一般社団法人 電子情報通信学会

信学技報

THE INSTITUTE OF ELECTRONICS,

IEICE Technical Report

INFORMATION AND COMMUNICATION ENGINEERS

TL2013-29(2013-8)

Binding and Dependency Length in Gapless Relative Clauses Hajime ONO

and

†Faculty of Science & Engineering, Kinki University

Yu IKEMOTO

3-4-1 Kowakae, Higashi-Osaka-shi, Osaka, 658-8502 Japan

E-mail: [email protected], [email protected] Abstract Previous studies on Japanese relative clauses suggested that prediction makes an effect on the processing cost for the relative clause head noun. Our study used a pronoun soko ‘there’ in the relative clause in order to control the influence of the relative clause prediction. The pronoun in the relative clause was bound by a universal quantifier, which appeared as a relative clause head. The relative clause head that binds the pronoun attached to the subject was read slower than that binds the pronoun attached to the object, suggesting that the previously observed preference for subject relative clause in Japanese could be largely due to the influence of prediction. Keyword Japanese, sentence processing, relative clause, pronoun, dependency formation

1. Introduction

of time.

Theories of sentence processing have benefited quite a

Another prominent hypothesis that accounts for the

lot from the cross-linguistic investigation, and the issue of

slower

dependency formation could be one of the areas that

complexity as an index for calculating the processing cost

enjoyed the diversity found in human language. Probably

(referred to as Structural Distance Hypothesis, Hawkins,

even within a language, there are a large number of variety

1999; O’Grady, 1997). According to Structural Distance

with respect to the dependency, and examining each one of

Hypothesis, ORC is harder to process than SRC because

them is highly valuable for combining the results from

the dependency between the filler and the gap in ORC

those obtained elsewhere. In this paper, we focus on the

crosses more phrasal boundaries such as S and VP nodes

diversity found in relative clauses, aiming at examining to

than that in SRC does. The more phrasal boundaries the

what extent seemingly different processing steps can be

dependency crosses, the more costly the processing of

handled by a common procedure in the dependency

relative clause will be.

formation.

reading

time

for

ORC

uses

the

structural

Some of the processing studies on Japanese relative clauses pointed out that investigating Japanese relative

2. Relative clauses

clauses was particularly relevant in deciding which of the

There have been a lot of studies showing that subject

two hypotheses is better to account for the processing

relative clause (SRC) is easier to process than object

contrast

relative clause (ORC) in a number of different languages

Miyamoto & Nakamura, 2003; Ueno & Garnsey, 2008;

(King & Just, 1991; Mak, et al., 2006; Traxler, et al.,

Sato, Kahraman, Ono, & Sakai, 2010). Japanese relative

2002; Holmes & O’Regan, 1981; Kwon, et al., 2006,

clauses are different from English relative clauses in that

among others). For instance in (1), the relative clause

the head noun follows the relative clause part that contains

structure in (1a) took longer to read than that in (1b).

a gap as shown in (2).

(1a) ORC: the student [ S who i the professor [ VP saw __ i ]

(2a) ORC: [ S kyoozyu-ga [ VP __ i

(1b) SRC: the student [ S who i __ i [ VP saw the professor]

between

SRC

and

professor- NOM

One of the major hypotheses that account for the different processing cost observed between (1a) and (1b) uses the linear distance between the filler and the gap as an index (Dependency Locality Theory, Gibson, 1998,

ORC

(Ishizuka,

2005;

mita]] gakusei i ... saw

student

‘the student who the professor saw’ (2b) SRC: [ S __ i [ VP kyoozyu-o

mita]] gakusei i ...

professor- ACC saw

student

‘the student who saw the professor’

2000, often referred to in the literature as Linear Distance

This syntactic property of Japanese relative clauses is

Hypothesis (LDH)). According to Gibson’s DLT, ORC is

important because the two competing hypotheses reviewed

more costly to process than SRC because there are more

above make different predictions with respect to the

words intervening between the filler and the gap. Longer

processing cost of the relative clauses. According to LDH,

dependency length is more taxing because the parser has

the processing cost of ORC in Japanese should be smaller

to hold the filler in its working memory in a longer period

than that of SRC because there is only one word, a verb,

- 87 This article is a technical report without peer review, and its polished and/or extended version may be published elsewhere. Copyright ©2013 by IEICE

intervening between the gap and the filler. In SRC, there

is then quite unlikely that Active Gap Filling is at work in

are two words, an object and a verb; hence, it is more

the processing of Japanese relative clauses, though it is

costly to process SRC than ORC. On the other hand, SDH

assumed elsewhere that Active Gap Filling is quite

predicts that the processing cost of SRC is smaller than

relevant for the processing of scrambling in Japanese

that of ORC. Assuming that the relative structural

elsewhere

positions of the subject and the object are the same in

Phillips, & Weinberg, 2004)

English and Japanese, SDH makes the same prediction in Japanese as it does in English.

