asian. There was no compensation for completing this study. Materials We used two questionnaires for our study. The first was a demographic questionnaire that we developed asking about the race and gender the participants identified with (see

VOLUME 12 – FALL 2015 - www.psych.umn.edu/sentience © 2015 Regents of the University of Minnesota

Haut, Malzacher, Sunde, and Martin

Appendix A). The second was the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire (OHQ), which was used to measure happiness (Hills & Argyle, 2002). This was a 29-item questionnaire asking participants to respond to happiness statements using a rating scale that ranged from 1 to 6, with 1 representing strongly disagree with the statement and 6 for strongly agree with the statement (See Appendix C). An example of one question on the questionnaire is, “I do not think that the world is a good place.” In order for the participants to keep track of their weak tie interactions for the first week, we distributed a sheet with the directions “Please mark the number of weak tie interactions, defined as short conversations, or comments, lasting less than two minutes with people you have never met before, on the appropriate table for the corresponding day” (see Appendix A). Participants were instructed to tally the number of interactions throughout Tuesday, Wednesday, and Thursday and circle their final total weak tie interactions for these three days. This sheet was collected and a new sheet was given to participants for the following week. This sheet was also used to keep track of participants’ weak tie interactions, and it provided participants with a reminder to increase the amount of weak tie interactions by two each day (see Appendix B).

Procedure We included a written informed consent paragraph at the beginning of the study to inform participants their participation was voluntary and they were able to withdraw at any time from the study. Next, participants were given the demographics questionnaire. After, the participants were given an explanation about what weak ties are and instructions on how to use the worksheet to keep track of their interactions (see Appendix A). After completing this sheet, the participants were given the OHQ while a researcher explained how to complete this questionnaire. A researcher provided participants with a new worksheet and asked them to add two additional weak tie interactions per day than their amount from the previous week (see Appendix B). After the second week, the OHQ was taken again. Finally, the researchers provided the participants with a written debriefing statement explaining the purpose of the study, and answered any remaining questions the participants had.

RESULTS Level of happiness was scored by taking the average of 29 items on the OHQ. The items 1, 5, 6, 10, 13, 14, 19, 23, 24, 27, 28, and 29 needed to be reverse-scored before they were added to the total. We then conducted a paired t-test to determine whether increasing weak-tie interactions increases happiness levels. Analyses showed that participants did not differ significantly (t(19) = -0.7354, p = 0.4711) on happiness levels between week 1 (M =4.44, SD = 0.653) and week 2 (M = 4.40, SD = 0.573).

2

EFFECT OF WEAK TIES ON STUDENT HAPPINESS

Haut, Malzacher, Sunde, and Martin

DISCUSSION The results of this experiment did not support our hypothesis that increasing weak tie interactions improves overall happiness as measured by the OHQ. Our results are not consistent with the findings of previous studies, such as Sandstrom and Dunn (2014) who found that the more weak tie interactions people had, the higher they rated their subjective well-being and belonging. There were several methodological problems in this study that may have contributed to the insignificant results. Sandstrom and Dunn’s (2014) participants recorded the number of weak tie interactions they had by using a clicker, allowing them to record interactions at that moment. In the present study, participants recorded the number of weak tie interactions on a sheet of paper, which they may not have had with them at the time of interaction or forgot to write down. Additionally, it is uncertain whether each participant adhered to the directions of the study completely. There was no way to detect whether a participant actually engaged in the number of weak tie interactions as stated on their papers. Furthermore, our operational definition may have been insufficient for the purposes of the experiment. Increasing weak tie interactions by two each day may not have been enough to produce a significant difference in overall happiness. Future research should look at weak tie interactions involving conversations of five minutes or more and the effect, if any, they have on happiness. A larger sample size would increase the strength of such a test. In addition, improved measures for keeping track of weak tie interactions are needed, as well as a measure to control for other factors in the participants’ current situation. Although we did not find any significant results, recognizing these limitations can promote future research on the effect of weak tie interactions and happiness in order to benefit from all of the advantages happiness provides. APPENDIX A

Day 3 Thursday Weak Ties

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

APPENDIX B Please mark the number of weak tie interactions, defined as such as short conversations or comments, lasting less than two minutes with people you have never met before, on the appropriate table for the corresponding day. * Don’t forget to add TWO additional weak tie interactions per day this week! Day 1 Tuesday Weak Ties

Day 2 Wednesday Weak Ties

Day 3 Thursday Weak Ties

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

APPENDIX C Oxford Happiness Questionnaire Below are a number of statements about happiness. Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with each by entering a number in the blank after each statement, according to the following scale:

Gender You Identify As: □ Female □ Male

1 = strongly disagree, 2 = moderately disagree, 3 = slightly disagree, 4 = slightly agree, 5 = moderately agree, 6 = strongly agree