(Miyamoto

&

Takahashi,

2002;

Aoshima,

Summing up the discussion so far, we have seen that SRC was easier to process than ORC in English, and two

Using a self-paced reading method, Miyamoto &

competing hypotheses, Linear Distance Hypothesis and

Nakamura (2003) examined the reading times for SRC and

Structural Distance Hypothesis, have been shown to

ORC in Japanese, and found that the reading time of the

account for the processing contrast. Processing studies on

head noun in SRC was faster than that in ORC. Their

Japanese relative clauses are potentially useful in order to

results supported that the structural complexity that is

test which hypothesis has a wider empirical coverage. It

involved in the dependency between the relative clause

has been observed that, in Japanese, SRC was easier to

gap and the head noun worked well as a measure

process than ORC as well, suggesting that SDH is

computing the processing cost of the relative clauses.

empirically superior to LDH. However, there is some

However, as Miyamoto & Nakamura (2003) as well as

concern about the results in Japanese with respect to the

some others pointed out, there are a number of issues that

timing when the parser might be able to notice the

might have affected their findings. One of those issues

presence of the relative clause structure.

that are also of interest in the current study is a concern that there is a difference between the two conditions

3. The current study

regarding the point where the parser may notice the

It is quite disappointing if the results on Japanese

existence of a gap. In SRC, the parser encounters an

relative clauses cannot provide firm empirical findings

accusative NP at the beginning of the sentence, which tells

toward the source of the processing difficulty of relative

the parser that the nominative NP is missing for some

clause, especially about the competition between LDH and

reason (it could be due to a topic drop or scrambling, for

SDH, due to some confounding. However, even though

instance). This does not provide the parser any definite

Japanese relative clause structure involves a dependency

information that the parser is now dealing with a relative

that is different from English, namely the one in which the

clause structure, but the accusative NP appearing at the

gap precedes the filler, it seems quite valuable to

beginning of the sentence may be sufficient for increasing

investigate how the parser deals with such a dependency.

the probability of the upcoming relative clause structure to

For instance, to what extent is the processing of a

a certain level. In ORC, on the other hand, the parser may

gap-filler dependency similar to the processing of a

not be able to recognize the presence of a gap until it

filler-gap dependency? This is particularly an interesting

encounters the transitive verb in the relative clause.

issue if looking at languages like Japanese. Note that a lot

Although it is not straightforwardly clear how this

of studies on the processing of scrambled sentences in

difference about the gap recognition (or maybe more

Japanese argue that it involves processing of a filler-gap

properly “the recognition of the dependency”) would

dependency.

affect the reading times of the head noun of the relative

filler-gap dependency and a gap-filler dependency within

clause,

one

the

difference

nonetheless

seems

to

be

a

confounding factor for the reading time results.

That

language.

means

Then,

Japanese

whether

or

involves not

both

those

a

two

dependencies are similar to each other in terms of

Furthermore, it has been suggested that English relative

processing is crucial because it has a direct influence on

clause processing involves Active Gap Filling (Crain &

how many different types of processing algorithms a

Fodor, 1985; Stowe, 1986; Kluender & Kutas, 1993) where

language has to be equipped with.

the parser actively tries to identify the gap position after it

Furthermore, although it is quite unlikely that the

sees the filler (i.e., the head noun in the relative clause).

processing of the dependency formation in English

However, the relative ordering between the filler and the

relative clauses is exactly the same as that in Japanese

gap is different in Japanese: The relative clause that

relative clauses, it is still conceivable that there are some

involves a gap precedes the head of the relative clause. It

processing steps that are shared between the two. In other - 88 -

words,

the

term

‘dependency

formation’

obviously

dono-ryokan-mo yokujyo-no soozi-ga ikitodoiteiru.

involves multiple lower-level steps. For example, we can

every-inn-also bathroom- GEN cleaning- NOM well-done

imagine that, as a step in the dependency formation in

‘Every inn where its landlady welcomes guests cleans

Japanese relative clauses, there is a process of linking between the head noun of the relative clause and the gap

the bathroom thoroughly.’ (4b) Gapless Relative Clause: Pronoun in Object-RC

once the parser finally encounters the head noun and the

[okami-ga

relative clause gap is posited in the parsed structure.

landlady- NOM there- GEN

sono-no

kyaku-o

kantaisiteiru]

Assuming that this process of linking occurs only after

dono-ryokan-mo yokuzyo-no soozi-ga ikitodoiteiru.

guest- ACC welcome

both the filler and the gap are identified, it seems

every-inn-also bathroom- GEN cleaning- NOM well-done

plausible that it occurs as a late stage even in English. As

‘Every inn where the landlady welcomes its guests

we have seen above, previous studies may have failed to

cleans the bathroom thoroughly.’

fully control when the parser notices the existence of the

Note that examples in (4) do not have a gap in the relative

dependency, then they may not have been able to

clauses; while the pronoun soko is attached to the subject

appropriately measure the processing cost of this linking

in the relative clause in (4a) “Pronoun in Subject-RC”, the

step.

same pronoun is attached to the object in the relative

In the current study, we used a Japanese pronoun soko

clause in (4b) “Pronoun in Object-RC.”

‘there’ in the relative clause instead of having a gap in an

Using this type of pronoun as a bound variable inside

argument position, i.e., subject or object, and the pronoun

the relative clause has some advantages for our current

soko forms a dependency with the head noun of the

concerns. First, although this kind of relative clauses do

relative clause. Therefore, they are called gapless relative

not have a real “gap” in a strict sense, the pronoun forms a

clauses. This type of relative clause is arguably quite

similar dependency with the head noun of the relative

useful in order to examine the processing cost of linking,

clause. Then, it is conceivable to hypothesize that the way

which is one of the steps in the dependency formation,

the parser handles dependency formation of this type share

while controlling the timing of the recognition of the

some features with the dependency formation between a

dependency by the parser.

gap and the filler. In addition, in examples (4), all

Pronoun soko allows two types of usage: a referential

arguments

(subject

and

object)

are

present;

unlike

reading or a bound variable reading, as shown in (3). In

examples in (2), both of the relative clauses in (4) start

(3a), the pronoun is used to refer to the entity, a place

with a nominative NP followed by an accusative NP. This

Saijo, in the previous sentence. In (3b), the pronoun is

suggests that it is quite unlikely that there is a difference

bound by a universal quantifier dono-ryokan-mo ‘every

between the conditions about the timing of the parser’s

inn’. To obtain a bound variable reading, the pronoun must

recognition of the relative clause. In other words, the

be bound by a quantifier denoting a place or organization.

parser’s reanalysis for the relative clause structure should

(3a) referential reading

occur at the same point in a sentence: The parser has to

Taro-ga

Saizyo-e itta.

Soko-no

T- NOM

S-to

there- GEN sake- TOP good

went

sake-wa

oisii.

reanalyze the main clause structure to the relative clause structure

‘Taro went to Saijo. Sake which is found there is good.’ (3b) bound variable reading Dono-ryokan-mo soko-no every-inn-also

just

when

it

encounters

the

head

noun.

Furthermore, there has been suggested that the processing costs of the relative clause reflect the frequency of the

okami-ga

yoku syaberu.

there- GEN landlady- NOM much talk

structure (see Sato, 2011). It seems plausible to assume that the paradigm in (4) is independent from the frequency

‘For every inn, its landlady talks a lot.’

issue.

It is possible to have this pronoun inside the relative

Using the two conditions, we would like to examine

clause and bound by a universal quantifier that appears as

what determines the processing cost of establishing the

a head noun of the relative clause as in (4) (Hoji 1991,

dependency with the head noun of the relative clause.

1995; Hoji, Kinsui, Takubo & Ueyama, 2000, Ueyama

More specifically, in the case of dependency formation in

1998).