Race You Identify With: □ Caucasian □ African American □ Chicano/Latino □ American Indian □ Pacific Islander/Asian □ Other Please mark the number of weak tie interactions, defined as such as short conversations or comments, lasting less than two minutes with people you have never met before, on the appropriate table for the corresponding day. Day 1 Tuesday Weak Ties

Day 2 Wednesday Weak Ties

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

VOLUME 12 – FALL 2015 - www.psych.umn.edu/sentience © 2015 Regents of the University of Minnesota

Please read the statements carefully, some of the questions are phrased positively and others negatively. Don’t take too long over individual questions; there are no “right” or “wrong” answers (and no trick questions). The first answer that comes into your head is probably the right one for you. If you find some of the questions difficult, please give the answer that is true for you in general or for most of the time. The Questionnaire 1. I don’t feel particularly pleased with the way I am. (R) _____ 2. I am intensely interested in other people. _____ 3. I feel that life is very rewarding. _____ 4. I have very warm feelings towards almost everyone. _____ 5. I rarely wake up feeling rested. (R) _____ 6. I am not particularly optimistic about the future. (R) _____ 7. I find most things amusing. _____ 8. I am always committed and involved. _____

3

EFFECT OF WEAK TIES ON STUDENT HAPPINESS

Haut, Malzacher, Sunde, and Martin

REFERENCES 9. Life is good. _____ 10. I do not think that the world is a good place. (R) _____ 11. I laugh a lot. _____ 12. I am well satisfied about everything in my life. _____ 13. I don’t think I look attractive. (R) _____ 14. There is a gap between what I would like to do and what I have done. (R) _____ 15. I am very happy. _____ 16. I find beauty in some things. _____ 17. I always have a cheerful effect on others. _____ 18. I can fit in (find time for) everything I want to. _____ 19. I feel that I am not especially in control of my life. (R) _____ 20. I feel able to take anything on. _____ 21. I feel fully mentally alert. _____ 22. I often experience joy and elation. _____ 23. I don’t find it easy to make decisions. (R) _____ 24. I don’t have a particular sense of meaning and purpose in my life. (R) _____ 25. I feel I have a great deal of energy. _____ 26. I usually have a good influence on events. _____ 27. I don’t have fun with other people. (R) _____ 28. I don’t feel particularly healthy. (R) _____ 29. I don’t have particularly happy memories of the past. (R) _____ Calculate your score Step 1. Items marked (R) should be scored in reverse: For example, if you gave yourself a “1,” cross it out and change it to a “6.” Change “2″ to a “5″ Change “3″ to a “4″ Change “4″ to a “3″ Change “5″ to a “2″ Change “6″ to a “1″ Step 2. Add the numbers for all 29 questions. (Use the converted numbers for the 12 items that are reverse scored.) Step 3. Divide by 29. So your happiness score = the total (from step 2) divided by 29. Your Happiness Score: ____________

VOLUME 12 – FALL 2015 - www.psych.umn.edu/sentience © 2015 Regents of the University of Minnesota

Baer, M. (2010). The strength-of-weak-ties perspective on creativity: A comprehensive examination and extension. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(3), 592-601. doi: 10.1037/a0018761 Durlak, J.A., Weissberg, R.P., Dymnicki, A.B., Taylor, R.D., & Schellinger, K.B. (2011). The impact of enhancing students social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development, 82(1), 405-432. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x Goldberg, L.R. (1992). The development of markers for the big-five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 26-42. Granovetter, M. (1983). The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. Soicological Theory, 1, 201-233. doi:10.1086/226707. Gunderman, R.B. (2008). Happiness. Journal of the American College of Radiology, 5(11), 1109-1111. doi: DOI 10.1016/j.jacr.2008.06.00 Hansen, M.T. (1999). The search-transfer problem: The role of weak ties in sharing knowledge across organization subunits. Administrative Science Quarterly. Hills, P., & Argyle, M. (2002). The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: a compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1073–1082. Myers, D.G. (2015). Happiness: Emotions, stress, and health. San Francisco, CA: Worth Publishers. Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2013). OECD guidelines on measuring subjective wellbeing. doi:10.1787/9789264191655-en Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1990). Extraversion and happiness. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 1299-1306. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90157-M Sandstrom G.M., & Dunn E.W. (2014). Social interactions and well-being: The surprising power of weak ties. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(7), 910-922. doi: 10.1177/0146167214529799

4

Does Increasing Interactions Involving Less Frequent Contact, Low ...

demonstrate the benefits of weak tie interactions beyond well being and belonging. ... We collected data from 20 University of Minnesota .... A larger sample size would .... learning: A meta analysis of school based universal interventions. Child.

202KB Sizes 9 Downloads 99 Views

Recommend Documents

pdf-0712\interactions-mosaic-silver-edition-interactions-1-low ...
... the apps below to open or edit this item. pdf-0712\interactions-mosaic-silver-edition-interaction ... iate-reading-class-audio-cd-by-elaine-kirn-pamela-h.pdf.