Japanese relative clauses, where the dependent element

(4a) Gapless Relative Clause: Pronoun in Subject-RC

(i.e., usually a gap) precedes the licensor (i.e., a filler),

[soko-no

okami-ga

kyaku-o

kantaisiteiru]

which

there- GEN landlady- NOM guest- ACC welcome

hypothesis,

SDH

or

LDH,

makes

a

correct

prediction about the processing cost associated with the - 89 -

dependency formation. Recall that this is exactly one of

‘there- GEN young-landlady- NOM ’), while in the pronoun in

the questions the previous research on Japanese relative

object-RC condition, the pronoun showed up with the

clauses aimed to investigate.

accusative NP in region 3 (soko-no syukuhakukyaku-o ‘there- GEN guest- ACC ’). Region 4 contains a transitive

4. Experiment

verb in the relative clause, and region 5 is the critical

A self-paced reading experiment was conducted to

region that contains a head noun of the relative clause

examine which type of relative clause is more costly to

(dono ryokan-mo ‘every inn-also’). At region 5, the

process (Just, Carpenter & Woolley, 1982). Twenty-six

pronoun in object-RC condition was read reliably faster

sets of target sentences were prepared with 2 conditions

than the pronoun in subject-RC condition (F1(1,25)=6.45,

(pronoun in subject-RC and in object-RC, similar to

p<0.02; F2(1,21)=7.76, p<0.01). The reading time data in

sentences in (4)), distributed into 2 lists. Each list was

region 4 and 6 did not show any statistically reliable

combined with 50 filler sentences. Thirty university

difference.

students

participated

the

read

results

indicated

that

the

sentences

processing of the gapless relative clause where the head noun forms a dependency with the pronoun attached to the

times for each phrase (‘bunsetu’ in Japanese) were

object was less costly than that involves a dependency

recorded.

with the pronoun to the subject.

were

and

overall

presented in a moving window fashion, where the reading Participants

study

The

instructed

to

read

the

sentences at the speed as they do normally. Each sentence was

followed

by

a

comprehension

question,

which

encourages the participants to pay attention to the content of the sentence.

5. General discussion and conclusion Our results showed that the gapless relative clause was easy to process where the dependency was linearly short,

Data from 4 participants whose comprehension accuracy

supporting

the

idea

that

processing

cost

for

the

rates were below 66% were eliminated. Data from 4 target

dependency formation between a pronoun and the relative

sentences

their

clause head is accounted for by LDH. This result contrasts

comprehension accuracy rates were below 66%. Data from

with the previous results that SRC was easier to process

the target sentences where the participant answered

than ORC, suggesting that the previous results do not

incorrectly to their comprehension question were also

directly

eliminated from further analyses. Reading time data were

establishing the dependency between the filler and the gap

trimmed so that data points beyond 2 SD from the relevant

that seems to occur after the parser encounters the head

condition × region cell mean were discarded. Figure 1

noun of the relative clause and identification of the gap.

illustrates the reading time data. Regions 2 and 3 showed a

As was discussed in section 2, the processing cost found in

large difference between the two conditions, but this was

previous studies may have resulted from the difference

due to the length of the phrase; in the pronoun in

based on the predictability of the relative clause structure.

subject-RC condition, the pronoun soko showed up with

In addition, there are some more recent proposals for the

the nominative NP in region 2 (soko-no wakaokami-ga

processing

were

also

eliminated

because

reflect

cost

the

processing

connected

to

cost

the

associated

ORC

based

with

on

interference (Gennari, et al., 2012; Gennari & MacDonald, 2008), frequency (Reali & Christiansen, 2007), discourse (Roland, et al., 2012), and so on. Any one of those may have affected the previous results. One possible reason for getting a result that contrasts with those in the previous studies could be that the use of pronoun in the relative clause makes those factors ineffective in processing the structure. Based on the idea that the parser does not recognize the existence of the dependency until it encounters the head noun of the relative clause, we can hypothesize that the

Figure. 1, Reading time data from the experiment. The x-axle shows the reading time in millisecond, and the y-axle shows the region number. The error bars represent standard errors.

parser conducts a backward search triggered by the head noun. It has been argued that a certain dependency formation involves a backward search (Sprouse, Fukuda, - 90 -

Ono & Kluender, 2011), then the current case with a

somehow come up with a structure in which a pronoun is

bound variable use of a pronoun inside the relative clause

bound

might be another situation where a backward search is in

quantifier or a wh-phrase. Although it seems to us quite

order.

unlikely that the parser actively set up a structure and

by

a

quantificational

expression,

such

as

a

Related to the backward search in Japanese, Yasunaga

search for the antecedent (i.e., the filler), it is necessary to

(2010:88, experiment 6) conducted an experiment to

figure out whether this assumption is well defended.