Does Less Income Mean Less Representation?
social pressure, since politicians spend a lot of time hanging out with the ... Perhaps because of the increasing costs of campaigns, or the greater participa- ..... political body that receives less media attention.12 Further, to the degree voters a

WFTDA Rules LOW CONTACT SUPPLEMENT.pdf
A “hit” is defined as a block with a legal blocking zone with. momentum created before making contact with an. opposing skater that forces the skater off balance, ...

pdf-1818\less-fat-hypoglycemic-low-gi-diet-britannicachinese ...
Sign in. Loading… Whoops! There was a problem loading more pages. Retrying... Whoops! There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying.

DN175 - Off-Line Low Noise Power Supply Does ... - Linear Technology
USE CAUTION IN CONSTRUCTION AND TESTING! 1 In depth coverage of this device, its use and performance verification appears in LTC Application Note 70, “A Monolithic Switching Regulator with 100μV Output. Noise,” by Jim Williams.

Increasing participation.pdf
critically judge the ideas as you approach a solution. 9) Clustering is an alternative to brainstorming. To do. this, begin with a word, name, or concept written in ...

Frequent Service Map
Every 15 Minutes or Better. All Day, Every Day. 6 AM - 8 PM Mon - Sat. 8 AM - 6 PM Sun. Trabue Rd. R iverside D r. R ive rsid e. D r. N. Some trips continue east.

A Duality Involving Borel Spaces
A Duality Involving Borel Spaces. Dharmanand Baboolal & Partha Pratim Ghosh. School of Mathematical Sciences. University of KwaZulu Natal. Westville Campus. Private Bag X54001. Durban 4041. South Africa. The purpose of the talk is to exhibit a dual e

Research-involving-children-diagram.pdf
Feb 22, 2017 - to consent for. experimental. treatment*. Page 1 of 1. Research-involving-children-diagram.pdf. Research-involving-children-diagram.pdf. Open.

CALCULATIONS INVOLVING STRONG ACIDS Notes Blank.pdf ...
CALCULATIONS INVOLVING STRONG ACIDS Notes Blank.pdf. CALCULATIONS INVOLVING STRONG ACIDS Notes Blank.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with.

Computing multiple integrals involving matrix ...
Aug 3, 2006 - Key words: multiple integrals, matrix exponential. ... the context of filtering theory, as the system noise covariance matrix of the extended ...

accelerated simulation method involving markovian ...
packet network traffic. Recent studies had led to the conclusion that the Ethernet, ATM traffic, telnet,. FTP and variable-bit-rate (VBR) video traffic can be more ...

Exterminating inefficiencies, increasing ROI
The ability to report and optimize based on the offline impact of paid search is important for ... specific keyword by search engine in almost real time. The ability to ...

KOSHLIAKOV KERNEL AND IDENTITIES INVOLVING ...
we obtain the following result established in [9, Equation (4.23)]. ...... functions in Ramanujan's Lost Notebook, The legacy of Alladi Ramakrishnan in the ...

A QUASILINEAR ELLIPTIC PROBLEM INVOLVING ...
direct methods of calculus of variations. Mathematics Subject Classification (2010): 35J20, 35J92. Key words and phrases: quasilinear elliptic equation, critical ...

Commodity Money with Frequent Search
... between period length and determinacy has also arisen in the real business ..... panel describes these when the individual is storing good i + 1 while the bottom ..... P. Milgrom, and D. Pearce (1991): “Information and Timing in Repeated Part-

Frequent-Service-Utilizers-Issue-Brief.pdf
services including emergency room,. homeless shelters, detox centers,. and jails. September 2016. Information on individuals referenced in the following case studies were compiled by: 1) New Hampshire Coalition to End. Homelessness; 2) Mental Health

Frequent Actions with Infrequent Coordination
Sep 23, 2013 - Private. Lemma 2. Table 1: Equilibrium with Frequent Actions .... Theorem, meaning that every non-negative payoff is virtually attainable in ...

1 Low-cloud, boundary layer, and sea ice interactions over the ... - dust
cloud radiative effect contributed by low-clouds are presented. Finally, ten ..... The domain can be split into three regions based on sea ice. 286 ... computing the number of observations in each bin, and normalizing by the total. 312 ...... Remote

Commodity Money with Frequent Search
Jun 28, 2012 - which goods can function as a medium of exchange, sparking a .... 7Kiyotaki and Wright (1989) allow for storage costs to vary by good and by ...

Commodity Money with Frequent Search
Jun 12, 2012 - Consequently this limiting equilibrium is a good approximation to ... hence a smaller set of dynamic equilibria. ... The relationship between period length and determinacy has also arisen in the real business cycle literature. .... Fig