measure the processing cost observed in the dependency

Related to the construal of the pronoun, we must make

formation between an NP and a numeral quantifier in the

sure that the parser establishes the appropriate dependency

paradigm in (5). According to Yasunaga (2010), the

when it hits the head noun of the relative clause. As

examples in (5a) and (5b) involve a dependency between

mentioned above, the pronoun allows the referential use,

an NP zassi ‘magazine’ and a numeral quantifier 3-satu

and it is possible for the parser to construal the pronoun as

‘3-classifier’. However, no effect showed up at 3-satu

referential, not bound, even if there is a quantifier that is a

when comparing (5a) and (5b). On the other hand, a P600

potential licensor for the bound variable usage for the

effect was observed by comparing 3-satu in (5b) and kinoo

pronoun. A follow-up experiment, possibly an offline

in (5c).

experiment, is needed to explore the interpretation of the

(5a) gakusee-ga

kinzyo-no konbini-de

student- NOM neighbor 3-satu

katta.

3- CL

bought

store-at

zassi-o

pronoun: a similar concern was investigated in Aoshima,

magazine- ACC

et al. (2009). Finally, there is an ambiguity (a left-edge ambiguity) regarding the relative clause structure the parser can build.

‘The student bought three magazines at a convenient

We have assumed so far that, when the parser read the

store in his neighborhood.’

head noun of the relative clause, it reanalyzes the structure

(5b) gakusee-ga

zassi-o

kinzyo-no konbini-de

student- NOM magazine- ACC neighbor 3-satu

katta.

3- CL

bought

(5c) gakusee-ga

zassi-o katta.

yesterday

bought

store-at

relative clause. However, it might be possible for the parser to build a structure where only the accusative NP is in the relative clause but the nominative NP is in the main

kinzyo-no konbini-de

student- NOM magazine- ACC neighbor kinoo

so that both nominative and accusative NPs are inside the

clause. It could be problematic if the parser has some bias

store-at

for this ambiguity in either condition, it might affect the reading time. To sum up, the previous studies on Japanese relative

‘The student bought a magazine at a convenient store

clause have some concerns regarding the predictability of

in his neighborhood yesterday.’

the gap and the relative clause structure itself due to the

It is slightly disappointing for not observing the distance

case-marking pattern. Such factor may have masked some

effect for the backward dependency in the above cases, but

effects observed at the head noun of the relative clause,

the results may indicate that the numeral quantifier 3-satu

which is a critical region. We tried to overcome the

actually forms a dependency not with an NP but with a

problem by using a pronoun inside the relative clause, and

verb (Nakanishi, 2007). It is desirable to investigate

examine the processing cost of dependency formation

further with the paradigm used in our experiment.

between the gap and the filler, which has been the main

There are some further concerns for our current findings.

goal as in the previous studies. We found that the

First, we assumed that there was no processing difference

condition where a pronoun was attached to the object was

between the two conditions up to the point when the parser

read faster, somewhat in contrast to the results in the

reads the verb in the relative clauses in our experimental

literature. We suggested that the repeatedly found reading

paradigm, but it should be made clear what exactly the

time pattern that SRC is easier to process than ORC arises

parser does when it sees an NP with the pronoun soko with

due to the influence such as the predictability of the gap

no context related to that pronoun. There are multiple

before the parser sees the head noun of the relative clause.

possibilities; it could be that the parser takes the pronoun to be a referential pronoun, considering some appropriate

References

context not mentioned in the experiment. Or, the parser

[1] S. Aoshima, C. Phillips, & A. Weinberg. Processing filler-gap dependencies in a head-final language. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, pp.23–54,

might actively construe the pronoun to be a bound pronoun, - 91 -

2004. [2] S. Aoshima, M. Yoshida, & C. Phillips. Incremental processing of coreference and binding in Japanese. Syntax, 12, 2, pp.93–134, 2009. [3] S. Crain & J. D. Fodor. “How can grammars help parsers?” in Natural language parsing, ed. by D. R. Dowty, L. Karttunen, & A. M. Zwicky, pp.94–128, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1985. [4] S. P. Gennari & M. C. MacDonald. Semantic indeterminacy in object relative clauses. Journal of Memory & Language, 58, pp.161–187, 2008. [5] S. P. Gennari, J. Mirković, M. C. MacDonald. Animacy and competition in relative clause production: A cross-linguistic investigation. Cognitive Psychology, 65, pp.141–176, 2012. [6] E. Gibson. Syntactic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 1–75, 1998. [7] E. Gibson. “The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity,” in Image, language, brain, ed. Y. Miyashita, A. Marantz, & W. O’Neil, pp.95–126, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2000. [8] J. A. Hawkins. Processing complexity and filler-gap dependencies across grammars. Language, 75, pp.244–285, 1999. [9] H. Hoji. “KARE,” in Interdisciplinary approaches to language: Essays in honor of S.-Y. Kuroda, ed. by C. Georgopoulos & R. Ishihara, Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp.287–304, 1991. [10] H. Hoji. “Demonstrative Binding and Principle B,” in proceedings of NELS 25, pp.255–271, 1995. [11] H. Hoji, S. Kinsui, Y. Takubo & A. Ueyama. “Demonstratives, bound variables, and reconstruction effects,” in Proceedings of the Nanzan GLOW: The Second GLOW meeting in Asia, pp.295–329. Nanzan University, Nagoya, 2000. [12] V. M. Holmes & J. K. O’Regan. Eye fixation patterns during the reading of relative-clause sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, pp.417–430, 1981. [13] T. Ishizuka. “Processing relative clauses in Japanese,” in UCLA working papers in linguistics, ed. Okabe & Nielsen, 13, pp.135–157, 2005. [14] M. A. Just, P. A. Carpenter, & J. D. Woolley. Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, pp.228–238, 1982. [15] J. King & M. A. Just. Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, pp.580–602, 1991. [16] R. Kluender & M. Kutas. Bridging the gap: Evidence from ERPs on the processing of unbounded dependencies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, pp.196–214, 1993. [17] N. Kwon, M. Polinsky, & R. Kluender. “Subject preference in Korean,” in WCCFL 25 proceedings, ed. D. Baumer, D. Montero, & M. Scanlon, pp.1–14, Cascadilla Press, Somerville, MA, 2006. [18] W. M. Mak, W. Vonk, & H. Schriefers. Processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, pp.464–490, 2006. [19] E. T. Miyamoto & S. Takahashi. “Sources of difficulty in processing scrambling in Japanese,” in - 92 -

Sentence processing in East Asian Languages, ed. by M. Nakayama, pp.167–188, CSLI Publications, Stanford, 2002. [20] E. T. Miyamoto & M. Nakamura. “Subject/object asymmetries in the processing of relative clauses in Japanese,” in Proceedings of WCCFL 22, ed. by G. Garding & M. Tsujimura, pp.342–355, Cascadilla Press, Somerville, MA, 2003. [21] K. Nakanishi. Formal properties of measurement constructions. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, 2007. [22] W. O’Grady. Syntactic development. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1997. [23] F. Reali & M. Christiansen. Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, pp.1–23, 2007. [24] D. Roland, G. Mauner, C. O’Meara, & H. Yun. Discourse expectations and relative clause processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 3, pp.479–508, 2012. [25] A. Sato, B. Kahraman, H. Ono, & H. Sakai. “Expectation driven by case-markers: Its effect in Japanese relative clause processing,” in Proceedings of the 10th Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics, ed. by Y. Otsu, pp.215−237, Hituzi Syobo, 2010. [26] A. Sato. 日本語関係節の処理負荷を決定する要因の 検討:コーパスにおける使 用 頻 度 の 影 響 を 中 心 に . 広 島 大 学 大 学 院 教 育 学 研 究 科 博 士 論 文 , 2011. [27] J. Sprouse, S. Fukuda, H. Ono, & R. Kluender, Reverse island effects and the backward search for a licensor in multiple wh-questions. Syntax, 14, 2, pp.179−203. 2011. [28] L. Stowe. Parsing wh-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, pp.227–245, 1986. [29] M. J. Traxler, R. K. Morris, & R. E. Seely. Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, pp.69–90, 2002. [30] M. Ueno & S. M. Garnsey. An ERP study of the processing of subject and object relative clauses in Japanese. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, pp.646–688, 2008. [31] A. Ueyama Two types of dependency, Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, 1998. [32] D. Yasunaga. 日 本 語 に お け る 要 素 間 の 関 連 付 け 処理過程. 九州大学大学院人文科学府・大学院人 文 科 学 研 究 院 博 士 論 文 , 2010.

Binding and Dependency Length in Gapless Relative ...

THE INSTITUTE OF ELECTRONICS,. IEICE Technical Report ... This article is a technical report without peer review, and its polished and/or extended version may be published elsewhere. ..... dissertation, University of Southern California, Los.

275KB Sizes 2 Downloads 213 Views

Recommend Documents

No documents